Agnosia | |
---|---|
Agnosia causes the victims to lose the ability to recognize or comprehend the meaning of objects even with intact senses. | |
Specialty | Psychiatry, Neurology, Neuropsychology |
Agnosia is the inability to process sensory information. Often
there is a loss of ability to recognize objects, persons, sounds,
shapes, or smells while the specific sense is not defective nor is there
any significant memory loss. It is usually associated with brain injury or neurological illness, particularly after damage to the occipitotemporal border, which is part of the ventral stream. Agnosia only affects a single modality, such as vision or hearing. More recently, a top-down interruption is considered to cause the disturbance of handling perceptual information.
Types
Name | Description |
Akinetopsia | Also known as cerebral akinetopsia, this is associated with the inability to perceive visual motion. One cause of cerebral akinetopsia is lesions outside the striate cortex. |
Allotopagnosia | Patients cannot point at external targets located outside their own body, whether other persons' body parts or objects, but they perfectly point at their own body parts. |
Anosognosia | This is the inability to gain feedback about one's own condition and can be confused with lack of insight but is caused by problems in the feedback mechanisms in the brain. It is caused by neurological damage and can occur in connection with a range of neurological impairments but is most commonly referred to in cases of paralysis following stroke. Those with Anosognosia with multiple impairments may even be aware of some of their impairments but completely unable to perceive others. |
Apperceptive visual agnosia | Patients are unable to distinguish visual shapes and so have trouble recognizing, copying, or discriminating between different visual stimuli. Unlike patients suffering from associative agnosia, those with apperceptive agnosia are unable to copy images.[7] |
Associative visual agnosia | Patients can describe visual scenes and classes of objects but still fail to recognize them. They may, for example, know that a fork is something you eat with but may mistake it for a spoon. Patients suffering from associative agnosia are still able to reproduce an image through copying. |
Astereognosis | Also known as somatosensory agnosia, it is connected to tactile sense—that is, touch. Patient finds it difficult to recognize objects by touch based on its texture, size and weight. However, they may be able to describe it verbally or recognize same kind of objects from pictures or draw pictures of them. Thought to be connected to lesions or damage in somatosensory cortex. |
Auditory agnosia | Auditory agnosia has been recognized since 1877. With auditory agnosia, there is difficulty distinguishing environmental and non-verbal auditory cues including difficulty distinguishing speech from non-speech sounds even though hearing is usually normal. There are two types of auditory agnosia: semantic associative and discriminative agnosia. Semantic associative agnosia is associated with lesions to the left hemisphere, whereas discriminative agnosia is associated with lesions to the right hemisphere. |
Auditory verbal agnosia | Also known as pure word deafness (PWD). This presents as a form of meaning 'deafness' in which hearing is intact but there is significant difficulty recognising spoken words as semantically meaningful. |
Autotopagnosia | Is associated with the inability to orient parts of the body, and is often caused by a lesion in the parietal part of the posterior thalmic radiations. |
Cerebral achromatopsia | A difficulty in perceiving colors in which the world may appear drab or in shades of gray. Cerebral achromatopsia is caused by neurological damage. There are two regions of the brain which specialize for color recognition, areas V4 and V8. If there is a unilateral lesion to area V4, a loss of color perception in only half of the visual field may result known as hemiachromatopsia. Similar, but distinct, is color agnosia, which involves having difficulty recognizing colors, while still being able to perceive them as measured by a color matching or categorizing task. |
Cortical deafness | Refers to people who do not perceive any auditory information but whose hearing is intact. |
Environmental agnosia | It is the inability to locate a specific room or building that one is familiar with, as well as the inability to provide directions for how to arrive at a particular location. These individuals experience difficulty with learning routes. This form of agnosia is often associated with lesions to the bilateral or right hemisphere posterior regions. It is also associated with prosopagnosia and Parkinson's disease. |
Finger agnosia | Is the inability to distinguish the fingers on the hand. It is present in lesions of the dominant parietal lobe, and is a component of Gerstmann syndrome. |
Form agnosia | Patients perceive only parts of details, not the whole object. |
Heterotopagnosia | Patients cannot point at another person's body parts, but can point at their own body parts. |
Integrative agnosia | Usually a patient has a form of associative agnosia or apperceptive agnosia. However, in the case of integrative agnosia a patient falls in between a form of associative and apperceptive agnosia. This is where one has the ability to recognize elements of something but yet be unable to integrate these elements together into comprehensible perceptual wholes. |
Pain agnosia | Also referred to as congenital analgesia, this is the difficulty perceiving and processing pain; thought to underpin some forms of self injury. |
Phonagnosia | Is the inability to recognize familiar voices, even though the hearer can understand the words used. |
Prosopagnosia | Also known as faceblindness and facial agnosia: Patients cannot consciously recognize familiar faces, sometimes even including their own. This is often misperceived as an inability to remember names. |
Pure alexia | Inability to recognize text. Patients with pure alexia often have damage to their corpus callosum, as well as damage to the left visual association areas. Pure alexia involves not being able to read printed material, but these individuals still have the ability to write. Individuals with pure alexia usually read words letter by letter. However, individuals with pure alexia show a frequency effect. They are able to read high frequency words better and faster than they can read low frequency words. |
Semantic agnosia | Those with this form of agnosia are effectively 'object blind' until they use non-visual sensory systems to recognise the object. For example, feeling, tapping, smelling, rocking or flicking the object, may trigger realisation of its semantics (meaning). |
Social-emotional agnosia | Sometimes referred to as expressive agnosia, this is a form of agnosia in which the person is unable to perceive facial expression, body language and intonation, rendering them unable to non-verbally perceive people's emotions and limiting that aspect of social interaction. |
Simultagnosia | The inability to process visual input as a whole. The person instead processes faces, bodies, objects, rooms, places, pictures in a bit-by-bit fashion. When looking at a picture they can describe the parts of the picture but struggle to comprehend the picture as a whole. Simultagnosia occurs in Bálint syndrome but may also occur in brain injury. This condition can also be described by only seeing one object at a time. An example is having two cups in your visual field and only being able to see one at a time. |
Tactile agnosia | Impaired ability to recognize or identify objects by touch alone. |
Time agnosia | Is the loss of comprehension of the succession and duration of events. |
Topographical disorientation | Also known as topographical agnosia or topographagnosia, this is a form of visual agnosia in which a person cannot rely on visual cues to guide them directionally due to the inability to recognize objects. Nevertheless, they may still have an excellent capacity to describe the visual layout of the same place. Patients with topographical agnosia have the ability to read maps, but become lost in familiar environments. |
Visuospatial dysgnosia | This is a loss of the sense of "whereness" in the relation of oneself to one's environment and in the relation of objects to each other. It may include constructional apraxia, topographical disorientation, optic ataxia, ocular motor apraxia, dressing apraxia, and right-left confusion. |
Visual agnosia | Is associated with lesions of the left occipital lobe and temporal lobes. Many types of visual agnosia involve the inability to recognize objects. |
Visual agnosia
Visual agnosia
is a broad category that refers to a deficiency in the ability to
recognize visual objects. Visual agnosia can be further subdivided into
two different subtypes: apperceptive visual agnosia and associative visual agnosia.
Individuals with apperceptive visual agnosia display the ability
to see contours and outlines when shown an object, but they experience
difficulty if asked to categorize objects. Apperceptive visual agnosia
is associated with damage to one hemisphere, specifically damage to the
posterior sections of the right hemisphere.
In contrast, individuals with associative visual agnosia
experience difficulty when asked to name objects. Associative agnosia is
associated with damage to both the right and left hemispheres at the
occipitotemporal border. A specific form of associative visual agnosia is known as prosopagnosia. Prosopagnosia is the inability to recognize faces. For example, these individuals have difficulty recognizing friends, family and coworkers. However, individuals with prosopagnosia can recognize all other types of visual stimuli.
Speech agnosia
Speech agnosia, or auditory verbal agnosia, refers to "an inability to comprehend spoken words despite intact hearing, speech production and reading ability".
Patients report that they do indeed hear sounds being produced, but
that the sounds are fundamentally unrecognizable/untranslatable.
- EXAMINER: What did you eat for breakfast?
- PATIENT: Breakfast, breakfast, it sounds familiar but it doesn't speak to me. (Obler & Gjerlow 1999:45)
Despite an inability to process what the speaker is saying, some
patients have been reported to recognize certain characteristic
information about the speaker's voice (such as being a man or woman).
Causes
Agnosia can result from strokes, dementia, or other neurological disorders.
It may also be trauma-induced by a head injury, brain infection, or
hereditary. Additionally, some forms of agnosia may be the result of
developmental disorders.
Damage causing agnosia usually occurs in either the occipital or
parietal lobes of the brain. Although one modality may be affected,
cognitive abilities in other areas are preserved.
Patients who experience dramatic recovery from blindness experience significant to total agnosia.
The effect of damage to the superior temporal sulcus
is consistent with several types of neurolinguistic deficiencies, and
some contend that agnosia is one of them. The superior temporal sulcus
is vital for speech comprehension because the region is highly involved
with the lexical interface. According to the 1985 TRACE II Model, the
lexical interface associates sound waves (phonemes) with morphological
features to produce meaningful words. This association process is
accomplished by lateral inhibition/excitement of certain words within an
individual's lexicon (vocabulary).
For instance, if an experimenter were to say DOG aloud, the utterance
would activate and inhibit various words within the subjects lexical
interface:
- DOG activates 3, and inhibits 0 letters in DOG. – +3
- DOG activates 2, and inhibits 1 letters in FOG. – +2
- DOG activates 1, and inhibits 2 letters in DAN. – +1
The consistency of this model to agnosia is shown by evidence that
bilateral lesions to the superior temporal sulcus produces 'pure word
deafness' (Kussmaul, 1877), or as it's understood today—speech agnosia.
Patients with pure word deafness demonstrate the inability to recognize
and process speech sounds with normal auditory processing for non-speech
sounds below the level of the cortex.
Diagnosis
In
order to assess an individual for agnosia, it must be verified that the
individual is not suffering from a loss of sensation, and that both
their language abilities and intelligence are intact. In order for an
individual to be diagnosed with agnosia, they must only be experiencing a
sensory deficit in a single modality. To make a diagnosis, the
distinction between apperceptive and associative agnosia must be made.
This distinction can be made by having the individual complete copying
and matching tasks. If the individual is suffering from a form of
apperceptive agnosia they will not be able to match two stimuli that are
identical in appearance. In contrast, if an individual is suffering
from a form of associative agnosia, they will not be able to match
different examples of a stimulus. For example, an individual who has
been diagnosed with associative agnosia in the visual modality would not
be able to match pictures of a laptop that is open with a laptop that
is closed.
Pure alexia
Individuals
with pure alexia usually have difficulty reading words as well as
difficulty with identifying letters. In order to assess whether an
individual has pure alexia, tests of copying and recognition must be
performed. An individual with pure alexia should be able to copy a set
of words, and should be able to recognize letters.
Prosopagnosia
Individuals
are usually shown pictures of human faces that may be familiar to them
such as famous actors, singers, politicians or family members. The
pictures shown to the patient are selected to be age and culture
appropriate. The task involves the examiner asking the individual to
name each face. If the individual cannot name whose face appears in the
picture, the examiner may ask a question that would help to recognize
the face in the picture.
Treatment
For
all practical purposes, there is no direct cure. Patients may improve
if information is presented in other modalities than the damaged one.
Different types of therapies can help to reverse the effects of agnosia.
In some cases, occupational therapy or speech therapy can improve agnosia, depending on its cause.
Initially many individuals with a form of agnosia are unaware of
the extent to which they have either a perceptual or recognition
deficit. This may be caused by anosognosia which is the lack of
awareness of a deficit. This lack of awareness usually leads to a form
of denial and resistance to any form of help or treatment. There are
various methods that can be used which can help the individual recognize
the impairment in perception
or recognition that they may have. A patient can be presented with a
stimulus to the impaired modality only to help increase their awareness
of their deficit. Alternatively, a task can be broken down into its
component parts so that the individual can see each part of the problem
caused by the deficit. Once the individual acknowledges their
perceptual or recognition deficit, a form of treatment may be
recommended. There are various forms of treatment such as compensatory
strategies with alternate modalities, verbal strategies, alternate cues
and organizational strategies.
Verbal strategies
Using
verbal descriptions may be helpful for individuals with certain types
of agnosia. Individuals such as prosopagnosics may find it useful to
listen to a description of their friend or family member and recognize
them based on this description more easily than through visual cues.
Alternate cues
Alternate
cues may be particularly useful to an individual with environmental
agnosia or prosopagnosia. Alternate cues for an individual with
environmental agnosia may include color cues or tactile
markers to symbolize a new room or to remember an area by.
Prosopagnosics may use alternate cues such as a scar on an individual's
face or crooked teeth in order to recognize the individual. Hair color and length can be helpful cues as well.
Organizational strategies
Organizational
strategies may be extremely helpful for an individual with visual
agnosia. For example, organizing clothes according to different hangers
provides tactile cues for the individual, making it easier to identify
certain forms of clothing as opposed to relying solely on visual cues.
Alternative medicine
These
strategies elicit the use of an unaffected modality. For example,
visual agnosics can use tactile information in replacement of visual
information. Alternatively, an individual with prosopagnosia can use auditory information in order to replace visual information.
For example, an individual with prosopagnosia can wait for someone to
speak, and will usually recognize the individual from their speech.
Current research
There are clinical trials being done to further research for treatments. At the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) they support research for rare diseases like agnosia. Some organizations that are recruiting for trials are using clincaltrials.gov and give status updates on the trials.
History
The term 'agnosia' comes from the Ancient Greek ἀγνωσία (agnosia), "ignorance", "absence of knowledge". It was introduced by Sigmund Freud in 1891:
"For disturbances in the recognition of objects, which Finkelnburg
classes as asymbolia, I should like to propose the term 'agnosia'."
Prior to Freud's introduction of the term, some of the first ideas about
agnosia came from Carl Wernicke, who created theories about receptive aphasia
in 1874. He noted that individuals with receptive aphasia did not
possess the ability to understand speech or repeat words. He believed
that receptive aphasia was due to lesions of the posterior third of the
left superior temporal gyrus.
Due to these lesions, Wernicke believed that individuals with
receptive aphasia had a limited deafness for certain sounds and
frequencies in speech.
After Wernicke, came Kussmaul in 1877 who attempted to explain why auditory verbal agnosia,
also known as word deafness, occurs. Contrary to Wernicke's
explanations, Kussmaul believed auditory verbal agnosia was the result
of major destruction to the first left temporal gyrus. Kussmaul also
posited about the origins of alexia (acquired dyslexia) also known as word blindness. He believed that word blindness was the result of lesions to the left angular and supramarginal gyri.
Heinrich Lissauer shared his ideas about agnosia after Wernicke and Kussmaul.
In 1890, he theorized that there were two ways in which object
recognition impairment could occur. One way in which impairment could
occur was if there was damage to early perceptual processing or if there
was damage to the actual object representation. If the actual object
representation was damaged, this would not allow the object to be stored
in visual memory, and therefore the individual would not be able to
recognize the object. During the time of Wernicke, Kussmaul and Lissauer there was little known about the cerebral cortex. Today, with new neuroimaging techniques, we have been able to expand our knowledge on agnosia greatly.