From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Problem_solving
Problem solving is the process of achieving a goal by
overcoming obstacles, a frequent part of most activities. Problems in
need of solutions range from simple personal tasks (e.g. how to turn on
an appliance) to complex issues in business and technical fields. The
former is an example of simple problem solving (SPS) addressing one
issue, whereas the latter is complex problem solving (CPS) with multiple
interrelated obstacles.
Another classification of problem-solving tasks is into well-defined
problems with specific obstacles and goals, and ill-defined problems in
which the current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind
of resolution to aim for. Similarly, one may distinguish formal or fact-based problems requiring
psychometric intelligence, versus socio-emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups, such as
tactful behavior, fashion, or gift choices.
Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain
the goal. Professionals such as lawyers, doctors, programmers, and
consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require
technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence. Many
businesses have found profitable markets by recognizing a problem and
creating a solution: the more widespread and inconvenient the problem,
the greater the opportunity to develop a scalable solution.
There are many specialized problem-solving techniques and methods in fields such as science, engineering, business, medicine, mathematics, computer science, philosophy, and social organization. The mental techniques to identify, analyze, and solve problems are studied in psychology and cognitive sciences.
Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent people
from finding solutions; problem-solving impediments include confirmation bias, mental set, and functional fixedness.
Definition
The term problem solving has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline. For instance, it is a mental process in psychology and a computerized process in computer science.
There are two different types of problems: ill-defined and
well-defined; different approaches are used for each. Well-defined
problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions, while
ill-defined problems do not. Well-defined problems allow for more
initial planning than ill-defined problems. Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics (the way that context contributes to meaning) and semantics
(the interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the
end goal of the problem is, and what rules could be applied, represents
the key to solving the problem. Sometimes a problem requires abstract thinking or coming up with a creative solution.
Problem solving has two major domains: mathematical problem solving and personal problem solving. Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered.
Psychology
Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in life. Solutions to these problems are usually situation- or context-specific. The process starts with problem finding and problem shaping,
in which the problem is discovered and simplified. The next step is to
generate possible solutions and evaluate them. Finally a solution is
selected to be implemented and verified. Problems have an end goal
to be reached; how you get there depends upon problem orientation
(problem-solving coping style and skills) and systematic analysis.
Mental health professionals study the human problem-solving processes using methods such as introspection, behaviorism, simulation, computer modeling, and experiment.
Social psychologists look into the person-environment relationship
aspect of the problem and independent and interdependent problem-solving
methods. Problem solving has been defined as a higher-order cognitive process and intellectual function that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills.
Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem solving. Rehabilitation psychologists
studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that deficits in
emotional control and reasoning can be re-mediated with effective
rehabilitation and could improve the capacity of injured persons to
resolve everyday problems.
Interpersonal everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal
motivational and contextual components. One such component is the emotional valence
of "real-world" problems, which can either impede or aid
problem-solving performance. Researchers have focused on the role of
emotions in problem solving,
demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the
target task, impede problem resolution, and lead to negative outcomes
such as fatigue, depression, and inertia. In conceptualization,human
problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation,
and the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic
situations and problem-solving skills. People's strategies cohere with their goals and stem from the process of comparing oneself with others.
Cognitive sciences
Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the Gestaltists in Germany, such as Karl Duncker in The Psychology of Productive Thinking (1935). Perhaps best known is the work of Allen Newell and Herbert A. Simon.
Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to
solve relatively simple, well-defined, but not previously seen
laboratory tasks. These simple problems, such as the Tower of Hanoi, admitted optimal solutions
that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full
problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems
would elicit the characteristic cognitive processes by which more complex "real world" problems are solved.
An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of decomposition.
Computer science
Much of computer science and artificial intelligence
involves designing automated systems to solve a specified type of
problem: to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate
response, reasonably quickly. Algorithms are recipes or instructions that direct such systems, written into computer programs.
Steps for designing such systems include problem determination, heuristics, root cause analysis, de-duplication, analysis, diagnosis, and repair. Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear programming, queuing systems, and simulation. A large, perennial obstacle is to find and fix errors in computer programs: debugging.
Logic
Formal logic
concerns issues like validity, truth, inference, argumentation, and
proof. In a problem-solving context, it can be used to formally
represent a problem as a theorem to be proved, and to represent the
knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a
proof that the problem has a solution.
The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of automated theorem proving in the 1950s. It included the use of heuristic methods designed to simulate human problem solving, as in the Logic Theory Machine, developed by Allen Newell, Herbert A. Simon and J. C. Shaw, as well as algorithmic methods such as the resolution principle developed by John Alan Robinson.
In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems, automated theorem-proving has also been used for program verification in computer science. In 1958, John McCarthy proposed the advice taker,
to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to
questions using automated theorem-proving. An important step in this
direction was made by Cordell Green
in 1969, who used a resolution theorem prover for question-answering
and for such other applications in artificial intelligence as robot
planning.
The resolution theorem-prover used by Cordell Green bore little
resemblance to human problem solving methods. In response to criticism
of that approach from researchers at MIT, Robert Kowalski developed logic programming and SLD resolution, which solves problems by problem decomposition. He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving and computational logic to improve human thinking.
Engineering
When
products or processes fail, problem solving techniques can be used to
develop corrective actions that can be taken to prevent further failures.
Such techniques can also be applied to a product or process prior to an
actual failure event—to predict, analyze, and mitigate a potential
problem in advance. Techniques such as failure mode and effects analysis can proactively reduce the likelihood of problems.
In either the reactive or the proactive case, it is necessary to
build a causal explanation through a process of diagnosis. In deriving
an explanation of effects in terms of causes, abduction generates new ideas or hypotheses (asking "how?"); deduction evaluates and refines hypotheses based on other plausible premises (asking "why?"); and induction justifies a hypothesis with empirical data (asking "how much?"). The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or proposition to test, not which one to adopt or assert. In the Peircean
logical system, the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our
conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the logic of induction
adds quantitative details (empirical substantiation) to our conceptual
knowledge.
Forensic engineering is an important technique of failure analysis that involves tracing product defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.
Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original
problem-solving logic used in developing a product by disassembling the
product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and assembling
its parts.
Military science
In military science, problem solving is linked to the concept of "end-states", the conditions or situations which are the aims of the strategy. Ability to solve problems is important at any military rank, but is essential at the command and control level. It results from deep qualitative and quantitative understanding of possible scenarios. Effectiveness in this context is an evaluation of results: to what extent the end states were accomplished. Planning is the process of determining how to effect those end states.
Processes
Some models of problem solving involve identifying a goal and then a sequence of subgoals towards achieving this goal. Andersson, who introduced the ACT-R model of cognition, modelled this collection of goals and subgoals as a goal stack in which the mind contains a stack of goals and subgoals to be completed, and a single task being carried out at any time.
Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem, in a process known as transfer.
Problem-solving strategies
Problem-solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to
achieving a goal. The iteration of such strategies over the course of
solving a problem is the "problem-solving cycle".
Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem,
defining it, developing a strategy to fix it, organizing knowledge and
resources available, monitoring progress, and evaluating the
effectiveness of the solution. Once a solution is achieved, another
problem usually arises, and the cycle starts again.
Insight is the sudden aha!
solution to a problem, the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex
situation. Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than
those from step-by-step analysis. A quick solution process requires
insight to select productive moves at different stages of the
problem-solving cycle. Unlike Newell and Simon's formal definition of a move problem, there is no consensus definition of an insight problem.
Some problem-solving strategies include:
- Abstraction
- solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system
- Analogy
- adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms
- Brainstorming
- (especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of
solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum
solution is found
- Bypasses
- transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve,
bypassing the barrier, then transform that solution back to a solution
to the original problem.
- Critical thinking
- analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational, skeptical, and unbiased evaluation
- Divide and conquer
- breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
- Help-seeking
- obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles
- Hypothesis testing
- assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts, disprove) the assumption
- Lateral thinking
- approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
- Means-ends analysis
- choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
- Morphological analysis
- assessing the output and interactions of an entire system
- Observation / Question
- in the natural sciences an observation is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving and the acquisition of information from a primary source. A question is an utterance which serves as a request for information.
- Proof of impossibility
- try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it
- Reduction
- transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
- Research
- employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
- Root cause analysis
- identifying the cause of a problem
- Trial-and-error
- testing possible solutions until the right one is found
Problem-solving methods
Common barriers
Common
barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an
efficient search for solutions. Five of the most common identified by
researchers are: confirmation bias, mental set, functional fixedness, unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.
Confirmation bias
Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use
data which favors preconceived notions. Such notions may be incidental
rather than motivated by important personal beliefs: the desire to be
right may be sufficient motivation.
Scientific and technical professionals also experience
confirmation bias. One online experiment, for example, suggested that
professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to
view scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more
favorably than clashing studies.
According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of
confirmation bias in real-life situations, which range in severity from
inefficient government policies to genocide. Nickerson argued that those
who killed people accused of witchcraft demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation.
Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with
motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science
experiments to produce favorable results.
However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, Peter Cathcart Wason
conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three
numbers and then created a hypothesis in the form of a rule that could
have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When testing their
hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of
numbers that would confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create
triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses.
Mental set
Mental set is the inclination to re-use a previously successful
solution, rather than search for new and better solutions. It is a
reliance on habit.
It was first articulated by Abraham S. Luchins in the 1940s with his well-known water jug experiments.
Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water
by using other jugs with different maximum capacities. After Luchins
gave a set of jug problems that could all be solved by a single
technique, he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same
technique, but also by a novel and simpler method. His participants
tended to use the accustomed technique, oblivious of the simpler
alternative. This was again demonstrated in Norman Maier's
1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by
using a familiar tool (pliers) in an unconventional manner. Participants
were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its
typical use, a type of mental set known as functional fixedness (see the
following section).
Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called fixation, which can deepen to an obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.
In the late 1990s, researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional
expertise in a field can create a mental set, perhaps leading to
fixation.
Groupthink, in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group, can produce and exacerbate mental set. Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching the same conclusions.
Functional fixedness
Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only
one function, and to be unable to conceive of any novel use, as in the
Maier pliers experiment described above. Functional fixedness is a
specific form of mental set, and is one of the most common forms of
cognitive bias in daily life.
As an example, imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house,
but the only thing at hand is a can of air freshener. He may start
searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the
can, thinking only of its main function of deodorizing.
Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier: "subjects
become 'fixed' on the design function of the objects, and problem
solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's
function is not demonstrated." Their research found that young children's limited knowledge of an object's intended function reduces this barrier
Research has also discovered functional fixedness in educational
contexts, as an obstacle to understanding: "functional fixedness may be
found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."
There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving. It may waste time, delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool.
Unnecessary constraints
Unnecessary
constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task
at hand, which foreclose a productive avenue of solution. The solver may
become fixated on only one type of solution, as if it were an
inevitable requirement of the problem. Typically, this combines with
mental set—clinging to a previously successful method.
Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints.
A famous example is the dot problem: nine dots arranged in a
three-by-three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four straight
line segments, without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a
line. The subject typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer
square of dots, but the solution requires lines continuing beyond this
frame, and researchers have found a 0% solution rate within a brief
allotted time.
This problem has produced the expression "think outside the box".
Such problems are typically solved via a sudden insight which leaps
over the mental barriers, often after long toil against them.
This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the
problem in their mind, how they draw on past experiences, and how well
they juggle this information in their working memory. In the example,
envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires
visualizing an unconventional arrangement, which is a strain on working
memory.
Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the solution.
If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used,
this often derails the problem solving process, making relatively simple
problems much harder.
For example: "Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have
unlisted telephone numbers. You select 200 names at random from the
Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone
numbers?" The "obvious" answer is 15%, but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed among the 200. This kind of "trick question" is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations. Though not inherently difficult, they require independent thinking that is not necessarily common. Mathematical word problems often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical information as an extra challenge.
Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation
The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is represented: visually, verbally, or mathematically. A classic example is the Buddhist monk problem:
A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one
day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at sunset, meditates at the
top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the
foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no assumptions
about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove
that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of
the day on the two separate journeys.
The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context, trying to
describe the monk's progress on each day. It becomes much easier when
the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function: one
visualizes a graph
whose horizontal axis is time of day, and whose vertical axis shows the
monk's position (or altitude) on the path at each time. Superimposing
the two journey curves, which traverse opposite diagonals of a
rectangle, one sees they must cross each other somewhere. The visual
representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty.
Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests.
Other barriers for individuals
People
who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive
changes, even those that are critical elements of efficient solutions.
This tendency to solve by first, only, or mostly creating or adding
elements, rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to
intensify with higher cognitive loads such as information overload.
Dreaming: problem solving without waking consciousness
People
can also solve problems while they are asleep. There are many reports
of scientists and engineers who solved problems in their dreams. For example, Elias Howe, inventor of the sewing machine, figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream.
The chemist August Kekulé
was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen atoms.
Thinking about the problem, he dozed off, and dreamt of dancing atoms
that fell into a snakelike pattern, which led him to discover the
benzene ring. As Kekulé wrote in his diary,
One of the snakes seized hold of
its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes. As if by a
flash of lightning I awoke; and this time also I spent the rest of the
night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis.
There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously
about a problem before going to sleep, and then solve the problem with a
dream image. Dream researcher William C. Dement
told his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to
think about an infinite series, whose first elements were OTTFF, to see
if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what the next
elements of the series would be.
He asked them to think about this problem every night for 15 minutes
before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had.
They were instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes
when they awakened in the morning.
The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers: one, two,
three, four, five. The next five elements of the series are SSENT (six,
seven, eight, nine, ten). Some of the students solved the puzzle by
reflecting on their dreams. One example was a student who reported the
following dream:
I was standing in an art gallery,
looking at the paintings on the wall. As I walked down the hall, I began
to count the paintings: one, two, three, four, five. As I came to the
sixth and seventh, the paintings had been ripped from their frames. I
stared at the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was
about to be solved. Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh
spaces were the solution to the problem!
With more than 500 undergraduate students, 87 dreams were judged to
be related to the problems students were assigned (53 directly related
and 34 indirectly related). Yet of the people who had dreams that
apparently solved the problem, only seven were actually able to
consciously know the solution. The rest (46 out of 53) thought they did
not know the solution.
Mark Blechner conducted this experiment and obtained results similar to Dement's.
He found that while trying to solve the problem, people had dreams in
which the solution appeared to be obvious from the dream, but it was
rare for the dreamers to realize how their dreams had solved the puzzle.
Coaxing or hints did not get them to realize it, although once they
heard the solution, they recognized how their dream had solved it. For
example, one person in that OTTFF experiment dreamed:
There is a big clock. You can see
the movement. The big hand of the clock was on the number six. You could
see it move up, number by number, six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven,
twelve. The dream focused on the small parts of the machinery. You
could see the gears inside.
In the dream, the person counted out the next elements of the
series—six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve—yet he did not
realize that this was the solution of the problem. His sleeping
mindbrain solved the problem, but his waking mindbrain was not aware how.
Albert Einstein
believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously, and the
person must then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain
has already solved. He believed this was his process in formulating the
theory of relativity: "The creator of the problem possesses the
solution."
Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words, mostly in
images. "The words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do
not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought. The psychical
entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs
and more or less clear images which can be 'voluntarily' reproduced and
combined."
Cognitive sciences: two schools
Problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise. For this reason, cognitive sciences
findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to
problem-solving situations outside the laboratory. This has led to a
research emphasis on real-world problem solving, since the 1990s. This
emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and
Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically
concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge
domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex
problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios.
Europe
In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by Donald Broadbent in the United Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dörner
in Germany. The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex,
semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to
resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their
theoretical goals and methodology. The tradition initiated by Broadbent
emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes
that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically
employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition
initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay
of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem
solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain
up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables.
North America
In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert A. Simon on "learning by doing" in semantically rich domains, researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural knowledge domains—such as physics, writing, or chess playing—rather than attempt to extract a global theory of problem solving. These researchers have focused on the development of problem solving within certain domains, that is on the development of expertise.
Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:
- calculation
- computer skills
- game playing
- lawyers' reasoning
- managerial problem solving
- mathematical problem solving
- mechanical problem solving
- personal problem solving
- political decision making
- problem solving in electronics
- problem solving for innovations and inventions: TRIZ
- reading
- social problem solving
- writing
Characteristics of complex problems
Complex
problem solving (CPS) is distinguishable from simple problem solving
(SPS). In SPS there is a singular and simple obstacle. In CPS there may
be multiple simultaneous obstacles. For example, a surgeon at work has
far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes to
wear. As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner, and later expanded upon by
Joachim Funke, complex problems have some typical characteristics, which
include:
- complexity (large numbers of items, interrelations, and decisions)
- enumerability
- heterogeneity
- connectivity (hierarchy relation, communication relation, allocation relation)
- dynamics (time considerations)
- intransparency (lack of clarity of the situation)
- commencement opacity
- continuation opacity
- polytely (multiple goals)
- inexpressivenes
- opposition
- transience
Collective problem solving
People solve problems on many different levels—from the individual to
the civilizational. Collective problem solving refers to problem
solving performed collectively. Social issues and global issues can typically only be solved collectively.
The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive
capacity of any individual and requires different but complementary
varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability.
Collective intelligence is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the collaboration, collective efforts, and competition of many individuals.
In collaborative problem solving people work together
to solve real-world problems. Members of problem-solving groups share a
common concern, a similar passion, and/or a commitment to their work.
Members can ask questions, wonder, and try to understand common issues.
They share expertise, experiences, tools, and methods.
Groups may be fluid based on need, may only occur temporarily to finish
an assigned task, or may be more permanent depending on the nature of
the problems.
For example, in the educational context, members of a group may
all have input into the decision-making process and a role in the
learning process. Members may be responsible for the thinking, teaching,
and monitoring of all members in the group. Group work may be
coordinated among members so that each member makes an equal
contribution to the whole work. Members can identify and build on their
individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant
contribution to the task. Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills, problem solving skills, social skills, and self-esteem.
By using collaboration and communication, members often learn from one
another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better
learning outcomes than individual work.
Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve social interactions to solve problems together. The knowledge shared during these interactions is acquired during communication, negotiation, and production of materials.
Members actively seek information from others by asking questions. The
capacity to use questions to acquire new information increases
understanding and the ability to solve problems.
In a 1962 research report, Douglas Engelbart
linked collective intelligence to organizational effectiveness, and
predicted that proactively "augmenting human intellect" would yield a
multiplier effect in group problem solving: "Three people working
together in this augmented mode [would] seem to be more than three times
as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented person
working alone".
Henry Jenkins,
a theorist of new media and media convergence, draws on the theory that
collective intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and participatory culture.
He criticizes contemporary education for failing to incorporate online
trends of collective problem solving into the classroom, stating
"whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of
work as a group, schools grade individuals". Jenkins argues that
interaction within a knowledge community builds vital skills for young
people, and teamwork through collective intelligence communities
contributes to the development of such skills.
Collective impact
is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a
common agenda for solving a specific social problem, using a structured
form of collaboration.
After World War II the UN, the Bretton Woods organization, and the WTO
were created. Collective problem solving on the international level
crystallized around these three types of organization from the 1980s
onward. As these global institutions remain state-like or state-centric
it is unsurprising that they perpetuate state-like or state-centric
approaches to collective problem solving rather than alternative ones.
Crowdsourcing
is a process of accumulating ideas, thoughts, or information from many
independent participants, with aim of finding the best solution for a
given challenge. Modern
information technologies allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their suggestions in ways that provide good results. The
Internet allows for a new capacity of collective (including planetary-scale) problem solving.