From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Academic dishonesty, academic misconduct, academic fraud and academic integrity
are related concepts that refer to various actions on the part of
students that go against the expected norms of a school, university or
other learning institution. Definitions of academic misconduct are
usually outlined in institutional policies. Academic dishonesty has been documented in every type of educational setting from elementary school to graduate school. Throughout history this type of dishonesty has been met with varying degrees of penalties.
History
Chinese Examination Cells at the South River School (Nanjiangxue) Nanjing (China). This structure prevents cheating in exams.
Academic dishonesty dates back to the first tests. Scholars note that cheating was prevalent on the Chinese civil service exams thousands of years ago, even when cheating carried the penalty of death for both examinee and examiner. Bribery of examiners was also common, as represented in works such as the Ming-dynasty story collection The Book of Swindles and Qing-dynasty novel Rulin waishi [The Unofficial History of the Scholars].
Standards for citation and referencing began at the end of the
19th century with the emergence of guidance provided by scholarly
societies that developed discipline-specific expectations for
referencing such as the MLA and the APA.
About the same time, scholars began researching issues related to
cheating, with some early research dating back to the late 19th century and early 20th centuries, when cheating was identified as a widespread problem at college campuses in the United States.
It has been estimated that as many as two-thirds of students
cheated at some point of their college careers at the turn of the 20th
century. Fraternities often operated so-called essay banks,
where term papers were kept on file and could be resubmitted over and
over again by different students, often with the only change being the
name on the paper.
The issue of academic dishonesty became more prominent in the
latter half of the twentieth century, as universities shifted towards
more democratic approaches to admission.
Today
Academic dishonesty does not have a universal definition.
Educational institutions in different regions understand and act on
academic dishonesty in different ways. Much like many other areas of
student affairs, academic integrity also boasts organizations that help
students, faculty and staff of postsecondary institutions discuss and
understand the values of academic integrity such as the International Centre for Academic Integrity (ICAI). Multiple institutions, such as the University of Waterloo, Queen's University and York University
base their culture of academic integrity on ICAI's 6 fundamental values
(honesty, trust, respect, fairness, responsibility and courage). The following is not an exhaustive listing of regions.
United States
In the United States, one study has shown that 20% of students started cheating in the first grade. Similarly, other studies reveal that currently in the U.S., 56% of middle school students and 70% of high school students have cheated.
A large-scale study in Germany found that 75% of the university
students admitted that they conducted at least one of seven types of
academic misconduct (such as plagiarism or falsifying data) within the
previous six months.
Students are not the only ones to cheat in an academic setting. A study among North Carolina
school teachers found that some 35% of respondents said they had
witnessed their colleagues cheating in one form or another. The rise of high-stakes testing
and the consequences of the results on the teacher is cited as a reason
why a teacher might want to inflate the results of their students.
The first scholarly studies in the 1960s of academic dishonesty in higher education found that nationally in the U.S., somewhere between 50 and 70% of college students had cheated at least once.
While nationally, these rates of cheating in the U.S. remain stable
today, there are large disparities between different schools, depending
on the size, selectivity, and anti-cheating policies of the school.
Generally, the smaller and more selective the college, the less cheating
occurs there. For instance, the number of students who have engaged in
academic dishonesty at small elite liberal arts colleges can be as low as 15–20%, while cheating at large public universities can be as high as 75%. Moreover, researchers have found that students who attend a school with an honor code are less likely to cheat than students at schools with other ways of enforcing academic integrity. As for graduate education, a recent study found that 56% of MBA students admitted cheating, along with 54% of graduate students in engineering, 48% in education, and 45% in law.
There is also a great difference in students' perceptions and the
reality of their own ethical behavior. In a 2008 survey of 30,000
students in high school carried out by the Josephson Institute for Youth
Ethics, 62 percent of students polled said they "copied another's
homework two or more times in the past year." Yet, on the same survey, 92 percent said they were "satisfied with their personal ethics and character." Hence, there is generally a discrepancy between actual behavior and self-image of high school students' character.
As more students take courses and assessments online, there is a
persistent perception that it is easier to cheat in an online class than
a face-to-face course.
Moreover, there are online services that offer to prepare any kind of
homework of high school and college level and take online tests for
students.
While administrators are often aware of such websites, they have been
unsuccessful in curbing cheating in homework and non-proctored online
tests, resorting to a recommendation by the Ohio Mathematics Association
to derive at least 80% of the grade of online classes from proctored
tests.
In addition, colleges and universities are increasingly turning to
online proctoring services to oversee tens of thousands of exams per
year.
While research on academic dishonesty in other countries is less
extensive, anecdotal evidence suggests cheating could be even more
common in countries like Japan and the Philippines.
A typology of academic misconduct has been devised by Perry (2010).
Perry's typology presents a two dimensional model of academic
misconduct with one dimension measuring the degree to which rules are
understood and the other dimension measuring how closely these rules are
followed.
According to the typology only those students who understand the rules
but fail to adhere to the rules are classified as 'cheats'.
Australia
In Australia, academic dishonesty is addressed by the Tertiary Education Quality Standards Agency, which is a government agency.
UK
In the UK, the
Quality Assurance Agency is responsible for quality assurance in higher
education. It has produced several policy and guidance documents for
policy makers, educators and the general public.
Canada
In
Canada, academic misconduct is handled by individual post-secondary
institutions with the help of policies and guidelines published by the
university itself, though research into the topic has lagged behind that of other countries. Research has shown that the incidence of academic dishonesty in Canada is similar to that of the United States. While many institutions are guided by ICAI, there also exists provincial organizations, such as the Academic Integrity Council of Ontario (AICO).
Handling cases of academic dishonesty was mainly done using the rule
compliance approach, which was more punitive in nature. However, more
and more institutions are now adopting the integrity approach, which is
based on a more educational and restorative model.
Types
Bribery
Bribery is an act of giving money or gift giving that alters
the behavior of the recipient. Bribery constitutes a crime and is
defined by Black's Law Dictionary as the offering, giving, receiving, or soliciting of any item of value to influence the actions of an official or other person in charge of a public or legal duty.
The bribe is the gift bestowed to influence the recipient's conduct. It may be any money, good, right in action, property, preferment, privilege, emolument,
object of value, advantage, or merely a promise or undertaking to
induce or influence the action, vote, or influence of a person in an
official or public capacity.
Cheating
Cheating can take the form of crib notes, looking over someone's
shoulder during an exam, or any forbidden sharing of information between
students regarding an exam or exercise. Many elaborate methods of
cheating have been developed over the years. For instance, students have
been documented hiding notes in the bathroom toilet tank, in the brims
of their baseball caps, up their sleeves, along their thighs or in their
cleavage. Also, the storing of information in graphing calculators,
pagers, cell phones,
and other electronic devices has cropped up since the information
revolution began. While students have long surreptitiously scanned the
tests of those seated near them, some students actively try to aid those
who are trying to cheat. Methods of secretly signalling the right
answer to friends are quite varied, ranging from coded sneezes or pencil
tapping to high-pitched noises beyond the hearing range of most
teachers. Some students have been known to use more elaborate means,
such as using a system of repetitive body signals like hand movements or
foot jerking to distribute answers (i.e. where a tap of the foot could
correspond to answer "A", two taps for answer "B", and so on).
Cheating differs from most other forms of academic dishonesty, in
that people can engage in it without benefiting themselves academically
at all. For example, a student who illicitly telegraphed
answers to a friend during a test would be cheating, even though the
student's own work is in no way affected. Another example of academic
dishonesty is a dialogue between students in the same class but in two
different time periods, both of which a test is scheduled for that day.
If the student in the earlier time period informs the other student in
the later period about the test, that is considered academic dishonesty,
even though the first student has not benefited him or herself.
One other method is taking advantage of time zones, particularly
in exams administered worldwide. Those who take the exam first (likely
in Oceania) can then post answers for those about to take the exam (in a time zone behind like Europe).
Deception
Deception is providing false information to a teacher/instructor
concerning a formal academic exercise. Examples of this include taking
more time on a take-home test than is allowed, giving a dishonest excuse
when asking for a deadline extension, or falsely claiming to have
submitted work. This type of academic misconduct is often considered
softer than the more obvious forms of cheating, and otherwise-honest
students sometimes engage in this type of dishonesty without considering
themselves cheaters. It is also sometimes done by students who have
failed to complete an assignment, to avoid responsibility for doing so.
Fabrication
Fabrication
is the falsification of data, information, or citations in any formal
academic exercise. This includes making up citations to back up
arguments or inventing quotations. Fabrication predominates in the
natural sciences, where students sometimes falsify data to make
experiments "work". It includes data falsification, in which false
claims are made about research performed, including selective submitting
of results to exclude inconvenient data to generating bogus data.
Bibliographical references are often fabricated, especially when a
certain minimum number of references is required or considered
sufficient for the particular kind of paper. This type of fabrication
can range from referring to works whose titles look relevant but which
the student did not read, to making up bogus titles and authors.
There is also the practice of dry-labbing—which can occur in chemistry
or other lab courses, in which the teacher clearly expects the
experiment to yield certain results (which confirm established laws), so
the student starts from the results and works backward, calculating
what the experimental data should be, often adding variation to the
data. In some cases, the lab report is written before the experiment is
conducted—in some cases, the experiment is never carried out. In either
case, the results are what the instructor expects.
Impersonation
Impersonation is a form of cheating whereby a different person than the student assigned an assignment or exam completes it. Attending a class or completing an interview on another student's behalf is also considered impersonation. The academic work is totally 'outsourced' to another person or organization, usually for pay.
Contract cheating
Contract cheating, also known as ghostwriting is similar to impersonation in that a student hires a third party to complete work on their behalf. Essay mills
fall under this category. While it is believed that contract cheating
has significantly increased since 2014, there are currently no figures
to demonstrate this surge.
This is a relatively new form of cheating, seemingly gaining traction
in the 1940s when an increasing amount of advertisements for
ghostwriters could be seen on university campuses and in newspapers. This trend continued to grow into the 1960s and 1970s.
According to a study conducted in 2019, it is estimated that the
ghostwriting industry produces a revenue of approximately $100 million.
Plagiarism
Plagiarism, as defined in the 1995 Random House Compact Unabridged Dictionary, is the "use or close imitation of the language and thoughts of another author and the representation of them as one's own original work". In academia, this can range from borrowing without attribution a particularly apt phrase, to paraphrasing someone else's original idea without citation, to wholesale contract cheating.
The modern concept of plagiarism as immoral and originality as an ideal
emerged in Europe only in the 18th century, while in the previous
centuries authors and artists were encouraged to "copy the masters as
closely as possible" and avoid "unnecessary invention".
The 18th century new morals have been institutionalized and enforced
prominently in the sectors of academia (including academic science,
education, engineering etc.) and journalism, where plagiarism is now
considered academic dishonesty and a breach of journalistic ethics, subject to sanctions like expulsion and other severe career damages. Not so in the arts, which have resisted in their long-established tradition of copying as a fundamental practice of the creative process, with plagiarism being still hugely tolerated by 21st-century artists.
Lawmaking is a professional field which is not structured around the
concept of originality and for which plagiarism is less relevant.
Plagiarism is not a crime but is disapproved more on the grounds of moral offence. It may be a case for civil law if it is so substantial to constitute copyright infringement.
Since 2000, discussions on the subjects of student plagiarism have increased
with a major strand of this discussion centering on the issue of how
best students can be helped to understand and avoid plagiarism.
Given the serious consequences that plagiarism has for students there
has been a call for a greater emphasis on learning in order to help
students avoid committing plagiarism.
Also under the scope of plagiarism is self-plagiarism.
Self-plagiarism occurs when a student submits an assignment, essay or
piece of work that was originally submitted for the purpose of another
course without the instructor's permission to do so.
Professorial misconduct
Professorial
misconduct includes improper grading of students' papers and oral
exams, grade fraud, deliberate negligence towards cheating or assistance
in cheating. This can be done for reasons of personal bias towards
students (favoritism) or a particular viewpoint (intellectual
dishonesty), for a bribe,
or to improve the teacher's own perceived performance by increasing the
passing rate. It is still occasionally done for matters of ego or to
procure sexual favors (sexual harassment).
Sabotage
Sabotage is when a student or professor prevents others from
completing their work. This includes cutting pages out of library books,
deleting data off of classmate's computer or otherwise willfully
disrupting the experiments of others. Sabotage is usually only found in
highly competitive, cutthroat environments, such as at extremely elite
schools where class rankings are highly prized. Poor behavior and the
low level disruption of other students' learning, however, is extremely
common in all educational settings. Some medical-school librarians have
noted that important articles—required reading for key courses—are
frequently missing from bound journals—sliced out with razor blades,
scalpels, or other sharp blades. Other journals will be marked up in
crayon.
Abuse of confidentiality
This
takes place when data or results from research or a piece of academic
work is disseminated or shared while the author(s) expectation was for
them to remain confidential.
Aiding & abetting
Aiding
and abetting is the act of helping, enabling or encouraging someone to
engage or attempt to engage in any act of academic dishonesty.
Improper research practices
Improper
research practices involve fabricating, misrepresenting or selectively
reporting research data as well as not giving proper credit to authors
or researchers when reporting results of their work.
Causes
There are
a variety of causes of academic misconduct. Researchers have studied
the correlation of cheating to personal characteristics, demographics,
contextual factors, methods of deterring misconduct, even stages of moral development.
Incentives to cheat
Some scholars contend that there are students who have a pathological urge to cheat. The writer Thomas Mallon noted that many scholars had found plagiarism in literature (Samuel Taylor Coleridge and Charles Reade being two notable examples) to often be perpetrated in a way similar to kleptomania (a psychological disease associated with uncontrollable stealing, even when it is against the interests of the thief). On the other hand, Mallon concludes it is probable that most "cheaters" make a rational choice to commit academic misconduct. A common reason for unethical behavior is the desire to "gain a competitive advantage in the race for position or power".
Richard Fass puts forward the possibility that business scandals
in the real world make students believe dishonesty is an acceptable
method for achieving success in contemporary society.
Academic dishonesty, in this case, would be practice for the real
world. For some students, there would be a dichotomy between success and
honesty, and their decision is that: "It is not that we love honesty
less, but that we love success more."
Conversely, other scholars consider that with the recent rise in
corporate ethics related dismissals in the business world, this approach
to cheating may be losing its appeal, if it ever really had any.
However, it has been shown that the expected benefits of cheating as
well as student's morality plays an important role for the engagement in
dishonest behavior.
Recent studies have indicated that there is no clear link between academic dishonesty and academic success.
One study showed that students given an unexpected opportunity to cheat
did not improve their grades significantly from the control group. Another study showed that students who were allowed to bring cheat sheets to a test did not improve their grades.
While this may conflict with the common perception of cheating (one
survey found only 13% of males and 46% of females think that cheating
does not help grades,)
it is often apparent to professors and members of academic conduct
committees when a paper has been plagiarized by its inferior quality.
In the US, William Bowers reported that, on average, one third of grade A students cheated in 1964.
And asserts that academic dishonesty acts as a shortcut, so even grade A
students might be tempted to cheat. He contends that even if a
plagiarized paper receives a relatively low grade, that grade is
actually high, given how much time and effort went into the paper. In
the study mentioned above (in which students were allowed to bring crib
sheets to a test but did not improve their scores), the researcher
concluded that the students used the crib notes as alternatives to
studying, rather than as complements to studying, and thus spent less
time preparing for the exam.
Teachers
The federal government of the United States has mandated high-stakes testing as part of the No Child Left Behind Act,
signed into law in 2002. Schools and teachers are held accountable for
the results. According to Steven Levitt and Stephen Dubner, co-authors
of Freakonomics, teachers are known to "teach to the test": while
not teaching the actual answers, they teach the questions and similar
ones, and they neglect any topic that will not be tested on. Levitt also
states that teachers may inflate the results of tests given in their
classroom. Teachers and librarians can have a significant proactive impact on doing honest work.
Demographic and personal causes
Research
has identified a number of demographic characteristics that appear to
be important influences on cheating, including age, gender and grade
point average. Older students, females, and students with higher academic achievement
are less likely to cheat, whereas students involved with many
extra-curricular activities are more likely to do so. Students involved
in extra-curricular activities may be less committed to their studies,
or may have more demands on their time, which interfere with their
studies, creating a greater incentive to cheat. It has been found that
younger students are somewhat more likely to cheat: one study finding
the highest incidence of cheating occurs during sophomore year at
college.
Although cheating might be expected to decline with greater moral
development, one experiment found that there was no relationship between
how a student performed on a morality test and his likelihood of cheating (that is, students at a pre-conventional stage of morality are as likely to cheat as those at a post-conventional stage).
Higher academic procrastination was also found to increase the
frequency of seven different forms of academic misconduct (using
fraudulent excuses, plagiarism, copying from someone else in exams,
using forbidden means in exams, carrying forbidden means into exams,
copying parts of homework from others, and fabrication or falsification
of data) as well as the variety of academic misconduct.
This German panel study among thousands of university students argues
that academic misconduct might be a coping-strategy to overcome the
negative consequences of academic procrastination such as lower
performance.
Race, nationality, and class
all show little correlation with academic misconduct. There is also no
correlation between how religious someone is and the likelihood that
that person will cheat. A comparison between students of different religions yielded similar results, although the study did show that Jews tend to cheat less than members of other religions.
One of the strongest demographic correlations with academic misconduct
in the United States is with language. Students who speak English
as a second language have been shown to commit academic dishonesty more
and are more likely to be caught than native speakers, since they will
often not want to rewrite sources in their own words, fearing that the
meaning of the sentence will be lost through poor paraphrasing skills. In the University of California system, international students make up 10% of the student body but comprise 47% of academic dishonesty cases. In British universities, students from outside of the European Union make up 12% of the student body but comprise 35% of academic dishonesty cases.
Impostor syndrome
and academically dishonest behaviours have been found to be correlated.
Students who do not believe they deserve to be where they are in terms
of academics actively engage in self-sabotaging behaviour (plagiarism
and cheating) in order to prove that they do not belong where they are
while students who do not suffer from impostor syndrome are less likely
to engage in academic dishonesty.
Contextual causes
Academic
misconduct is more easily traced to the academic and social environment
of students than to their background. These contextual factors can be
as broad as the social milieu at school to as narrow as what
instructions a teacher gives before an exam.
Contextual factors that individual teachers can affect often make
the least difference on cheating behavior. A study found that
increasing the distance between students taking an exam has little
effect on academic misconduct, and that threatening students before an
exam with expulsion if they cheat actually promotes cheating behavior. Indeed, increased exam proctoring
and other methods of detecting cheating in the classroom are largely
ineffective. According to one survey of American college students, while
50% had cheated at least once in the previous six months, and 7% had
cheated more than five times in that period, only 2.5% of the cheaters
had been caught.
As teachers invent more elaborate methods of deterring cheating,
students invent even more elaborate methods of cheating (sometimes even
treating it as a game), leading to what some teachers call a costly and
unwinnable arms race.
Increased punishment for academic misconduct also has little
correlation with cheating behavior. It has been found that students with
markedly different perceptions of what the severity of the punishment
for cheating were all equally likely to cheat, probably indicating that
they thought that increased penalties were immaterial since their
cheating would never be discovered.
However, if a professor makes clear that he disapproves of cheating,
either in the syllabus, in the first class, or at the beginning of a
test, academic dishonesty can drop by 12%.
Some professors may have little incentive to reduce cheating in their
classes below a point that would otherwise be obvious to outside
observers, as they are rated by how many research papers they publish
and research grants they win for the college, and not by how well they
teach.
Teachers can, however, accidentally promote cheating behavior. A
study found a correlation between how harsh or unfair a professor is
perceived as and academic misconduct, since students see cheating as a
way of getting back at the teacher. Also, students who see themselves in a competition, such as when the teacher is using a grade curve, are more likely to cheat.
Research has also shown a correlation between goal orientation
and the occurrence of academic cheating. Students who perceive their
classroom to have high mastery goals are less likely to engage in
cheating than those who perceive their classroom to emphasize
performance goals.
In other words, students who are encouraged to learn for the sake of
learning and who exhibit an intrinsic value of education are less likely
to cheat than those who are encouraged primarily by grades and other
extrinsic rewards.
The most important contextual causes of academic misconduct are
often out of individual teachers' hands. One very important factor is time management. One survey reported two-thirds of teachers believed that poor time management was the principal cause of cheating. Often social engagements are to blame. It has been found that there is a strong correlation between extracurricular activities and cheating, especially among athletes, even those on intramural teams. It has also been found that student cheating rates rise significantly the more time students spend playing cards, watching television, or having a few drinks with friends. Relatedly, fraternity or sorority membership is also strongly correlated with academic misconduct.
One of the most important causes of academic misconduct is the
contextual factor of an environment of peer disapproval of cheating,
that is, peer pressure. Psychologists note that all people tend to follow the norms of their peer group, which would include norms about academic dishonesty.
Thus, students who believe that their peers disapprove of cheating are
less likely to cheat. Indeed, multiple studies show that the most
decisive factor in a student's decision to cheat is his perception of
his peers' relationship with academic dishonesty.
For instance, on average 69% of students cheat at colleges with low
community disapproval of academic misconduct, whereas only about 23% of
students cheat at colleges with strong community disapproval of academic
misconduct.
Peer pressure works both ways, as a study found that there is a 41%
increase in the probability of a student cheating if he or she has seen
someone else cheat.
However, even if most students strongly disapprove of cheating, there
has to be a community in order for those norms to be enforced via peer
pressure. For instance, larger schools, which usually have much higher
cheating rates than small schools, tend to have a weaker community,
being more split up into different peer groups that exert little social
pressure on each other.
Another measure of a college community, how many students live on
campus, further shows a significant relation with a school's cheating
rate. Relatedly, many professors argue that smaller classes reduce cheating behavior.
Ethical causes
No
matter what the demographic or contextual influences are on a student
who decides to engage in cheating behavior, before they can cheat they
must overcome their own conscience.
This depends both on how strongly someone disapproves of academic
dishonesty and what types of justifications the student uses to escape a
sense of guilt. For instance, students who personally do not have a
moral problem with academic misconduct can cheat guilt-free.
However, while many students have been taught and have internalized
that academic dishonesty is wrong, it has been shown that on average a
third of students who strongly disapprove of cheating have in fact
cheated.
People who cheat despite personal disapproval of cheating engage in
something called "neutralization", in which a student rationalizes the
cheating as being acceptable due to certain mitigating circumstances.
According to psychologists of deviant behavior, people who engage in
neutralization support the societal norm in question, but "conjure up"
reasons why they are allowed to violate that norm in a particular case. Neutralization is not a simple case of ex post facto rationalization, but is rather a more comprehensive affair, occurring before, during, and after the act of cheating.
Researchers have found four major types of neutralization of academic
dishonesty, which they categorize by type of justification. Denial of responsibility
– that is, the accusation that others are to blame or that something
forced the student to cheat – is the most common form of neutralization
among college students who cheated, with 61% of cheaters using this form
of justification. Condemnation of condemner
– that is, that the professors are hypocrites or brought it on
themselves – is the second most common form of college student
neutralization at 28%. The third most popular form of neutralization among college students is the appeal to higher loyalties,
where the student thinks their responsibility to some other entity,
usually their peers, is more important than doing what they know to be
morally right. About 6.8% of cheaters in higher education use this form
of neutralization. Denial of injury – thinking that nobody is worse off for the cheating – is the fourth most popular kind of neutralization at 4.2% of cheaters.
Cultural considerations
Many
studies have revealed that academic honesty is not a universal concept.
Some cultures accept the memorization and regurgitation of information,
without citing sources, while others would consider this plagiarism.
Additionally, some cultures believe that knowledge belongs to everyone
and that this knowledge needs to be shared. Studies have shown that in
certain Asian cultures, it is more important to share information widely
than to properly cite the owner of this knowledge.
COVID-19 and academic dishonesty
Cases of academic dishonesty have increased during the COVID-19 pandemic.
As a result of the sudden transition from in-person to online course
delivery, instructors (who may have been engaging in the same evaluation
practices for years) did not have the opportunity to consider how to
deliver online evaluations, how they differ from in-person evaluations
and what the online process entails.
The sharing of academic files, contract cheating and the unauthorized
receipt of assistance from classmates and other sources have increased
due to the transition to online course delivery.
Effects
Cheating in academics has a host of effects on students, on teachers,
on individual schools, and on the educational system itself.
For instance, students who engage in neutralization to justify
cheating, even once, are more likely to engage in it in the future,
potentially putting them on a road to a life of dishonesty.
Indeed, one study found that students who are dishonest in class are
more likely to engage in fraud and theft on the job when they enter the
workplace.
Students are also negatively affected by academic dishonesty after
graduation. A university diploma is an important document in the labor market.
Potential employers use a degree as a representation of a graduate's
knowledge and ability. However, due to academic dishonesty, not all
graduates with the same grades actually did the same work or have the
same skills. Thus, when faced with the fact that they do not know which
graduates are skilled and which are the "lemons",
employers must pay all graduates based on the quality of the average
graduate. Therefore, the more students who cheat, getting by without
achieving the required skills or learning, the lower the quality of the
average graduate of a school, and thus the less employers are willing to
pay a new hire from that school. Because of this reason, all students,
even those that do not cheat themselves, are negatively affected by
academic misconduct.
Academic dishonesty also creates problems for teachers. In economic terms, cheating causes an underproduction of knowledge, where the professor's job is to produce knowledge.
Moreover, a case of cheating often will cause emotional distress to
faculty members, many considering it to be a personal slight against
them or a violation of their trust. Dealing with academic misconduct is
often one of the worst parts of a career in education, one survey
claiming that 77% of academics agreed with the statement "dealing with a
cheating student is one of the most onerous aspects of the job".
Academic misconduct can also have an effect on a college's
reputation, one of the most important assets of any school. An
institution plagued by cheating scandals may become less attractive to
potential donors and students and especially prospective employers.
Alternatively, schools with low levels of academic dishonesty can use
their reputation to attract students and employers.
Ultimately, academic dishonesty undermines the academic world. It
interferes with the basic mission of education, the transfer of
knowledge, by allowing students to get by without having to master the
knowledge.
Furthermore, academic dishonesty creates an atmosphere that is not
conducive to the learning process, which affects honest students as
well.
When honest students see cheaters escape detection, it can discourage
student morale, as they see the rewards for their work cheapened.
Cheating also undermines academia when students steal ideas. Ideas are a
professional author's "capital and identity", and if a person's ideas
are stolen it retards the pursuit of knowledge.
If never formally retracted, fraudulent publications can remain
an issue for many years as articles and books remain on shelves and
continue to be cited. The case of S. Walter Poulshock, a 1960s early-career historian whose work was found to contain wholly fabricated material, was exposed in 1966 with the American Historical Review providing a warning on the topic.
Nonetheless, his book was never removed from the shelves of many
university libraries and (together with his related thesis) was still
being cited in 2013, 47 years after it was intended to have been
withdrawn by its publisher.
Deterrence
All parties involved in the dishonesty—not just the individual whose grade is increased by it—can be punished.
Historically the job of preventing cheating has been given to the teacher. It used to be that in college the professor acted in loco parentis and was able to regulate student behavior as a parent.
Thus, professors who discovered cheating could assign essentially any
punishment they deemed appropriate. Students often had no mechanism for
appeal. Generally, proctors were hired to patrol exams. If a case was
particularly serious, a dean or other top-level administrator might have
been involved. Against this inconsistent and paternalistic system,
students at some schools rebelled and demanded to be treated as adults.
Honor codes
First at the College of William and Mary in 1779, and then followed by schools like the University of Virginia in the 1850s and Wesleyan University
in 1893, the students, with the agreement of faculty who declared
themselves dedicated to ideals of democracy and human character, created
honor codes.
B. Melendez of Harvard University defined an honor code as a code of
academic conduct that includes a written pledge of honesty that students
sign, a student controlled judiciary that hears alleged violations, unproctored examinations, and an obligation for all students help enforce the code.
This system relied on student self-enforcement, which was considered
more becoming of young gentlemen than the policing by proctors and
professors that existed previously. Of interest, the military academies
of the US took the honor code one step further than civilian colleges,
disallowing "tolerance", which means that if a cadet or midshipman is
found to have failed to report or outright protected someone engaged in
academic dishonesty (as well as other dishonesties or stealing), that
individual is to be expelled along with the perpetrator.
Mixed judicial boards
However,
many people doubted the advisability of relying on an abstract notion
of honor to prevent academic dishonesty. This doubt has perhaps led to
the reality that no more than a quarter of American universities have
adopted honor codes.
Moreover, many professors could not envisage a student run trial
process that treated faculty accusers fairly. In response to these
concerns, in the middle of the twentieth century, many schools devised
mixed judicial panels composed of both students and faculty. This type
of academic integrity system was similar to the traditional faculty
control system in that it relied on professors to detect cheating,
except in this system cheaters were brought before centralized boards of
students and faculty for punishment. By the 1960s over a quarter of
American universities had adopted this system of mixed judicial boards. Still, though, over half of American universities continued to use faculty-centered control systems.
Student due process rights
Starting in the 1960s, the U.S. Supreme Court began chipping away at the in loco parentis doctrine, giving college students more civil liberties such as the right of due process in disciplinary proceedings (Dixon v. Alabama Board of Education, 1961). In Cooper v. Blair
(1973), specifically academic misconduct was ruled to require due
process, being a disciplinary matter and not an educational matter.
The due process rights of students in academic misconduct cases is not
to the same degree as in a court of law. For instance, the student has
no right to representation and the burden of proof is not necessarily
stringent. In the "General Order on Judicial Standards of Procedure and
Substance in Review of Student Discipline in Tax Supported Institutions
of Higher Education", (1968) student due process rights were laid out as
follows:
- The student should be given adequate notice in writing of the
specific ground or grounds and the nature of the evidence on which the
discipline proceedings are based.
- The student should be given an opportunity for a hearing in which
the disciplinary authority provides a fair opportunity for hearing of
the student's position, explanations, or evidence.
- No disciplinary action may be taken on grounds which are not supported by any substantial evidence.
These new rules put an end to the old faculty-based system of
policing academic dishonesty, now students were entitled to an impartial
hearing. While schools using the old honor code method or the mixed
judicial system were not affected by these decisions, schools using the
faculty based system generally instituted systems that relied on a
committee of faculty and administrators or a dean to run the academic
misconduct hearings.
Modified honor codes
Recently,
Donald L. McCabe and Linda Klebe Trevino, two experts in the field of
academic dishonesty, have proposed a new way of deterring cheating that
has been implemented in schools such as the University of Maryland.
Modified honor codes put students in charge of the judicial hearing
process, making it clear that it is the students' responsibility to stop
cheating amongst themselves, but at the same time students still have
proctored exams and are not allowed to take pledges of good conduct in
place of professor oversight.
The researchers who advocate this type of code seem to think that the
normal honor code is something of a special case that is not applicable
to many schools.
According to supporters of this system, schools with a large student
body, a weak college community, or no history of student self-governance
will not be able to support a full honor code. However, while modified
honor codes seem to be more effective than faculty or administration run
integrity codes of conduct, research shows that schools with modified
codes still have higher rates of cheating than schools with full honor
codes.
Comparison of different systems of enforcement
Research
has shown that there is a strong correlation between forms of academic
integrity system and levels of cheating at a school. Several studies
have found students who attend schools with honor codes are less likely
to cheat than students at schools with traditional integrity codes.
Another study found that only 28% of schools with honor codes have
high levels of cheating, whereas 81% of schools with mixed judicial
boards have high rates of cheating.
Whereas faculty or administration run codes of conduct tend to rely on
policing and punishment to deter students from cheating, honor codes
tend to rely on and cultivate student senses of honor and group peer
pressure to deter academic misconduct.
As mentioned above in the section on causes of cheating, increased
enforcement or punishment is rarely effective at discouraging cheating,
whereas there is a high correlation between peer pressure and academic
honesty. The modified honor code attempts to cultivate peer disapproval
of cheating while maintaining the traditional proctor system, although
critics argue that the proctor system undermines the creation of an
atmosphere of student self-policing, reducing the effectiveness of the
honor code, possibly explaining why modified honor codes have not been
as effective as the original version.
Faculty issues in deterring academic dishonesty
There
are limitations to relying on the faculty to police academic
dishonesty. One study found that up to 21% of professors have ignored at
least one clear cut case of cheating.
Another study revealed that 40% of professors "never" report cheating,
54% "seldom" report cheating, and that a mere 6% act on all cases of
academic misconduct that confront them. A third survey of professors found that while 79% had observed cheating, only 9% had penalized the student. According to a manual for professors on cheating,
the reasons for this lack of action include unwillingness to devote
time and energy to the issue, reluctance to undergo an emotional
confrontation, and fear of retaliation by the student, of losing
students, of being accused of harassment or discrimination, and even of
being sued for these offenses and/or defamation of character.
There are other reasons as well. Some professors are reluctant to
report violations to the appropriate authorities because they believe
the punishment to be too harsh.
Some professors may have little incentive to reduce cheating in
their classes below a point that would otherwise be obvious to outside
observers, as they are rated by how many research papers they publish and research grants they win for the college, and not by how well they teach.
Others do not report academic misconduct because of postmodernist
views on cheating. Postmodernism calls into question the very concepts
of "authorship" and "originality." From the perspective of cultural
studies and historicism, authors themselves are simply constructs of
their social surroundings, and thus they simply rewrite already written
cultural stories. Moreover, in the field of composition studies,
students are being encouraged more and more to do group work and
participate in ongoing collective revision. The postmodernist view is
that "the concept of intellectual malpractice is of limited epistemological
value. Under the ironic gaze of postmodernism, the distinctions between
guilt and innocence, integrity and deceit permeating the scandal
debates appear irrelevant." However, there is an argument that postmodernism is just moral relativism,
therefore cheating is condoned as a valid academic method, even if it
is morally and legally wrong. One professor wrote in an article in The English Journal
that when he peeked in on an unproctored class taking a test and saw
several students up and consulting with one another, he decided that
they were not cheating, but were using non-traditional techniques and
collaborative learning to surmount the obstacles teachers had put in
their way.
Issues of cultural relativism also affect professors' views on
cheating; the standard objection being that "students from certain
Middle Eastern, Asian, and African cultures are baffled by the notion
that one can 'own' ideas, since their cultures regard words and ideas as
the property of all rather than as individual property".
Another issue teachers may have with deterring cheating is that
they may decide that it is not their job. The argument that "they're
professors, not policemen" is often heard in academia.
In economic terms, some professors believe they are being paid to
provide learning, and if the student loses that learning through
cheating, he is only cheating himself out of the money he paid.
Detection
There are multiple online tools now available to faculty and students to detect the incidence of plagiarism and similarity. Turnitin
is often used by professors to analyze the similarity index of a
student's assignment against previously published writing. Turnitin is
also being used by students prior to submitting assignments to eliminate
the chances of a faculty member detecting plagiarism.
With the advancement of the internet, there are now several tools available to aid in the detection of plagiarism and multiple publication within biomedical literature. One tool developed in 2006 by researchers in Harold Garner's laboratory at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas is Déjà Vu, an open-access database containing several thousand instances of duplicate publication.
Due to the nature of contract cheating, it is hard to detect it,
as it is often an original piece of work that is being produced. While
Turnitin has recently released its Authorship Investigate tool, it has
not yet been vetted by research and is not being widely used in
postsecondary institutions. Clue sleuthing is another way of detecting
contract cheating, which entails examining an assignment based on the
depth and quality of answers as well as the instructions being followed
in terms of an assignment. This is due to the fact that ghostwriters
often do not follow the instructions for an assignment and provide vague
and superficial content in their writing. The use of stylometry software (such as JStylo and JGAAP) is also being evaluated to analyze its efficacy in detecting contract cheating.