From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
A
near-death experience (
NDE) is a
personal experience associated with death or impending
death.
Such experiences may encompass a variety of sensations including
detachment from the body, feelings of levitation, total serenity,
security, warmth, the experience of absolute dissolution, and the
presence of a light.
[1] NDEs are a recognized part of some
transcendental and
religious beliefs in an
afterlife.
Different models have been described to explain NDEs.
[5] Neuroscience research suggests that an NDE is a
subjective phenomenon resulting from "disturbed bodily multisensory integration" that occurs during life-threatening events.
[6]
Etymology
The equivalent
French term
expérience de mort imminente (experience of imminent death) was proposed by the French
psychologist and
epistemologist Victor Egger as a result of discussions in the 1890s among
philosophers and psychologists concerning
climbers' stories of the panoramic life review during falls.
[9][10]
In 1892 a series of subjective observations by workers falling from
scaffolds, war soldiers who suffered injuries, climbers who had fallen
from heights or other individuals who had come close to death (near
drownings, accidents) was reported by
Albert Heim. This was also the first time the phenomenon was described as clinical syndrome.
[11] In 1968
Celia Green published an analysis of 400 first-hand accounts of
out-of-body experiences.
[12] This represented the first attempt to provide a
taxonomy of such experiences, viewed simply as anomalous perceptual experiences, or
hallucinations. These experiences were popularized by the work of
psychiatrist Raymond Moody in 1975 who coined the term "near-death experience" (NDE).
[11]
Characteristics
Common elements
Researchers have identified the common elements that define near-death experiences.
[3] Bruce Greyson
argues that the general features of the experience include impressions
of being outside one's physical body, visions of deceased relatives and
religious figures, and transcendence of egotic and spatiotemporal
boundaries.
[13] Many common elements have been reported, although the person's interpretation of these events often corresponds with the
cultural,
philosophical, or
religious beliefs of the person experiencing it. For example, in the USA, where 46% of the population believes in
guardian angels,
they will often be identified as angels or deceased loved ones (or will
be unidentified), while Hindus will often identify them as messengers
of the god of death.
[14][15]
Common traits that have been reported by NDErs are as follows:
- A sense/awareness of being dead.[3]
- A sense of peace, well-being and painlessness. Positive emotions. A sense of removal from the world.[3]
- An out-of-body experience.
A perception of one's body from an outside position, sometimes
observing medical professionals performing resuscitation efforts.[3][16]
- A "tunnel experience" or entering a darkness. A sense of moving up, or through, a passageway or staircase.[3][16]
- A rapid movement toward and/or sudden immersion in a powerful light (or "Being of Light") which communicates with the person.[17]
- An intense feeling of unconditional love and acceptance.[18]
- Encountering "Beings of Light", "Beings dressed in white", or
similar. Also, the possibility of being reunited with deceased loved
ones.[3][16]
- Receiving a life review, commonly referred to as "seeing one's life flash before one's eyes".[3]
- Approaching a border or a decision by oneself or others to return to one's body, often accompanied by a reluctance to return.[3][16]
- Suddenly finding oneself back inside one's body.[19]
- Connection to the cultural beliefs held by the individual, which
seem to dictate some of the phenomena experienced in the NDE and
particularly the later interpretation thereof.
Stages
Kenneth Ring (1980) subdivided the NDE on a five-stage
continuum. The subdivisions were:
[20]
- Peace
- Body separation
- Entering darkness
- Seeing the light
- Entering the light
He stated that 60% experienced stage 1 (feelings of peace and
contentment), but only 10% experienced stage 5 ("entering the light").
[21] According to Alana Karran, the NDE stages resemble the so-called
hero's journey.
[22]
Clinical circumstances
Clinical circumstances associated with near-death experiences include
cardiac arrest in
myocardial infarction (
clinical death); shock in postpartum
loss of blood or in perioperative complications; septic or anaphylactic shock; electrocution;
coma resulting from
traumatic brain damage;
intracerebral hemorrhage or
cerebral infarction; attempted
suicide;
near-drowning or
asphyxia;
apnea; and serious
depression.
[citation needed]
In contrast to common belief, Kenneth Ring argues that attempted
suicides do not lead more often to unpleasant NDEs than unintended
near-death situations.
NDE variants
Some
NDEs have elements that bear little resemblance to the "typical"
near-death experience. Anywhere from one percent (according to a 1982
Gallup poll) to 20 percent of subjects may have distressing experiences
and feel terrified or uneasy as various parts of the NDE occur. For
instance, they may visit or view dark and depressing areas or are
accosted by what seem to be hostile or oppositional forces or presences.
[24]
Persons having bad experiences were not marked by more religiosity or
suicidal background. According to one study (Greyson 2006) there is
little association between NDEs and prior psychiatric treatment, prior
suicidal behavior, or family history of suicidal behavior. There was
also little association between NDEs and religiosity, or prior brushes
with death, suggesting the occurrence of NDEs is not influenced by
psychopathology, by religious denomination or religiosity, or by
experiencers' prior expectations of a pleasant dying process or
continued postmortem existence.
[25] Greyson (2007) also found that the long term recall of NDE incidents
was stable and did not change due to embellishment over time.
[26]
Greyson Bush, former Executive Director to the International
Association for Near-Death Studies, holds that not all negative NDE
accounts are reported by people with a religious background.
[27]
Suicide attempters, who should be expected to have a higher rate of
psychopathology according to Greyson (1991) did not show much difference
from non-suicides in the frequency of NDEs.
[28]
After-effects
NDEs are associated with changes in personality and outlook on life.
[3]
Kenneth Ring (professor of psychology) has identified a consistent set
of value and belief changes associated with people who have had a
near-death experience. Among these changes one finds a greater
appreciation for life, higher self-esteem, greater compassion for
others, less concern for acquiring material wealth, a heightened sense
of purpose and self-understanding, desire to learn, elevated
spirituality, greater ecological sensitivity and planetary concern, and a
feeling of being more intuitive. Changes may also include a need for
being alone more often, increased physical sensitivity; diminished
tolerance of light, sound, alcohol, or drugs; a feeling that the brain
has been "altered" to encompass more; and a feeling that one is now
using the "whole brain" rather than a small part.
[3] However, not all after-effects are beneficial
[29]
and Greyson describes circumstances where changes in attitudes and
behavior can lead to psychosocial and psychospiritual problems.
[30]
Historical reports, incidence and prevalence
NDEs have been recorded since ancient times.
[31]
In the 19th century a few studies moved beyond individual cases - one
privately done by the Mormons and one in Switzerland. Up to 2005, 95% of
world cultures are known to have made some mention of NDEs.
[31]
A number of more contemporary sources report incidences of near death experiences of:
- 17% amongst critically ill patients, in nine prospective studies from 4 different countries.[32]
- 10-20% of people who have come close to death.[11]
Research
Near-death studies
Contemporary interest in this field of study was originally spurred by the writings of
Raymond Moody such as his book
Life After Life,
which was released in 1975, brought public attention to the topic of
NDEs. This was soon to be followed by the establishment of the
International Association for Near-Death Studies (
IANDS)
in 1981. IANDS is an international organization that encourages
scientific research and education on the physical, psychological,
social, and spiritual nature and ramifications of near-death
experiences. Among its publications are the peer-reviewed
Journal of Near-Death Studies and the quarterly newsletter
Vital Signs.
Bruce Greyson (
psychiatrist),
Kenneth Ring (
psychologist), and
Michael Sabom (
cardiologist), helped to launch the field of
near-death studies
and introduced the study of near-death experiences to the academic
setting. From 1975 to 2005, some 2,500 self-reported individuals in the
US had been reviewed in retrospective studies of the phenomena
[31] with an additional 600 outside the US in the West,
[31] and 70 in Asia.
[31]
Additionally, prospective studies had identified 270 individuals. Prospective studies review groups of individuals (e.g., selected
emergency room patients) and then find who had an NDE during the study's
time; such studies cost more to perform.
[31]
In all, close to 3,500 individual cases between 1975 and 2005 had been
reviewed in one or another study. All these studies were carried out by
some 55 researchers or teams of researchers.
[31]
Melvin Morse, head of the Institute for the Scientific Study of Consciousness, and colleagues
[16][33] have investigated near-death experiences in a pediatric population.
Research scales used to classify a near-death experience
Major contributions to the field include Ring's construction of a "Weighted Core Experience Index"
[34] to measure the depth of the near-death experience, and Greyson's construction of the "Near-death experience scale"
[35] to differentiate between subjects that are more or less likely to have experienced an NDE.
The latter scale is also, according to its author, clinically useful
in differentiating NDEs from organic brain syndromes and non-specific
stress responses.
[35] The NDE-scale was later found to fit the Rasch rating scale model.
[36]
Research in animals
Heightened
brain activity has been recorded in experimental rats directly
following cardiac arrest, though there has been no similar research in
humans.
Clinical research in cardiac arrest patients
Parnia 2001 study
In 2001,
Sam Parnia
and colleagues published the results of a year-long study of cardiac
arrest survivors that was conducted at Southampton General Hospital. 63
survivors were interviewed. They had been resuscitated after being
clinically dead with no pulse, no respiration, and fixed dilated pupils. Parnia and colleagues investigated
out-of-body experience
claims by placing figures on suspended boards facing the ceiling, not
visible from the floor. Four had experiences that, according to the
study criteria, were NDEs but none of them experienced the
out-of-body experience. Thus, they were not able to identify the figures.
[41][5][42]
Psychologist
Chris French wrote regarding the study "unfortunately, and somewhat atypically, none of the survivors in this sample experienced an OBE".
[5]
Van Lommel's study
In 2001
Pim van Lommel,
a cardiologist from the Netherlands, and his team conducted a study on
NDEs including 344 cardiac arrest patients who had been successfully
resuscitated in 10 Dutch hospitals. Patients not reporting NDEs were
used as controls for patients who did, and psychological (e.g. fear
before cardiac arrest), demographic (e.g. age, sex), medical (e.g. more
than one
cardiopulmonary resuscitation
(CPR)) and pharmacological data were compared between the 2 groups. The
work also included a longitudinal study where the 2 groups (those who
had had an NDE and those who had not had one) were compared at 2 and 8
years, for life changes. One patient had a conventional out of body
experience. He reported being able to watch and recall events during the
time of his cardiac arrest. His claims were confirmed by hospital
personnel. "This did not appear consistent with hallucinatory or
illusory experiences, as the recollections were compatible with real and
verifiable rather than imagined events".
[42][43]
Awareness during Resuscitation (AWARE) study
While at University of Southampton, Parnia was the principal investigator of the AWARE Study, which was launched in 2008.
[13]
This study which concluded in 2012 included 33 investigators across 15
medical centers in the UK, Austria and the USA and tested consciousness,
memories and awareness during cardiac arrest. The accuracy of claims of
visual and auditory awareness was examined using specific tests.
[44]
One such test consisted in installing shelves, bearing a variety of
images and facing the ceiling, hence not visible by hospital staff, in
rooms where cardiac-arrest patients were more likely to occur. The
results of the study were published in October 2014; both the launch and
the study results were widely discussed in the media.
[45][46]
A review article analyzing the results reports that, out of 2060
cardiac arrest events, 101 of 140 cardiac arrest survivors could
complete the questionnaires. Of these 101 patients 9% could be
classified as near death experiences. 2 more patients (2% of those
completing the questionnaires) described "seeing and hearing actual
events related to the period of cardiac arrest". These two patients'
cardiac arrests did not occur in areas equipped with ceiling shelves
hence no images could be used to objectively test for visual awareness
claims. One of the two patients was too sick and the accuracy of her
recount could not be verified. For the second patient instead, it was
possible to verify the accuracy of the experience and to show that
awareness occurred paradoxically some minutes after the heart stopped,
at a time when "the brain ordinarily stops functioning and cortical
activity becomes isoelectric." The experience was not compatible with an
illusion, imaginary event or hallucination since visual (other than of
ceiling shelves' images) and auditory awareness could be corroborated.
[42]
AWARE II
As of May 2016, a posting at the UK Clinical Trials Gateway website described plans for
AWARE II,
a two-year multicenter observational study of 900-1500 patients
experiencing cardiac arrest, which said that subject recruitment had
started on 1 August 2014 and that the scheduled end date was 31 May
2017.
[47]
Explanatory models
In a review article, psychologist
Chris French[5]
has grouped approaches to explain NDEs in three broad groups which "are
not distinct and independent, but instead show considerable overlap":
spiritual theories (also called transcendental), psychological theories,
and physiological theories that provide a physical explanation for
NDEs.
Spiritual or transcendental theories
French
summarizes this model by saying : "the most popular interpretation is
that the NDE is exactly what it appears to be to the person having the
experience".
[5]
The NDE would then represent evidence of the supposedly immaterial
existence of a soul or mind, which would leave the body upon death. An
NDE would then provide information about an immaterial world where the
soul would journey upon ending its physical existence on earth.
[5]
According to Greyson
[11]
some NDE phenomena cannot be easily explained with our current
knowledge of human physiology and psychology. For instance, at a time
when they were unconscious patients could accurately describe events as
well as report being able to view their bodies "from an out-of-body
spatial perspective". In two different studies of patients who had
survived a cardiac arrest, those who had reported leaving their bodies
could describe accurately their resuscitation procedures or unexpected
events, whereas others "described incorrect equipment and procedures".
[11] Sam Parnia also refers to two cardiac arrest studies and one deep
hypothermic circulatory arrest study where patients reported visual
and/or auditory awareness occurring when their brain function had
ceased. These reports "were corroborated with actual and real events".
[48][42]
Limitations of spiritual or transcendental theories
Five
prospective studies have been carried out, to test the accuracy of out
of body perceptions by placing "unusual targets in locations likely to
be seen by persons having NDEs, such as in an upper corner of a room in
the emergency department, the coronary care unit, or the intensive care
unit of a hospital." Twelve patients reported leaving their bodies, but
unfortunately none could describe the hidden visual targets. Although
this is a small sample, the failure of purported out-of-body
experiencers to describe the hidden targets raises questions about the
accuracy of the anecdotal reports described above.
[11]
Psychologist
James Alcock has described the afterlife claims of NDE researchers as
pseudoscientific. Alcock has written the spiritual or transcendental interpretation "is
based on belief in search of data rather than observation in search of
explanation."
[49]
Chris French has noted that "the survivalist approach does not appear
to generate clear and testable hypotheses. Because of the vagueness and
imprecision of the survivalist account, it can be made to explain any
possible set of findings and is therefore unfalsifiable and
unscientific."
[50]
Psychological explanations
French
summarises the main psychological explanations which include: the
depersonalization, the expectancy and the dissociation models.
[5]
Depersonalization model
A
depersonalization model was proposed in the 1970s by professor of
psychiatry Russell Noyes and clinical psychologist Roy Kletti, which
suggested that the NDE is a form of
depersonalization
experienced under emotional conditions such as life-threatening danger,
potentially inescapable danger, and that the NDE can best be understood
as an hallucination.
According to this model, those who face their impending death become
detached from the surroundings and their own bodies, no longer feel
emotions, and experience time distortions.
[11]
Limitations of the depersonalization model
This
model suffers from a number of limitations to explain NDEs for subjects
who do not experience a sensation of being out of their bodies; unlike
NDEs, experiences are dreamlike, unpleasant and characterized by
"anxiety, panic and emptiness".
[11]
Also, during NDEs subjects remain very lucid of their identities, their
sense of identify is not changed unlike those experiencing
depersonalization.
[11]
Expectancy model
Another
psychological theory is called the expectancy model. It has been
suggested that although these experiences could appear very real, they
had actually been constructed in the mind, either consciously or
subconsciously, in response to the stress of an encounter with death (or
perceived encounter with death), and did not correspond to a real
event. In a way, they are similar to wish-fulfillment: because someone
thought they were about to die, they experienced certain things in
accordance with what they expected or wanted to occur. Imagining a
heavenly place was in effect a way for them to soothe themselves through
the stress of knowing that they were close to death.
[5] Subjects use their own personal and cultural expectations to imagine a
scenario that would protect them against an imminent threat to their
lives.
[11]
Limitations of the expectancy model
Subjects'
accounts often differed from their own "religious and personal
expectations regarding death" which contradicts the hypothesis they may
have imagined a scenario based on their cultural and personal
background.
[11]
Although the term NDE was first coined in 1975 and the experience
first described then, recent descriptions of NDEs do not differ from
those reported earlier than 1975. The only exception is the more
frequent description of a tunnel. Hence, the fact that information about
these experiences could be more easily obtained after 1975, did not
influence people's reports of the experiences.
[11]
Another flaw of this model can be found in children's accounts of
NDEs. These are similar to adults', and this despite children being less
affected by religious or cultural influences about death.
[11]
Dissociation model
The
dissociation model proposes that NDE is a form of withdrawal to protect
an individual from a stressful event. Under extreme circumstances some
people may detach from certain unwanted feelings in order to avoid
experiencing their emotional impact and suffering associated with them.
The person also detaches from one's immediate surroundings.
[5]
Birth model
The
birth model suggests that near death experiences could be a form of
reliving the trauma of birth. Since a baby travels from the darkness of
the womb to light and is greeted by the love and warmth of the nursing
and medical staff, and so, it was proposed, the dying brain could be
recreating the passage through a tunnel to light, warmth and affection.
[5]
Limitations of the birth model
Reports
of leaving the body through a tunnel are equally frequent among
subjects who were born by cesarean section and natural birth. Also,
newborns do not possess "the visual acuity, spatial stability of their
visual images, mental alertness, and cortical coding capacity to
register memories of the birth experience".
[11]
Physiological explanations (organic theories)
A wide range of physiological theories of the NDE have been put forward including those based upon
cerebral hypoxia, anoxia, and
hypercapnia; endorphins and other neurotransmitters; and abnormal activity in the
temporal lobes.
[5]
Neurobiological factors in the experience have been investigated by researchers in the field of medical science and psychiatry.
[55] Among the researchers and commentators who tend to emphasize a naturalistic and neurological base for the experience are the
British psychologist Susan Blackmore (1993), with her "dying brain hypothesis".
[56]
Neuroanatomical models
Neuroscientists Olaf Blanke and Sebastian Dieguez (2009),
[57] from the
Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne,
Switzerland, propose a brain based model with two types of NDEs :
- "type 1 NDEs are due to bilateral frontal and occipital, but
predominantly right hemispheric brain damage affecting the right
temporal parietal junction and characterized by out of body experiences,
altered sense of time, sensations of flying, lightness vection and
flying" [6]
- "type 2 NDEs are also due to bilateral frontal and occipital, but
predominantly left hemispheric brain damage affecting the left temporal
parietal junction and characterized by feeling of a presence, meeting
and communication with spirits, seeing of glowing bodies, as well as
voices, sounds, and music without vection" [6]
They suggest that damage to the bilateral
occipital cortex
may lead to visual features of NDEs such as seeing a tunnel or lights,
and "damage to unilateral or bilateral temporal lobe structures such as
the
hippocampus
and amygdala" may lead to emotional experiences, memory flashbacks or a
life review. They concluded that future neuroscientific studies are
likely to reveal the neuroanatomical basis of the NDE which will lead to
the demystification of the subject without needing paranormal
explanations.
[6]
Animation of the human left temporal lobe
French has written that the "temporal lobe is almost certain to be
involved in NDEs, given that both damage to and direct cortical
stimulation of this area are known to produce a number of experiences
corresponding to those of the NDE, including OBEs, hallucinations, and
memory flashbacks".
[5]
Vanhaudenhuyse
et al. 2009 reported that recent studies employing
deep brain stimulation and
neuroimaging have demonstrated that out-of-body experiences result from a deficient
multisensory integration at the
temporoparietal junction
and that ongoing studies aim to further identify the functional
neuroanatomy of near-death experiences by means of standardized EEG
recordings.
[58]
According to Greyson
[11]
multiple neuroanatomical models have been proposed where NDEs have been
hypothesized to originate from different anatomical areas of the brain,
namely: the limbic system, the hippocampus, the left temporal lobe,
Reissen's fiber in the central canal of the spinal cord, the prefrontal
cortex, the right temporal lobe.
Limits of neuroanatomical models
Blanke et al.
[6] admit that their model remains speculative to the lack of data. Likewise Greyson
[11]
writes that although some or any of the neuroanatomical models proposed
may serve to explain NDEs and pathways through which they are
expressed, they remain speculative at this stage since they have not
been tested in empirical studies.
[11]
Neurochemical models
Some theories hypothesize that drugs used during resuscitation induced NDEs, for example,
ketamine or as resulting from endogeneous chemicals that transmit signals between brain cells,
neurotransmitters:
[5]
- In the early eighties, Daniel Carr wrote that NDE has characteristics are suggestive of a limbic lobe syndrome and that the NDE can be explained by the release of endorphins and enkephalins in the brain.[59][60]
Endorphins are endogenous molecules "released in times of stress and
lead to a reduction in pain perception and a pleasant, even blissful,
emotional state." [5]
- Judson and Wiltshaw (1983) noted how the administration of endorphin-blocking agents such as naloxone had been occasionally reported to produce "hellish" NDEs.[61] This would be coherent with endorphins' role in causing a "positive emotional tone of most NDEs".[5]
- Morse et al. 1989 proposed a model arguing that serotonin played a more important role than endorphins in generating NDEs [62] "at least with respect to mystical hallucinations and OBEs".[5]
Limits of neurochemical models
According
to Parnia, neurochemical models are not backed by data. This is true
for "NMDA receptor activation, serotonin, and endorphin release" models.
[42]
Parnia writes that no data has been collected via thorough and careful
experimentation to back "a possible causal relationship or even an
association" between neurochemical agents and NDE experiences.
[48]
Multi-factorial models
The first formal neurobiological model for NDE, included endorphins, neurotransmitters of the
limbic system, the temporal lobe and other parts of the brain.
[63] Extensions and variations of their model came from other scientists such as Louis Appleby (1989).
[64]
Other authors suggest that all components of near-death experiences
can be explained in their entirety via psychological or
neurophysiological mechanisms, although the authors admit that these
hypotheses have to be tested by science.
[65]
Low oxygen levels (and G-LOC) model
Low
oxygen levels in the blood (hypoxia or anoxia) have been hypothesized
to induce hallucinations and hence possibly explain NDEs.
[14][5]
This is because low oxygen levels characterize life-threatening
situations and also by the apparent similarities between NDEs and
G-force induced loss of consciousness (
G-LOC) episodes.
These episodes are observed with fighter pilots experiencing very
rapid and intense acceleration that result in lack of sufficient blood
supply to the brain. Whinnery
[66]
studied almost 1000 cases and noted how the experiences often involved
"tunnel vision and bright lights, floating sensations, automatic
movement, autoscopy, OBEs, not wanting to be disturbed, paralysis, vivid
dreamlets of beautiful places, pleasurable sensations, psychological
alterations of euphoria and dissociation, inclusion of friends and
family, inclusion of prior memories and thoughts, the experience being
very memorable (when it can be remembered), confabulation, and a strong
urge to understand the experience."
[5][66]
Limitations of low oxygen levels (and G-LOC) model
However,
hypoxia-induced acceleration's primary characteristics are "rythmic
jerking of the limbs, compromised memory of events just prior to the
onset of unconsciousness, tingling of extremities ..." that are not
observed during NDEs.
[14]
Also G-LOC episodes do not feature life reviews, mystical experiences
and "long-lasting transformational aftereffects", although this may be
due to the fact that subjects have no expectation of dying.
[5]
Also, hypoxic hallucinations are characterized by "distress and
agitation" and this is very different from near death experiences which
subjects report as being pleasent.
[11]
Altered blood gas levels models and their limitations
Some investigators have studied whether
hypercarbia
or higher than normal carbon dioxide levels, could explain the
occurrence of NDEs. However, studies are difficult to interpret since
NDEs have been observed both with increased levels as well as decreased
levels of carbon dioxide, and finally some other studies have observed
NDEs when levels had not changed, and there is little data.
[14]
Other models
French said that at least some reports of NDEs might be based upon
false memories.
[67]
According to Engmann (2008) near-death experiences of people who are
clinically dead
are psychopathological symptoms caused by a severe malfunction of the
brain resulting from the cessation of cerebral blood circulation.
[68]
An important question is whether it is possible to "translate" the
bloomy experiences of the reanimated survivors into psychopathologically
basic phenomena, e.g., acoasms (nonverbal auditory hallucinations),
central narrowing of the visual field,
autoscopia,
visual hallucinations, activation of limbic and memory structures according to Moody's stages. The symptoms suppose a primary affliction of the
occipital and
temporal cortices under clinical death. This basis could be congruent with the thesis of
pathoclisis—the
inclination of special parts of the brain to be the first to be damaged
in case of disease, lack of oxygen, or malnutrition—established eighty
years ago by
Cécile and
Oskar Vogt.
[69]
Professor of neurology
Terence Hines (2003) claimed that near-death experiences are hallucinations caused by
cerebral anoxia, drugs, or brain damage.
[70]
Cross-cultural aspects
Gregory
Shushan published an analysis of the afterlife beliefs of five ancient
civilizations (Old and Middle Kingdom Egypt, Sumerian and Old Babylonian
Mesopotamia, Vedic India, pre-Buddhist China, and pre-Columbian
Mesoamerica) and compared them with historical and contemporary reports
of near-death experiences, and
shamanic
afterlife "journeys". Shushan found similarities across time, place,
and culture that he found could not be explained by coincidence; he also
found elements that were specific to cultures; Shushan concludes that
some form of mutual influence between experiences of an afterlife and
culture probably influence one another and that this inheritance in turn
influences individual NDEs.
[71]
According to Parnia, near death experiences' interpretations are
influenced by religious, social, cultural backgrounds. However, the core
elements appear to transcend borders and can be considered universal.
In fact, some of these core elements have even been reported by children
<3 age="" an="" and="" at="" been="" by="" children="" class="reference" communicated="" culture="" have="" id="cite_ref-Parnia2014rev_42-5" in="" influenced="" language="" many="" months="" not="" occurred="" old="" or="" other="" over="" playing="" s="" should="" sup="" they="" this="" tradition.="" using="" where="" whilst="" words="" years="">
[42]3>
Also, according to Greyson,
[11]
the central features of NDEs are universal and have not been influenced
by time. These have been observed throughout history and in different
cultures. This notwithstanding, cultural influences have probably played
a role in some NDEs' reported descriptions.