From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Score distribution chart for sample of 905 children tested on 1916 Stanford–Binet Test
IQ classification is the practice by IQ test publishers of labeling IQ score ranges with category names such as "superior" or "average".
The current scoring method for all IQ tests is the "deviation
IQ". In this method, an IQ score of 100 means that the test-taker's
performance on the test is at the median level of performance in the
sample of test-takers of about the same age as was used to norm the
test. An IQ score of 115 means performance one standard deviation above the median, a score of 85 performance, one standard deviation below the median, and so on.
Deviation IQs are now used for standard scoring of all IQ tests in
large part because they allow a consistent definition of IQ for both
children and adults. By the current "deviation IQ" definition of IQ test
standard scores, about two-thirds of all test-takers obtain scores from
85 to 115, and about 5 percent of the population scores above 125.
Lewis Terman
and other early developers of IQ tests noticed that most child IQ
scores come out to approximately the same number regardless of testing
procedure. All IQ tests show a slight variation in scores even when the
same person takes the same test over and over again. IQ scores also slightly differ for a test-taker taking tests from more than one publisher at the same age. The various test publishers do not use uniform names or definitions for IQ score classifications.
Even before IQ tests were invented, there were attempts to classify people into intelligence categories by observing their behavior in daily life.
Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for
validating classifications based primarily on IQ test scores. Both
intelligence classification by observation of behavior outside the
testing room and classification by IQ testing depend on the definition
of "intelligence" used in a particular case and on the reliability and error of estimation in the classification procedure.
Differences in individual IQ classification
IQ scores can differ to some degree for the same person on
different IQ tests, so a person does not always belong to the same IQ
score range each time the person is tested. (IQ score table data and
pupil pseudonyms adapted from description of KABC-II norming study cited
in Kaufman 2009.)
Pupil |
Asher |
90 |
95 |
111
|
Brianna |
125 |
110 |
105
|
Colin |
100 |
93 |
101
|
Danica |
116 |
127 |
118
|
Elpha |
93 |
105 |
93
|
Fritz |
106 |
105 |
105
|
Georgi |
95 |
100 |
90
|
Hector |
112 |
113 |
103
|
Imelda |
104 |
96 |
97
|
Jose |
101 |
99 |
86
|
Keoku |
81 |
78 |
75
|
Leo |
116 |
124 |
102
|
IQ tests generally are reliable enough that most people 10 years of age and older have similar IQ scores throughout life.
Still, some individuals score very differently when taking the same
test at different times or when taking more than one kind of IQ test at
the same age. For example, many children in the famous longitudinal Genetic Studies of Genius begun in 1921 by Lewis Terman showed declines in IQ as they grew up.
Terman recruited school pupils based on referrals from teachers, and gave them his Stanford–Binet
IQ test. Children with an IQ above 140 by that test were included in
the study. There were 643 children in the main study group. When the
students who could be contacted again (503 students) were retested at
high school age, they were found to have dropped 9 IQ points on average
in Stanford–Binet IQ. More than two dozen children dropped by 15 IQ
points and six by 25 points or more. Yet parents of those children
thought that the children were still as bright as ever, or even
brighter.
Because all IQ tests have error of measurement in the
test-taker's IQ score, a test-giver should always inform the test-taker
of the confidence interval around the score obtained on a given occasion
of taking each test. IQ scores are ordinal scores and are not expressed in an interval measurement unit.
Besides the inherent error band around any IQ test score because tests
are a "sample racks of learned behavior", IQ scores can also be
misleading because test-givers fail to follow standardized
administration and scoring procedures. In cases of test-giver mistakes,
the usual result is that tests are scored too leniently, giving the
test-taker a higher IQ score than the test-taker's performance
justifies. Some test-givers err by showing a "halo effect",
with low-IQ individuals receiving IQ scores even lower than if
standardized procedures were followed, while high-IQ individuals receive
inflated IQ scores.
IQ classifications for individuals also vary because category
labels for IQ score ranges are specific to each brand of test. The test
publishers do not have a uniform practice of labeling IQ score ranges,
nor do they have a consistent practice of dividing up IQ score ranges
into categories of the same size or with the same boundary scores. Thus psychologists should specify which test was given when reporting a test-taker's IQ.
Psychologists and IQ test authors recommend that psychologists adopt
the terminology of each test publisher when reporting IQ score ranges.
IQ classifications from IQ testing are not the last word on how a
test-taker will do in life, nor are they the only information to be
considered for placement in school or job-training programs. There is
still a dearth of information about how behavior differs between persons
with differing IQ scores.
For placement in school programs, for medical diagnosis, and for career
advising, factors other than IQ must also be part of an individual
assessment.
The lesson here is that
classification systems are necessarily arbitrary and change at the whim
of test authors, government bodies, or professional organizations. They
are statistical concepts and do not correspond in any real sense to the
specific capabilities of any particular person with a given IQ. The
classification systems provide descriptive labels that may be useful for
communication purposes in a case report or conference, and nothing
more.
— Alan S. Kaufman and Elizabeth O. Lichtenberger, Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence (2006)
IQ classification tables for current tests
There are a variety of individually administered IQ tests in use in the English-speaking world. Not all report test results as "IQ", but most now report a standard
score with a median score level of 100. When a test-taker scores higher
or lower than the median score, the score is indicated as 15 standard
score points higher or lower for each standard deviation difference
higher or lower in the test-taker's performance on the test item
content.
Wechsler Intelligence Scales
The Wechsler intelligence scales were originally developed from earlier intelligence scales by David Wechsler. The first Wechsler test published was the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale in 1939.
The Wechsler IQ tests for children and for adults are the most
frequently used individual IQ tests in the English-speaking world and in their translated versions are perhaps the most widely used IQ tests worldwide. The Wechsler tests have long been regarded as the "gold standard" in IQ testing. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV) was published in 2008 by The Psychological Corporation.
The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth Edition (WISC–V) was
published in 2014 by The Psychological Corporation, and the Wechsler
Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Fourth Edition (WPPSI–IV)
was published in 2012 by The Psychological Corporation. Like all current
IQ tests, the Wechsler tests report a "deviation IQ" as the standard
score for the full-scale IQ, with the norming sample median raw score
defined as IQ 100 and a score one standard deviation higher defined as
IQ 115 (and one deviation lower defined as IQ 85).
Current Wechsler (WAIS–IV, WPPSI–IV) IQ classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
130 and above
|
Very Superior
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High Average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low Average
|
70–79
|
Borderline
|
69 and below
|
Extremely Low
|
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fifth Edition (WISC-V) IQ classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
130 and above
|
Extremely High
|
120–129
|
Very High
|
110–119
|
High Average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low Average
|
70–79
|
Very Low
|
69 and below
|
Extremely Low
|
Psychologists have proposed alternative language for Wechsler IQ classifications.
The term "borderline", which implies being very close to being
intellectually disabled, is replaced in the alternative system by a term
that doesn't imply a medical diagnosis.
Alternate Wechsler IQ Classifications (after Groth-Marnat 2009)
Corresponding IQ Range |
Classifications |
More value-neutral terms
|
130+
|
Very superior
|
Upper extreme
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
Well above average
|
110–119
|
High average
|
High average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low average
|
Low average
|
70–79
|
Borderline
|
Well below average
|
69 and below
|
Extremely low
|
Lower extreme
|
Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale Fifth Edition
The current fifth edition of the Stanford–Binet scales (SB5) was
developed by Gale H. Roid and published in 2003 by Riverside Publishing.
Unlike scoring on previous versions of the Stanford–Binet test, SB5 IQ
scoring is deviation scoring in which each standard deviation up or down
from the norming sample median score is 15 points from the median
score, IQ 100, just like the standard scoring on the Wechsler tests.
Stanford–Binet Fifth Edition (SB5) classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
144+
|
Very gifted or highly advanced
|
130–144
|
Gifted or very advanced
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low average
|
70–79
|
Borderline impaired or delayed
|
55–69
|
Mildly impaired or delayed
|
40–54
|
Moderately impaired or delayed
|
Woodcock–Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities
The Woodcock–Johnson a III NU Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III NU) was developed by Richard W. Woodcock, Kevin S. McGrew and Nancy Mather and published in 2007 by Riverside. The WJ III classification terms are not applied.
Woodcock–Johnson R
IQ Score |
WJ III Classification
|
131 and above
|
Very superior
|
121 to 130
|
Superior
|
111 to 120
|
High Average
|
90 to 110
|
Average
|
80 to 89
|
Low Average
|
70 to 79
|
Low
|
69 and below
|
Very Low
|
Kaufman Tests
The Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1993 by American Guidance Service. Kaufman test scores "are classified in a symmetrical, nonevaluative fashion",
in other words the score ranges for classification are just as wide
above the median as below the median, and the classification labels do
not purport to assess individuals.
KAIT 1993 IQ classification
130 and above
|
Upper Extreme
|
120–129
|
Well Above Average
|
110–119
|
Above average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Below Average
|
70–79
|
Well Below Average
|
69 and below
|
Lower Extreme
|
The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition was
developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 2004
by American Guidance Service.
KABC-II 2004 Descriptive Categories
Range of Standard Scores |
Name of Category
|
131–160
|
Upper Extreme
|
116–130
|
Above Average
|
85–115
|
Average Range
|
70–84
|
Below Average
|
40–69
|
Lower Extreme
|
Cognitive Assessment System
The Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System test was developed by
Jack Naglieri and J. P. Das and published in 1997 by Riverside.
Cognitive Assessment System 1997 full scale score classification
Standard Scores |
Classification
|
130 and above
|
Very Superior
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High Average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low Average
|
70–79
|
Below Average
|
69 and below
|
Well Below Average
|
Differential Ability Scales
The Differential Ability Scales Second Edition (DAS–II) was developed
by Colin D. Elliott and published in 2007 by Psychological Corporation.
The DAS-II is a test battery given individually to children, normed for
children from ages two years and six months through seventeen years and
eleven months. It was normed on 3,480 noninstitutionalized, English-speaking children in that age range.
The DAS-II yields a General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score scaled like
an IQ score with the median standard score set at 100 and 15 standard
score points for each standard deviation up or down from the median. The
lowest possible GCA score on DAS–II is 30, and the highest is 170.
DAS-II 2007 GCA classification
GCA |
General Conceptual Ability Classification
|
≥ 130
|
Very high
|
120–129
|
High
|
110–119
|
Above average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Below average
|
70–79
|
Low
|
≤ 69
|
Very low
|
Reynolds Intellectual Ability Scales
Reynolds
Intellectual Ability Scales (RIAS) were developed by Cecil Reynolds and
Randy Kamphaus. The RIAS was published in 2003 by Psychological
Assessment Resources.
RIAS 2003 Scheme of Verbal Descriptors of Intelligence Test Performance
Intelligence test score range |
Verbal descriptor
|
≥ 130
|
Significantly above average
|
120–129
|
Moderately above average
|
110–119
|
Above average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Below average
|
70–79
|
Moderately below average
|
≤ 69
|
Significantly below average
|
Historical IQ classification tables
Reproduction
of an item from the 1908 Binet–Simon intelligence scale, showing three
pairs of pictures, about which the tested child was asked, "Which of
these two faces is the prettier?" Reproduced from the article "A
Practical Guide for Administering the Binet–Simon Scale for Measuring
Intelligence" by
J. E. Wallace Wallin in the March 1911 issue of the journal
The Psychological Clinic (volume 5 number 1), public domain.
Lewis Terman, developer of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales,
based his English-language Stanford–Binet IQ test on the French-language
Binet–Simon test developed by Alfred Binet. Terman believed his test measured the "general intelligence" construct advocated by Charles Spearman (1904). Terman differed from Binet in reporting scores on his test in the form of intelligence quotient ("mental age" divided by chronological age) scores after the 1912 suggestion of German psychologist William Stern.
Terman chose the category names for score levels on the Stanford–Binet
test. When he first chose classification for score levels, he relied
partly on the usage of earlier authors who wrote, before the existence
of IQ tests, on topics such as individuals unable to care for themselves
in independent adult life. Terman's first version of the Stanford–Binet
was based on norming samples that included only white, American-born
subjects, mostly from California, Nevada, and Oregon.
Terman's Stanford–Binet original (1916) classification
IQ Range ("ratio IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
Above 140
|
"Near" genius or genius
|
120–140
|
Very superior intelligence
|
110–120
|
Superior intelligence
|
90–110
|
Normal, or average, intelligence
|
80–90
|
Dullness, rarely classifiable as feeble-mindedness
|
70–80
|
Border-line deficiency, sometimes classifiable as dullness, often as feeble-mindedness
|
Below 70
|
Definite feeble-mindedness
|
Rudolph Pintner proposed a set of classification terms in his 1923 book Intelligence Testing: Methods and Results.
Pintner commented that psychologists of his era, including Terman, went
about "the measurement of an individual's general ability without
waiting for an adequate psychological definition." Pintner retained these terms in the 1931 second edition of his book.
Pintner 1923 IQ classification
IQ Range ("ratio IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
130 and above
|
Very Superior
|
120–129
|
Very Bright
|
110–119
|
Bright
|
90–109
|
Normal
|
80–89
|
Backward
|
70–79
|
Borderline
|
Albert Julius Levine and Louis Marks proposed a broader set of categories in their 1928 book Testing Intelligence and Achievement. Some of the entries came from contemporary terms for people with intellectual disability.
Levine and Marks 1928 IQ classification
IQ Range ("ratio IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
175 and over
|
Precocious
|
150–174
|
Very superior
|
125–149
|
Superior
|
115–124
|
Very bright
|
105–114
|
Bright
|
95–104
|
Average
|
85–94
|
Dull
|
75–84
|
Borderline
|
50–74
|
Morons
|
25–49
|
Imbeciles
|
0–24
|
Idiots
|
The second revision (1937) of the Stanford–Binet test retained
"quotient IQ" scoring, despite earlier criticism of that method of
reporting IQ test standard scores. The term "genius" was no longer used for any IQ score range. The second revision was normed only on children and adolescents (no adults), and only "American-born white children".
Terman's Stanford–Binet Second Revision (1937) classification
IQ Range ("ratio IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
140 and over
|
Very superior
|
120–139
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High average
|
90–109
|
Normal or average
|
80–89
|
Low average
|
70–79
|
Borderline defective
|
Below 70
|
Mentally defective
|
A data table published later as part of the manual for the 1960 Third
Revision (Form L-M) of the Stanford–Binet test reported score
distributions from the 1937 second revision standardization group.
Score Distribution of Stanford–Binet 1937 Standardization Group
IQ Range ("ratio IQ") |
Percent of Group
|
160–169
|
0.03
|
150–159
|
0.2
|
140–149
|
1.1
|
130–139
|
3.1
|
120–129
|
8.2
|
110–119
|
18.1
|
100–109
|
23.5
|
90–99
|
23.0
|
80–89
|
14.5
|
70–79
|
5.6
|
60–69
|
2.0
|
50–59
|
0.4
|
40–49
|
0.2
|
30–39
|
0.03
|
David Wechsler, developer of the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale of 1939 (which was later developed into the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale)
popularized the use of "deviation IQs" as standard scores of IQ tests
rather than the "quotient IQs" ("mental age" divided by "chronological
age") then used for the Stanford–Binet test. He devoted a whole chapter in his book The Measurement of Adult Intelligence
to the topic of IQ classification and proposed different category names
from those used by Lewis Terman. Wechsler also criticized the practice
of earlier authors who published IQ classification tables without
specifying which IQ test was used to obtain the scores reported in the
tables.
Wechsler–Bellevue 1939 IQ classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification |
Percent Included
|
128 and over
|
Very Superior
|
2.2
|
120–127
|
Superior
|
6.7
|
111–119
|
Bright Normal
|
16.1
|
91–110
|
Average
|
50.0
|
80–90
|
Dull normal
|
16.1
|
66–79
|
Borderline
|
6.7
|
65 and below
|
Defective
|
2.2
|
In 1958, Wechsler published another edition of his book Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence.
He revised his chapter on the topic of IQ classification and commented
that "mental age" scores were not a more valid way to score intelligence
tests than IQ scores. He continued to use the same classification terms.
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales 1958 Classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification |
(Theoretical) Percent Included
|
128 and over
|
Very Superior
|
2.2
|
120–127
|
Superior
|
6.7
|
111–119
|
Bright Normal
|
16.1
|
91–110
|
Average
|
50.0
|
80–90
|
Dull normal
|
16.1
|
66–79
|
Borderline
|
6.7
|
65 and below
|
Defective
|
2.2
|
The third revision (Form L-M) in 1960 of the Stanford–Binet IQ test
used the deviation scoring pioneered by David Wechsler. For rough
comparability of scores between the second and third revision of the
Stanford–Binet test, scoring table author Samuel Pinneau set 100 for the
median standard score level and 16 standard score points for each
standard deviation above or below that level. The highest score
obtainable by direct look-up from the standard scoring tables (based on
norms from the 1930s) was IQ 171 at various chronological ages from
three years six months (with a test raw score "mental age" of six years
and two months) up to age six years and three months (with a test raw
score "mental age" of ten years and three months).
The classification for Stanford–Binet L-M scores does not include terms
such as "exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" in the test
manual itself. David Freides, reviewing the Stanford–Binet Third
Revision in 1970 for the Buros Seventh Mental Measurements Yearbook (published in 1972), commented that the test was obsolete by that year.
Terman's Stanford–Binet Third Revision (Form L-M) classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
140 and over
|
Very superior
|
120–139
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High average
|
90–109
|
Normal or average
|
80–89
|
Low average
|
70–79
|
Borderline defective
|
Below 70
|
Mentally defective
|
The first edition of the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive
Abilities was published by Riverside in 1977. The classifications used
by the WJ-R Cog were "modern in that they describe levels of performance
as opposed to offering a diagnosis."
Woodcock–Johnson R
IQ Score |
WJ-R Cog 1977 Classification
|
131 and above
|
Very superior
|
121 to 130
|
Superior
|
111 to 120
|
High Average
|
90 to 110
|
Average
|
80 to 89
|
Low Average
|
70 to 79
|
Low
|
69 and below
|
Very Low
|
The revised version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (the
WAIS-R) was developed by David Wechsler and published by Psychological
Corporation in 1981. Wechsler changed a few of the boundaries for
classification categories and a few of their names compared to the 1958
version of the test. The test's manual included information about how
the actual percentage of persons in the norming sample scoring at
various levels compared to theoretical expectations.
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales 1981 Classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification |
Actual Percent Included |
Theoretical Percent Included
|
130+
|
Very Superior
|
2.6
|
2.2
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
6.9
|
6.7
|
110–119
|
High Average
|
16.6
|
16.1
|
90–109
|
Average
|
49.1
|
50.0
|
80–89
|
Low Average
|
16.1
|
16.1
|
70–79
|
Borderline
|
6.4
|
6.7
|
below 70
|
Mentally Retarded
|
2.3
|
2.2
|
The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) was developed by
Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1983 by American
Guidance Service.
K-ABC 1983 Ability Classifications
Range of Standard Scores |
Name of Category |
Percent of Norm Sample |
Theoretical Percent Included
|
130+
|
Upper Extreme
|
2.3
|
2.2
|
120–129
|
Well Above Average
|
7.4
|
6.7
|
110–119
|
Above Average
|
16.7
|
16.1
|
90–109
|
Average
|
49.5
|
50.0
|
80–89
|
Below Average
|
16.1
|
16.1
|
70–79
|
Well Below Average
|
6.1
|
6.7
|
below 70
|
Lower Extreme
|
2.1
|
2.2
|
The fourth revision of the Stanford–Binet scales (S-B IV) was
developed by Thorndike, Hagen, and Sattler and published by Riverside
Publishing in 1986. It retained the deviation scoring of the third
revision with each standard deviation from the median being defined as a
16 IQ point difference. The S-B IV adopted new classification
terminology. After this test was published, psychologist Nathan Brody
lamented that IQ tests had still not caught up with advances in research
on human intelligence during the twentieth century.
Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (S-B IV) 1986 classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
132 and above
|
Very superior
|
121–131
|
Superior
|
111–120
|
High average
|
89–110
|
Average
|
79–88
|
Low average
|
68–78
|
Slow learner
|
67 or below
|
Mentally retarded
|
The third edition of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III)
used different classification terminology from the earliest versions of
Wechsler tests.
Wechsler (WAIS–III) 1997 IQ test classification
IQ Range ("deviation IQ") |
IQ Classification
|
130 and above
|
Very superior
|
120–129
|
Superior
|
110–119
|
High average
|
90–109
|
Average
|
80–89
|
Low average
|
70–79
|
Borderline
|
69 and below
|
Extremely low
|
Classification of low IQ
The earliest terms for classifying individuals of low intelligence
were medical or legal terms that preceded the development of IQ testing.
The legal system recognized a concept of some individuals being so
cognitively impaired that they were not responsible for criminal
behavior. Medical doctors sometimes encountered adult patients who could
not live independently, being unable to take care of their own daily
living needs. Various terms were used to attempt to classify individuals
with varying degrees of intellectual disability. Many of the earliest
terms are now considered extremely offensive.
In current medical diagnosis, IQ scores alone are not conclusive
for a finding of intellectual disability. Recently adopted diagnostic
standards place the major emphasis on the adaptive behavior of each
individual, with IQ score just being one factor in diagnosis in addition
to adaptive behavior scales, and no category of intellectual disability
being defined primarily by IQ scores.
Psychologists point out that evidence from IQ testing should always be
used with other assessment evidence in mind: "In the end, any and all
interpretations of test performance gain diagnostic meaning when they
are corroborated by other data sources and when they are empirically or
logically related to the area or areas of difficulty specified in the
referral."
In the United States, the Supreme Court ruled in the case Atkins v. Virginia,
536 U.S. 304 (2002) that states could not impose capital punishment on
persons with "mental retardation", defined in subsequent cases as
persons with IQ scores below 70. This legal standard continues to be actively litigated in capital cases.
Historical
Historically, terms for intellectual disability eventually become perceived as an insult, in a process commonly known as the euphemism treadmill. The terms mental retardation and mentally retarded became popular in the middle of the 20th century to replace the previous set of terms, which included "imbecile", "idiot", "feeble-minded", and "moron", among others, and are now considered offensive, often extremely so. By the end of the 20th century, retardation and retard became widely seen as disparaging and politically incorrect, although they are still used in some clinical contexts.
The American Association for the Study of the Feeble-minded divided adults with intellectual deficits into three categories. Idiot indicated the greatest degree of intellectual disability in which a person's mental age is below three years. Imbecile indicated an intellectual disability less severe than idiocy and a mental age between three and seven years. Moron was defined as someone a mental age between eight and twelve. Alternative definitions of these terms based on IQ were also used.
The term cretin dates to 1770–80 and comes from a dialectal French word for Christian.
The implication was that people with significant intellectual or
developmental disabilities were "still human" (or "still Christian") and
deserved to be treated with basic human dignity. Although cretin is no longer in use, the term cretinism is still used to refer to the mental and physical disability resulting from untreated congenital hypothyroidism.
Mongolism and Mongoloid idiot were terms used to identify someone with Down syndrome, as the doctor who first described the syndrome, John Langdon Down, believed that children with Down syndrome shared facial similarities with the now-obsolete category of "Mongolian race". The Mongolian People's Republic requested that the medical community cease the use of the term; in 1960, the World Health Organization agreed the term should cease being used.
Retarded comes from the Latin retardare, "to make slow, delay, keep back, or hinder", so mental retardation meant the same as mentally delayed. The first record of retarded in relation to being mentally slow was in 1895. The term mentally retarded was used to replace terms like idiot, moron, and imbecile because retarded was not then a derogatory term. By the 1960s, however, the term had taken on a partially derogatory meaning. The noun retard is particularly seen as pejorative; a BBC survey in 2003 ranked it as the most offensive disability-related word. The terms mentally retarded and mental retardation are still fairly common, but organizations such as the Special Olympics and Best Buddies are striving to eliminate their use and often refer to retard and its variants as the "r-word". These efforts resulted in U.S. federal legislation, known as Rosa's Law, which replaced the term mentally retarded with the term intellectual disability in federal law.
Classification of high IQ
Genius
Galton in his later years
Francis Galton (1822–1911) was a pioneer in investigating both eminent human achievement and mental testing. In his book Hereditary Genius,
writing before the development of IQ testing, he proposed that
hereditary influences on eminent achievement are strong, and that
eminence is rare in the general population. Lewis Terman chose "'near'
genius or genius" as the classification label for the highest
classification on his 1916 version of the Stanford–Binet test.
By 1926, Terman began publishing about a longitudinal study of
California schoolchildren who were referred for IQ testing by their
schoolteachers, called Genetic Studies of Genius, which he conducted for the rest of his life. Catherine M. Cox, a colleague of Terman's, wrote a whole book, The Early Mental Traits of 300 Geniuses, published as volume 2 of The Genetic Studies of Genius
book series, in which she analyzed biographical data about historic
geniuses. Although her estimates of childhood IQ scores of historical
figures who never took IQ tests have been criticized on methodological
grounds, Cox's study was thorough in finding out what else matters besides IQ in becoming a genius.
By the 1937 second revision of the Stanford–Binet test, Terman no
longer used the term "genius" as an IQ classification, nor has any
subsequent IQ test.
In 1939, Wechsler wrote "we are rather hesitant about calling a person a
genius on the basis of a single intelligence test score."
The Terman longitudinal study in California eventually provided historical evidence on how genius is related to IQ scores.
Many California pupils were recommended for the study by
schoolteachers. Two pupils who were tested but rejected for inclusion in
the study because of IQ scores too low for the study grew up to be
Nobel Prize winners in physics: William Shockley and Luis Walter Alvarez. Based on the historical findings of the Terman study and on biographical examples such as Richard Feynman, who had an IQ of 125 and went on to win the Nobel Prize in physics and become widely known as a genius,
the current view of psychologists and other scholars of genius is that a
minimum IQ, about 125, is strictly necessary for genius, but that IQ is
sufficient for the development of genius only when combined with the
other influences identified by Cox's biographical study: an opportunity
for talent development along with the characteristics of drive and
persistence. Charles Spearman, bearing in mind the influential theory
that he originated—that intelligence comprises both a "general factor"
and "special factors" more specific to particular mental tasks—, wrote
in 1927, "Every normal man, woman, and child is, then, a genius at
something, as well as an idiot at something."
Giftedness
A major point of consensus among all scholars of intellectual
giftedness is that there is no generally agreed upon definition of
giftedness.
Although there is no scholarly agreement about identifying gifted
learners, there is a de facto reliance on IQ scores for identifying
participants in school gifted education programs. In practice, many
school districts in the United States use an IQ score of 130, including
roughly the upper 2 to 3 percent of the national population as a cut-off
score for inclusion in school gifted programs.
Five levels of giftedness have been suggested to differentiate
the vast difference in abilities that exists between children on varying
ends of the gifted spectrum.
Although there is no strong consensus on the validity of these
quantifiers, they are accepted by many experts of gifted children.
Levels of Giftedness (M.U. Gross)
Classification |
IQ Range |
σ |
Prevalence
|
Mildly gifted
|
115–129
|
+1.00–+1.99
|
1:6
|
Moderately gifted
|
130–144
|
+2.00–+2.99
|
1:44
|
Highly gifted
|
145–159
|
+3.00–+3.99
|
1:1,000
|
Exceptionally gifted
|
160–179
|
+4.00–+5.33
|
1:10,000
|
Profoundly gifted
|
180–
|
+5.33–
|
< 1:1,000,000
|
As long ago as 1937, Lewis Terman pointed out that error of
estimation in IQ scoring increases as IQ score increases, so that there
is less and less certainty about assigning a test-taker to one band of
scores or another as one looks at higher bands. Current IQ tests also have large error bands for high IQ scores.
As an underlying reality, such distinctions as those between
"exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" have never been well
established. All longitudinal studies of IQ have shown that test-takers
can bounce up and down in score, and thus switch up and down in rank
order as compared to one another, over the course of childhood. Some
test-givers claim that IQ classification categories such as "profoundly
gifted" are meaningful, but those are based on the obsolete
Stanford–Binet Third Revision (Form L-M) test. The highest reported standard score for most IQ tests is IQ 160, approximately the 99.997th percentile (leaving aside the issue of the considerable error in measurement at that level of IQ on any IQ test).
IQ scores above this level are dubious as there are insufficient
normative cases upon which to base a statistically justified
rank-ordering.
Moreover, there has never been any validation of the Stanford–Binet L-M
on adult populations, and there is no trace of such terminology in the
writings of Lewis Terman. Although two current tests attempt to provide
"extended norms" that allow for classification of different levels of
giftedness, those norms are not based on well validated data.