The Kronecker delta appears naturally in many areas of
mathematics, physics, engineering and computer science, as a means of
compactly expressing its definition above.
Here the Euclidean vectors are defined as n-tuples: and and the last step is obtained by using the values of the Kronecker delta to reduce the summation over .
It is common for i and j to be restricted to a set of the form {1, 2, ..., n} or {0, 1, ..., n − 1}, but the Kronecker delta can be defined on an arbitrary set.
Properties
The following equations are satisfied:
Therefore, the matrix δ can be considered as an identity matrix.
Another useful representation is the following form:
Often, a single-argument notation is used, which is equivalent to setting :
In linear algebra, it can be thought of as a tensor, and is written . Sometimes the Kronecker delta is called the substitution tensor.
Digital signal processing
In the study of digital signal processing (DSP), the unit sample function represents a special case of a 2-dimensional Kronecker delta function where the Kronecker indices include the number zero, and where one of the indices is zero. In this case:
Or more generally where:
However, this is only a special case. In tensor calculus, it
is more common to number basis vectors in a particular dimension
starting with index 1, rather than index 0. In this case, the relation
does not exist, and in fact, the Kronecker delta function and the unit
sample function are different functions that overlap in the specific
case where the indices include the number 0, the number of indices is 2,
and one of the indices has the value of zero.
While the discrete unit sample function and the Kronecker delta
function use the same letter, they differ in the following ways. For the
discrete unit sample function, it is more conventional to place a
single integer index in square braces; in contrast the Kronecker delta
can have any number of indexes. Further, the purpose of the discrete
unit sample function is different from the Kronecker delta function. In
DSP, the discrete unit sample function is typically used as an input
function to a discrete system for discovering the system function of the
system which will be produced as an output of the system. In contrast,
the typical purpose of the Kronecker delta function is for filtering
terms from an Einstein summation convention.
The discrete unit sample function is more simply defined as:
In addition, the Dirac delta function is often confused for both the Kronecker delta function and the unit sample function. The Dirac delta is defined as:
Unlike the Kronecker delta function and the unit sample function , the Dirac delta function does not have an integer index, it has a single continuous non-integer value t.
and in fact Dirac's delta was named after the Kronecker delta because of this analogous property.[2]
In signal processing it is usually the context (discrete or continuous
time) that distinguishes the Kronecker and Dirac "functions". And by
convention, generally indicates continuous time (Dirac), whereas arguments like , , , , , and
are usually reserved for discrete time (Kronecker). Another common
practice is to represent discrete sequences with square brackets; thus: . The Kronecker delta is not the result of directly sampling the Dirac delta function.
Equivalently, the probability density function of the distribution can be written using the Dirac delta function as
Under certain conditions, the Kronecker delta can arise from
sampling a Dirac delta function. For example, if a Dirac delta impulse
occurs exactly at a sampling point and is ideally lowpass-filtered (with
cutoff at the critical frequency) per the Nyquist–Shannon sampling theorem, the resulting discrete-time signal will be a Kronecker delta function.
Generalizations
If it is considered as a type tensor, the Kronecker tensor can be written with a covariant index and contravariant index :
This tensor represents:
The identity mapping (or identity matrix), considered as a linear mapping or
The map , representing scalar multiplication as a sum of outer products.
The generalized Kronecker delta or multi-index Kronecker delta of order is a type tensor that is completely antisymmetric in its upper indices, and also in its lower indices.
Two definitions that differ by a factor of are in use. Below, the version is presented has nonzero components scaled to be . The second version has nonzero components that are , with consequent changes scaling factors in formulae, such as the scaling factors of in § Properties of the generalized Kronecker delta below disappearing.
Definitions of the generalized Kronecker delta
In terms of the indices, the generalized Kronecker delta is defined as:
For any integer , using a standard residue
calculation we can write an integral representation for the Kronecker
delta as the integral below, where the contour of the integral goes
counterclockwise around zero. This representation is also equivalent to a
definite integral by a rotation in the complex plane.
The Kronecker comb
The Kronecker comb function with period is defined (using DSP notation) as:
where and are integers. The Kronecker comb thus consists of an infinite series of unit impulses N units apart, and includes the unit impulse at zero. It may be considered to be the discrete analog of the Dirac comb.
Kronecker integral
The Kronecker delta is also called degree of mapping of one surface into another. Suppose a mapping takes place from surface Suvw to Sxyz that are boundaries of regions, Ruvw and Rxyz which is simply connected with one-to-one correspondence. In this framework, if s and t are parameters for Suvw, and Suvw to Suvw are each oriented by the outer normal n:
while the normal has the direction of
Let x = x(u, v, w), y = y(u, v, w), z = z(u, v, w) be defined and smooth in a domain containing Suvw, and let these equations define the mapping of Suvw onto Sxyz. Then the degree δ of mapping is 1/4π times the solid angle of the image S of Suvw with respect to the interior point of Sxyz, O. If O is the origin of the region, Rxyz, then the degree, δ is given by the integral:
Transfer RNA (abbreviated tRNA and formerly referred to as sRNA, for soluble RNA) is an adaptor molecule composed of RNA, typically 76 to 90 nucleotides in length (in eukaryotes), that serves as the physical link between the mRNA and the amino acid
sequence of proteins. Transfer RNA (tRNA) does this by carrying an
amino acid to the protein synthesizing machinery of a cell called the ribosome. Complementation of a 3-nucleotide codon in a messenger RNA
(mRNA) by a 3-nucleotide anticodon of the tRNA results in protein
synthesis based on the mRNA code. As such, tRNAs are a necessary
component of translation, the biological synthesis of new proteins in accordance with the genetic code.
Typically, tRNAs genes from Bacteria are shorter (mean = 77.6 bp)
than tRNAs from Archaea (mean = 83.1 bp) and eukaryotes (mean = 84.7
bp).
The mature tRNA follows an opposite pattern with tRNAs from Bacteria
being usually longer (median = 77.6 nt) than tRNAs from Archaea (median =
76.8 nt), with eukaryotes exhibiting the shortest mature tRNAs (median =
74.5 nt).
Overview
While the specific nucleotide sequence of an mRNA specifies which amino acids
are incorporated into the protein product of the gene from which the
mRNA is transcribed, the role of tRNA is to specify which sequence from
the genetic code corresponds to which amino acid.
The mRNA encodes a protein as a series of contiguous codons, each of
which is recognized by a particular tRNA. One end of the tRNA matches
the genetic code in a three-nucleotide sequence called the anticodon. The anticodon forms three complementarybase pairs with a codon in mRNA during protein biosynthesis.
On the other end of the tRNA is a covalent attachment to the
amino acid that corresponds to the anticodon sequence. Each type of tRNA
molecule can be attached to only one type of amino acid, so each
organism has many types of tRNA. Because the genetic code contains
multiple codons that specify the same amino acid, there are several tRNA
molecules bearing different anticodons which carry the same amino acid.
The covalent attachment to the tRNA 3’ end is catalysed by enzymes called aminoacyl tRNA synthetases. During protein synthesis, tRNAs with attached amino acids are delivered to the ribosome by proteins called elongation factors,
which aid in association of the tRNA with the ribosome, synthesis of
the new polypeptide, and translocation (movement) of the ribosome along
the mRNA. If the tRNA's anticodon matches the mRNA, another tRNA already
bound to the ribosome
transfers the growing polypeptide chain from its 3’ end to the amino
acid attached to the 3’ end of the newly delivered tRNA, a reaction
catalysed by the ribosome. A large number of the individual nucleotides
in a tRNA molecule may be chemically modified, often by methylation or deamidation. These unusual bases sometimes affect the tRNA's interaction with ribosomes and sometimes occur in the anticodon to alter base-pairing properties.
Structure
The structure of tRNA can be decomposed into its primary structure, its secondary structure (usually visualized as the cloverleaf structure), and its tertiary structure (all tRNAs have a similar L-shaped 3D structure that allows them to fit into the P and A sites of the ribosome). The cloverleaf structure becomes the 3D L-shaped structure through coaxial stacking of the helices, which is a common RNA tertiary structure motif. The lengths of each arm, as well as the loop 'diameter', in a tRNA molecule vary from species to species.
The tRNA structure consists of the following:
The acceptor stem is a 7- to 9-base pair (bp) stem made
by the base pairing of the 5′-terminal nucleotide with the 3′-terminal
nucleotide (which contains the CCA 3′-terminal group used to attach the
amino acid). In general, such 3′-terminal tRNA-like structures are
referred to as 'genomic tags'. The acceptor stem may contain non-Watson-Crick base pairs.
The CCA tail is a cytosine-cytosine-adenine sequence at the 3′ end of the tRNA molecule. The amino acid loaded onto the tRNA by aminoacyl tRNA synthetases, to form aminoacyl-tRNA, is covalently bonded to the 3′-hydroxyl group on the CCA tail. This sequence is important for the recognition of tRNA by enzymes and critical in translation.
In prokaryotes, the CCA sequence is transcribed in some tRNA sequences.
In most prokaryotic tRNAs and eukaryotic tRNAs, the CCA sequence is
added during processing and therefore does not appear in the tRNA gene.
The D loop is a 4- to 6-bp stem ending in a loop that often contains dihydrouridine.
The anticodon loop is a 5-bp stem whose loop contains the anticodon.
The tRNA 5′-to-3′ primary structure contains the anticodon but in
reverse order, since 3′-to-5′ directionality is required to read the
mRNA from 5′-to-3′.
The ΨU loop is named so because of the characteristic presence of the unusual base ΨU in the loop, where Ψ is pseudouridine, a modified uridine. The modified base is often found within the sequence 5' -TΨUCG-3'.
The variable loop sits between the anticodon loop and the ΨU loop and, as its name implies, varies in size from 3 to 21 bases.
Anticodon
An anticodon is a unit of three nucleotides corresponding to the three bases of an mRNAcodon. Each tRNA has a distinct anticodon triplet sequence that can form 3 complementarybase pairs to one or more codons for an amino acid. Some anticodons pair with more than one codon due to wobble base pairing. Frequently, the first nucleotide of the anticodon is one not found on mRNA: inosine, which can hydrogen bond to more than one base in the corresponding codon position. In genetic code, it is common for a single amino acid to be specified by all four third-position possibilities, or at least by both pyrimidines and purines; for example, the amino acid glycine
is coded for by the codon sequences GGU, GGC, GGA, and GGG. Other
modified nucleotides may also appear at the first anticodon
position—sometimes known as the "wobble position"—resulting in subtle
changes to the genetic code, as for example in mitochondria.
Per cell, 61 tRNA types are required to provide one-to-one
correspondence between tRNA molecules and codons that specify amino
acids, as there are 61 sense codons of the standard genetic code.
However, many cells have under 61 types of tRNAs because the wobble base
is capable of binding to several, though not necessarily all, of the
codons that specify a particular amino acid. At least 31 tRNAs are
required to translate, unambiguously, all 61 sense codons.
Aminoacylation is the process of adding an aminoacyl group to a compound. It covalently links an amino acid to the CCA 3′ end of a tRNA molecule.
Each tRNA is aminoacylated (or charged) with a specific amino acid by an aminoacyl tRNA synthetase.
There is normally a single aminoacyl tRNA synthetase for each amino
acid, despite the fact that there can be more than one tRNA, and more
than one anticodon for an amino acid. Recognition of the appropriate
tRNA by the synthetases is not mediated solely by the anticodon, and the
acceptor stem often plays a prominent role.
Reaction:
Certain organisms can have one or more aminophosphate-tRNA
synthetases missing. This leads to charging of the tRNA by a chemically
related amino acid, and by use of an enzyme or enzymes, the tRNA is
modified to be correctly charged. For example, Helicobacter pylori has glutaminyl tRNA synthetase missing. Thus, glutamate tRNA synthetase charges tRNA-glutamine(tRNA-Gln) with glutamate.
An amidotransferase then converts the acid side chain of the glutamate
to the amide, forming the correctly charged gln-tRNA-Gln.
Interference with aminoacylation may be useful as an approach to
treating some diseases: cancerous cells may be relatively vulnerable to
disturbed aminoacylation compared to healthy cells. The protein
synthesis associated with cancer and viral biology is often very
dependent on specific tRNA molecules. For instance, for liver cancer
charging tRNA-Lys-CUU with lysine sustains liver cancer cell growth and
metastasis, whereas healthy cells have a much lower dependence on this
tRNA to support cellular physiology. Similarly, hepatitis E virus requires a tRNA landscape that substantially differs from that associated with uninfected cells.
Hence, inhibition of aminoacylation of specific tRNA species is
considered a promising novel avenue for the rational treatment of a
plethora of diseases.
Binding to ribosome
The ribosome has three binding sites for tRNA molecules that span the space between the two ribosomal subunits: the A (aminoacyl), P (peptidyl), and E (exit) sites. In addition, the ribosome has two other sites for tRNA binding that are used during mRNA decoding or during the initiation of protein synthesis. These are the T site (named elongation factor Tu) and I site (initiation). By convention, the tRNA binding sites are denoted with the site on the small ribosomal subunit listed first and the site on the large ribosomal subunit listed second. For example, the A site is often written A/A, the P site, P/P, and the E site, E/E.
The binding proteins like L27, L2, L14, L15, L16 at the A- and P- sites
have been determined by affinity labeling by A. P. Czernilofsky et al. (Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci, USA, pp. 230–234, 1974).
Once translation initiation is complete, the first aminoacyl tRNA
is located in the P/P site, ready for the elongation cycle described
below. During translation elongation, tRNA first binds to the ribosome
as part of a complex with elongation factor Tu (EF-Tu) or its eukaryotic (eEF-1)
or archaeal counterpart. This initial tRNA binding site is called the
A/T site. In the A/T site, the A-site half resides in the small ribosomal subunit where the mRNA decoding site is located. The mRNA decoding site is where the mRNAcodon is read out during translation. The T-site half resides mainly on the large ribosomal subunit
where EF-Tu or eEF-1 interacts with the ribosome. Once mRNA decoding is
complete, the aminoacyl-tRNA is bound in the A/A site and is ready for
the next peptide bond
to be formed to its attached amino acid. The peptidyl-tRNA, which
transfers the growing polypeptide to the aminoacyl-tRNA bound in the A/A
site, is bound in the P/P site. Once the peptide bond is formed, the
tRNA in the P/P site is acylated, or has a free 3’ end,
and the tRNA in the A/A site dissociates the growing polypeptide chain.
To allow for the next elongation cycle, the tRNAs then move through
hybrid A/P and P/E binding sites, before completing the cycle and
residing in the P/P and E/E sites. Once the A/A and P/P tRNAs have moved
to the P/P and E/E sites, the mRNA has also moved over by one codon and the A/T site is vacant, ready for the next round of mRNA decoding. The tRNA bound in the E/E site then leaves the ribosome.
The P/I site is actually the first to bind to aminoacyl tRNA, which is delivered by an initiation factor called IF2 in bacteria. However, the existence of the P/I site in eukaryotic or archaeal ribosomes
has not yet been confirmed. The P-site protein L27 has been determined
by affinity labeling by E. Collatz and A. P. Czernilofsky (FEBS Lett., Vol. 63, pp. 283–286, 1976).
tRNA genes
Organisms vary in the number of tRNA genes in their genome. For example, the nematode worm C. elegans, a commonly used model organism in genetics studies, has 29,647 genes in its nuclear genome, of which 620 code for tRNA. The budding yeast Saccharomyces cerevisiae
has 275 tRNA genes in its genome. The number of tRNA genes per genome
can vary widely, with bacterial species from groups such as Fusobacteria
and Tenericutes having around 30 genes per genome while complex
eukaryotic genomes such as the zebrafish (Danio rerio) can bear more than 10 thousand tRNA genes.
In the human genome, which, according to January 2013 estimates, has about 20,848 protein coding genes in total, there are 497 nuclear genes encoding cytoplasmic tRNA molecules, and 324 tRNA-derived pseudogenes—tRNA genes thought to be no longer functional (although pseudo tRNAs have been shown to be involved in antibiotic resistance in bacteria). As with all eukaryotes, there are 22 mitochondrial tRNA genes in humans. Mutations in some of these genes have been associated with severe diseases like the MELAS syndrome. Regions in nuclear chromosomes, very similar in sequence to mitochondrial tRNA genes, have also been identified (tRNA-lookalikes). These tRNA-lookalikes are also considered part of the nuclear mitochondrial DNA (genes transferred from the mitochondria to the nucleus).
The phenomenon of multiple nuclear copies of mitochondrial tRNA
(tRNA-lookalikes) has been observed in many higher organisms from human
to the opossum suggesting the possibility that the lookalikes are functional.
Cytoplasmic tRNA genes can be grouped into 49 families according
to their anticodon features. These genes are found on all chromosomes,
except the 22 and Y chromosome. High clustering on 6p is observed (140
tRNA genes), as well on 1 chromosome.
The HGNC, in collaboration with the Genomic tRNA Database (GtRNAdb) and experts in the field, has approved unique names for human genes that encode tRNAs.
Evolution
The
top half of tRNA (consisting of the T arm and the acceptor stem with
5′-terminal phosphate group and 3′-terminal CCA group) and the bottom
half (consisting of the D arm and the anticodon arm) are independent
units in structure as well as in function. The top half may have evolved
first including the 3′-terminal genomic tag which originally may have
marked tRNA-like molecules for replication in early RNA world.
The bottom half may have evolved later as an expansion, e.g. as protein
synthesis started in RNA world and turned it into a ribonucleoprotein
world (RNP world). This proposed scenario is called genomic tag hypothesis. In fact, tRNA and tRNA-like aggregates have an important catalytic influence (i.e., as ribozymes) on replication still today. These roles may be regarded as 'molecular (or chemical) fossils' of RNA world.
Genomic tRNA content is a differentiating feature of genomes
among biological domains of life: Archaea present the simplest situation
in terms of genomic tRNA content with a uniform number of gene copies,
Bacteria have an intermediate situation and Eukarya present the most
complex situation.
Eukarya present not only more tRNA gene content than the other two
kingdoms but also a high variation in gene copy number among different
isoacceptors, and this complexity seem to be due to duplications of tRNA
genes and changes in anticodon specificity.
Evolution of the tRNA gene copy number across different species
has been linked to the appearance of specific tRNA modification enzymes
(uridine methyltransferases in Bacteria, and adenosine deaminases in
Eukarya), which increase the decoding capacity of a given tRNA.
As an example, tRNAAla encodes four different tRNA isoacceptors (AGC,
UGC, GGC and CGC). In Eukarya, AGC isoacceptors are extremely enriched
in gene copy number in comparison to the rest of isoacceptors, and this
has been correlated with its A-to-I modification of its wobble base.
This same trend has been shown for most amino acids of eukaryal species.
Indeed, the effect of these two tRNA modifications is also seen in codon usage bias.
Highly expressed genes seem to be enriched in codons that are
exclusively using codons that will be decoded by these modified tRNAs,
which suggests a possible role of these codons—and consequently of these
tRNA modifications—in translation efficiency.
It is important to note that many species have lost specific
tRNAs during evolution. For instance, both mammals and birds lack the
same 14 out of the possible 64 tRNA genes, but other life forms contain
these tRNAs. For translating codons for which an exactly pairing tRNA is missing, organisms resort to a strategy called wobbling,
in which imperfectly matched tRNA/mRNA pairs still give rise to
translation, although this strategy also increases to propensity for
translation errors.
The reasons why tRNA genes have been lost during evolution remains
under debate but may relate improving resistance to viral infection.
Because nucleotide triplets can present more combinations than there
are amino acids and associated tRNAs, there is redundancy in the genetic
code, and several different 3-nucleotide codons can express the same
amino acid. This codon bias is what necessitates codon optimization.
tRNA-derived fragments
tRNA-derived fragments (or tRFs) are short molecules that emerge after cleavage of the mature tRNAs or the precursor transcript. Both cytoplasmic and mitochondrial tRNAs can produce fragments.
There are at least four structural types of tRFs believed to originate
from mature tRNAs, including the relatively long tRNA halves and short
5’-tRFs, 3’-tRFs and i-tRFs.
The precursor tRNA can be cleaved to produce molecules from the 5’
leader or 3’ trail sequences. Cleavage enzymes include Angiogenin,
Dicer, RNase Z and RNase P.
Especially in the case of Angiogenin, the tRFs have a
characteristically unusual cyclic phosphate at their 3’ end and a
hydroxyl group at the 5’ end. tRFs appear to play a role in RNA interference,
specifically in the suppression of retroviruses and retrotransposons
that use tRNA as a primer for replication. Half-tRNAs cleaved by angiogenin are also known as tiRNAs. The biogenesis of smaller fragments, including those that function as piRNAs, are less understood.
tRFs have multiple dependencies and roles; such as exhibiting significant changes between sexes, among races and disease status. Functionally, they can be loaded on Ago and act through RNAi pathways,participate in the formation of stress granules, displace mRNAs from RNA-binding proteins or inhibit translation.
At the system or the organismal level, the four types of tRFs have a
diverse spectrum of activities. Functionally, tRFs are associated with
viral infection, cancer, cell proliferation and also with epigenetic transgenerational regulation of metabolism.
tRFs are not restricted to humans and have been shown to exist in multiple organisms.
Two online tools are available for those wishing to learn more about tRFs: the framework for the interactive exploration of mitochondrial and nuclear tRNA fragments (MINTbase) and the relational database of Transfer RNA related Fragments (tRFdb). MINTbase also provides a naming scheme for the naming of tRFs called tRF-license plates (or MINTcodes) that is genome independent; the scheme compresses an RNA sequence into a shorter string.
Engineered tRNAs
Artificial suppressor elongator tRNAs are used to incorporate unnatural amino acids at nonsense codons placed in the coding sequence of a gene. Engineered initiator tRNAs (tRNAfMet2 with CUA anticodon encoded by metY gene) have been used to initiate translation at the amber stop codon UAG. This type of engineered tRNA is called a nonsense suppressor
tRNA because it suppresses the translation stop signal that normally
occurs at UAG codons. The amber initiator tRNA inserts methionine and glutamine at UAG codons preceded by a strong Shine-Dalgarno sequence.
An investigation of the amber initiator tRNA showed that it was
orthogonal to the regular AUG start codon showing no detectable
off-target translation initiation events in a genomically recoded E. coli strain.
tRNA biogenesis
In eukaryotic cells, tRNAs are transcribed by RNA polymerase III as pre-tRNAs in the nucleus.
RNA polymerase III recognizes two highly conserved downstream promoter
sequences: the 5′ intragenic control region (5′-ICR, D-control region,
or A box), and the 3′-ICR (T-control region or B box) inside tRNA genes.
The first promoter begins at +8 of mature tRNAs and the second promoter
is located 30–60 nucleotides downstream of the first promoter. The
transcription terminates after a stretch of four or more thymidines.
Pre-tRNAs undergo extensive modifications inside the nucleus. Some pre-tRNAs contain introns that are spliced, or cut, to form the functional tRNA molecule; in bacteria these self-splice, whereas in eukaryotes and archaea they are removed by tRNA-splicing endonucleases.
Eukaryotic pre-tRNA contains bulge-helix-bulge (BHB) structure motif
that is important for recognition and precise splicing of tRNA intron by
endonucleases.
This motif position and structure are evolutionarily conserved.
However, some organisms, such as unicellular algae have a non-canonical
position of BHB-motif as well as 5′- and 3′-ends of the spliced intron
sequence.
The 5′ sequence is removed by RNase P, whereas the 3′ end is removed by the tRNase Z enzyme.
A notable exception is in the archaeonNanoarchaeum equitans,
which does not possess an RNase P enzyme and has a promoter placed such
that transcription starts at the 5′ end of the mature tRNA.
The non-templated 3′ CCA tail is added by a nucleotidyl transferase.
Before tRNAs are exported into the cytoplasm by Los1/Xpo-t, tRNAs are aminoacylated.
The order of the processing events is not conserved.
For example, in yeast, the splicing is not carried out in the nucleus but at the cytoplasmic side of mitochondrial membranes.
Nonetheless, In March 2021, researchers reported evidence
suggesting that a preliminary form of transfer RNA could have been a
replicator molecule in the very early development of life, or abiogenesis.
History
The existence of tRNA was first hypothesized by Francis Crick as the "adaptor hypothesis"
based on the assumption that there must exist an adapter molecule
capable of mediating the translation of the RNA alphabet into the
protein alphabet. Paul C Zamecnik and Mahlon Hoagland discovered tRNA Significant research on structure was conducted in the early 1960s by Alex Rich and Donald Caspar, two researchers in Boston, the Jacques Fresco group in Princeton University and a United Kingdom group at King's College London. In 1965, Robert W. Holley of Cornell University reported the primary structure and suggested three secondary structures. tRNA was first crystallized in Madison, Wisconsin, by Robert M. Bock. The cloverleaf structure was ascertained by several other studies in the following years and was finally confirmed using X-ray crystallography studies in 1974. Two independent groups, Kim Sung-Hou working under Alexander Rich and a British group headed by Aaron Klug, published the same crystallography findings within a year.
In chemistry, a lone pair refers to a pair of valence electrons that are not shared with another atom in a covalent bond and is sometimes called an unshared pair or non-bonding pair. Lone pairs are found in the outermost electron shell of atoms. They can be identified by using a Lewis structure. Electron pairs are therefore considered lone pairs if two electrons are paired but are not used in chemical bonding. Thus, the number of electrons in lone pairs plus the number of electrons in bonds equals the number of valence electrons around an atom.
Lone pair is a concept used in valence shell electron pair repulsion theory (VSEPR theory) which explains the shapes of molecules. They are also referred to in the chemistry of Lewis acids and bases.
However, not all non-bonding pairs of electrons are considered by
chemists to be lone pairs. Examples are the transition metals where the
non-bonding pairs do not influence molecular geometry and are said to be
stereochemically inactive. In molecular orbital theory (fully delocalized canonical orbitals
or localized in some form), the concept of a lone pair is less
distinct, as the correspondence between an orbital and components of a
Lewis structure is often not straightforward. Nevertheless, occupied non-bonding orbitals (or orbitals of mostly nonbonding character) are frequently identified as lone pairs.
A single lone pair can be found with atoms in the nitrogen group, such as nitrogen in ammonia. Two lone pairs can be found with atoms in the chalcogen group, such as oxygen in water. The halogens can carry three lone pairs, such as in hydrogen chloride.
In VSEPR theory
the electron pairs on the oxygen atom in water form the vertices of a
tetrahedron with the lone pairs on two of the four vertices. The H–O–H bond angle is 104.5°, less than the 109° predicted for a tetrahedral angle, and this can be explained by a repulsive interaction between the lone pairs.
Various computational criteria for the presence of lone pairs have been proposed. While electron density ρ(r) itself generally does not provide useful guidance in this regard, the Laplacian of the electron density is revealing, and one criterion for the location of the lone pair is where L(r) = –∇2ρ(r) is a local maximum. The minima of the electrostatic potential V(r) is another proposed criterion. Yet another considers the electron localization function (ELF).
Angle changes
The pairs often exhibit a negative polar character with their high charge density and are located closer to the atomic nucleus
on average compared to the bonding pair of electrons. The presence of a
lone pair decreases the bond angle between the bonding pair of
electrons, due to their high electric charge, which causes great
repulsion between the electrons. They are also involved in the formation
of a dative bond. For example, the creation of the hydronium (H3O+) ion occurs when acids are dissolved in water and is due to the oxygen atom donating a lone pair to the hydrogen ion.
This can be seen more clearly when looked at it in two more common molecules. For example, in carbon dioxide (CO2), the oxygen atoms are on opposite sides of the carbon atom (linear molecular geometry), whereas in water (H2O) the angle between the hydrogen atoms is 104.5° (bent molecular geometry).
The repulsive force of the oxygen atom's two lone pairs pushes the
hydrogen atoms further apart, until the forces of all electrons on the
hydrogen atom are in equilibrium. This is an illustration of the VSEPR theory.
Dipole moments
Lone pairs can contribute to a molecule's dipole moment. NH3 has a dipole moment of 1.42 D. As the electronegativity
of nitrogen (3.04) is greater than that of hydrogen (2.2) the result is
that the N-H bonds are polar with a net negative charge on the nitrogen
atom and a smaller net positive charge on the hydrogen atoms. There is
also a dipole associated with the lone pair and this reinforces the
contribution made by the polar covalent N-H bonds to ammonia's dipole moment. In contrast to NH3, NF3 has a much lower dipole moment of 0.234 D. Fluorine is more electronegative than nitrogen and the polarity
of the N-F bonds is opposite to that of the N-H bonds in ammonia, so
that the dipole due to the lone pair opposes the N-F bond dipoles,
resulting in a low molecular dipole moment.
Stereogenic lone pairs
⇌
Inversion of a generic organic amine molecule at nitrogen
A lone pair can contribute to the existence of chirality in a
molecule, when three other groups attached to an atom all differ. The
effect is seen in certain amines, phosphines, sulfonium and oxonium ions, sulfoxides, and even carbanions.
The resolution of enantiomers where the stereogenic center is an amine is usually precluded because the energy barrier for nitrogen inversion
at the stereo center is low, which allow the two stereoisomers to
rapidly interconvert at room temperature. As a result, such chiral
amines cannot be resolved, unless the amine's groups are constrained in a
cyclic structure (such as in Tröger's base).
Unusual lone pairs
A stereochemically active lone pair is also expected for divalent lead and tin ions due to their formal electronic configuration of ns2. In the solid state this results in the distorted metal coordination observed in the tetragonallitharge structure adopted by both PbO and SnO.
The formation of these heavy metal ns2 lone pairs which was previously attributed to intra-atomic hybridization of the metal s and p states has recently been shown to have a strong anion dependence.
This dependence on the electronic states of the anion can explain why
some divalent lead and tin materials such as PbS and SnTe show no
stereochemical evidence of the lone pair and adopt the symmetric
rocksalt crystal structure.
In molecular systems the lone pair can also result in a
distortion in the coordination of ligands around the metal ion. The
lone-pair effect of lead can be observed in supramolecular complexes of lead(II) nitrate, and in 2007 a study linked the lone pair to lead poisoning. Lead ions can replace the native metal ions in several key enzymes, such as zinc cations in the ALAD enzyme, which is also known as porphobilinogen synthase, and is important in the synthesis of heme, a key component of the oxygen-carrying molecule hemoglobin. This inhibition of heme synthesis appears to be the molecular basis of lead poisoning (also called "saturnism" or "plumbism").
Computational experiments reveal that although the coordination number
does not change upon substitution in calcium-binding proteins, the
introduction of lead distorts the way the ligands organize themselves to
accommodate such an emerging lone pair: consequently, these proteins
are perturbed. This lone-pair effect becomes dramatic for zinc-binding
proteins, such as the above-mentioned porphobilinogen synthase, as the
natural substrate cannot bind anymore – in those cases the protein is inhibited.
In Group 14 elements (the carbon group), lone pairs can manifest themselves by shortening or lengthening single bond (bond order 1) lengths, as well as in the effective order of triple bonds as well. The familiar alkynes have a carbon-carbon triple bond (bond order 3) and a linear geometry of 180° bond angles (figure A in reference). However, further down in the group (silicon, germanium, and tin), formal triple bonds have an effective bond order 2 with one lone pair (figure B) and trans-bent geometries. In lead, the effective bond order is reduced even further to a single bond, with two lone pairs for each lead atom (figure C). In the organogermanium compound (Scheme 1 in the reference), the effective bond order is also 1, with complexation of the acidicisonitrile (or isocyanide) C-N groups, based on interaction with germanium's empty 4p orbital.
In elementary chemistry courses, the lone pairs of water are
described as "rabbit ears": two equivalent electron pairs of
approximately sp3 hybridization, while the HOH bond angle is
104.5°, slightly smaller than the ideal tetrahedral angle of
arccos(–1/3) ≈ 109.47°. The smaller bond angle is rationalized by VSEPR theory
by ascribing a larger space requirement for the two identical lone
pairs compared to the two bonding pairs. In more advanced courses, an
alternative explanation for this phenomenon considers the greater
stability of orbitals with excess s character using the theory of isovalent hybridization, in which bonds and lone pairs can be constructed with spx hybrids wherein nonintegral values of x
are allowed, so long as the total amount of s and p character is
conserved (one s and three p orbitals in the case of second-row p-block
elements).
To determine the hybridization of oxygen orbitals used to form
the bonding pairs and lone pairs of water in this picture, we use the
formula 1 + x cos θ = 0, which relates bond angle θ with the hybridization index x. According to this formula, the O–H bonds are considered to be constructed from O bonding orbitals of ~sp4.0 hybridization (~80% p character, ~20% s character), which leaves behind O lone pairs orbitals of ~sp2.3 hybridization (~70% p character, ~30% s character). These deviations from idealized sp3 hybridization (75% p character, 25% s character) for tetrahedral geometry are consistent with Bent's rule:
lone pairs localize more electron density closer to the central atom
compared to bonding pairs; hence, the use of orbitals with excess s
character to form lone pairs (and, consequently, those with excess p
character to form bonding pairs) is energetically favorable.
However, theoreticians often prefer an alternative description of
water that separates the lone pairs of water according to symmetry with
respect to the molecular plane. In this model, there are two
energetically and geometrically distinct lone pairs of water possessing
different symmetry: one (σ) in-plane and symmetric with respect to the
molecular plane and the other (π) perpendicular and anti-symmetric with
respect to the molecular plane. The σ-symmetry lone pair (σ(out)) is
formed from a hybrid orbital that mixes 2s and 2p character, while the
π-symmetry lone pair (p) is of exclusive 2p orbital parentage. The s
character rich O σ(out) lone pair orbital (also notated nO(σ)) is an ~sp0.7 hybrid (~40% p character, 60% s character), while the p lone pair orbital (also notated nO(π)) consists of 100% p character.
Both models are of value and represent the same total electron density, with the orbitals related by a unitary transformation. In this case, we can construct the two equivalent lone pair hybrid orbitals h and h' by taking linear combinations h = c1σ(out) + c2p and h' = c1σ(out) – c2p for an appropriate choice of coefficients c1 and c2. For chemical and physical properties of water that depend on the overall electron distribution of the molecule, the use of h and h'
is just as valid as the use of σ(out) and p. In some cases, such a view
is intuitively useful. For example, the stereoelectronic requirement
for the anomeric effect can be rationalized using equivalent lone pairs, since it is the overall
donation of electron density into the antibonding orbital that matters.
An alternative treatment using σ/π separated lone pairs is also valid,
but it requires striking a balance between maximizing nO(π)-σ* overlap (maximum at 90° dihedral angle) and nO(σ)-σ* overlap (maximum at 0° dihedral angle), a compromise that leads to the conclusion that a gauche
conformation (60° dihedral angle) is most favorable, the same
conclusion that the equivalent lone pairs model rationalizes in a much
more straightforward manner. Similarly, the hydrogen bonds
of water form along the directions of the "rabbit ears" lone pairs, as a
reflection of the increased availability of electrons in these regions.
This view is supported computationally.
However, because only the symmetry-adapted canonical orbitals have
physically meaningful energies, phenomena that have to do with the
energies of individual orbitals, such as photochemical reactivity or photoelectron spectroscopy, are most readily explained using σ and π lone pairs that respect the molecular symmetry.
Because of the popularity of VSEPR theory,
the treatment of the water lone pairs as equivalent is prevalent in
introductory chemistry courses, and many practicing chemists continue to
regard it as a useful model. A similar situation arises when describing
the two lone pairs on the carbonyl oxygen atom of a ketone.
However, the question of whether it is conceptually useful to derive
equivalent orbitals from symmetry-adapted ones, from the standpoint of
bonding theory and pedagogy, is still a controversial one, with recent
(2014 and 2015) articles opposing and supporting the practice.
In social and developmentalpsychology, an individual's implicit theory of intelligence
refers to his or her fundamental underlying beliefs regarding whether
or not intelligence or abilities can change, developed by Carol Dweck and colleagues.
History
Ellen
Leggett developed implicit theories of intelligence in 1985. Her paper
"Children's entity and incremental theories of intelligence:
Relationships to achievement behavior" was presented at the 1985 meeting
of the Eastern Psychological Association in Boston.
As a result, Dweck and her collaborators began studying how individuals
unknowingly (or implicitly) assess their own intelligence and abilities
through interaction and interpretation of their environment. It was
assumed that these assessments ultimately influenced the individual's
goals, motivations, behaviors, and self-esteem. The researchers began by
looking at students who were highly motivated to achieve, and students
who were not, though the levels of self-achievement were not clarified.
They noticed that the highly motivated students thrived in the face of
challenge while the other students quit or withdrew from their work, but
critically, a student's raw intelligence did not predict whether a
student was highly motivated or not.
Rather, they discovered that these two groups of students held
different beliefs (or implicit theories) about intelligence, categorized
as entity or incremental theories, which affected their classroom
performance.
Entity theory vs. incremental theory
Carol
Dweck identified two different mindsets regarding intelligence beliefs.
The entity theory of intelligence refers to an individual's belief that
abilities are fixed traits.
For entity theorists, if perceived ability to perform a task is high,
the perceived possibility for mastery is also high. In turn, if
perceived ability is low, there is little perceived possibility of
mastery, often regarded as an outlook of "learned helplessness" (Park
& Kim, 2015). However, the incremental theory of intelligence
proposes that intelligence and ability are malleable traits which can be
improved upon through effort and hard work. For incremental theorists,
there is a perceived possibility of mastery even when initial ability to
perform a task is low. Those who subscribe to this theory of
intelligence "don't necessarily believe that anyone can become an Einstein or Mozart, but they do understand even Einstein and Mozart had to put in years of effort to become who they were".
This possibility of mastery contributes in part to intrinsic motivation
of individuals to perform a task, since there is perceived potential
for success in the task.
Individuals may fall on some spectrum between the two types of
theories, and their views may change given different situations and
training.
By observing an individual's motivation and behavior towards
achievement, an individual's general mindset regarding intelligence is
revealed.
About 40% of the general population believe the entity theory, 40%
believe the incremental theory, and 20% do not fit well into either
category.
Performance level on a task is not always predetermined by an
individual's mindset. Previous research on the subject has shown that
when faced with failure on an initial task, those with an entity theory
mindset will perform worse on subsequent tasks that measure the same
ability than those with an incremental theory mindset (Park & Kim,
2015). However, a 2015 research study published in the Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin found that when the subsequent task measured a
different ability, entity theorists performed better than incremental
theorists. In certain situations, the incremental theorists studied were
self-critical about the previous failure; these thoughts disrupted
their performance on the subsequent task. Incremental theorists'
reactions to failure are traditionally seen as an "adaptive response",
meaning they link the failure to insufficient effort and therefore
search for ways to improve their performance. If there is no opportunity
for improvement on the task, such as in the research study, thoughts of
doubt about the failure affect future performance (Park & Kim,
2015).
For the individuals who believed in an entity theory of
intelligence, there were no such feelings of doubt when performing the
second task because they perceived the task as not measuring the ability
that they lacked on the initial task. After the first failure,
self-critical thoughts are less likely to linger in their minds while
performing the second task; the study points out that "entity theorists
will not necessarily feel helpless because the second task does not
measure the ability they think they lack" (Park & Kim 2015).
Therefore, in this study, entity theorists performed better on the
subsequent task than did incremental theorists if the measured ability
was different.
Motivation toward achievement
Different types of goals
An
individual's motivation towards achievement is shaped by their implicit
theory of intelligence (and their related implicit theories about
domain-specific aptitudes) and its associated goals. J.G. Nicholls
proposed two different types of goals related to achievement. Task involvement goals involve individuals aiming to improve their own abilities. Ego involvement goals involve individuals wanting to better themselves compared to others. Dweck modified Nicholls' ideas by proposing performance goals and mastery goals.
Performance goals are associated with entity theory and lead
individuals to perform actions in order to appear capable and avoid
negative judgments about their skills. Mastery goals are associated with
incremental theory and lead individuals to engage and work in order to
gain expertise in new things.
Response to challenge
Individuals
who believe they have the ability to grow and add to their knowledge
gladly accept challenges that can aid in growth towards mastery.
Individuals who believe their abilities are fixed will also accept and
persist through challenges as long as they feel they will succeed and
their abilities will not be questioned. However, when these individuals
lack confidence in their abilities, they will avoid, procrastinate, or
possibly cheat in challenging situations that might make them appear
incompetent. These behaviors can lead to a sense of learned helplessness
and stymied intellectual growth.
Attribution of failure
Attribution
of failure and coping with that failure are highly related to mindset.
Individuals who subscribe to an incremental view will attribute a
failure to not yet having learned something, looking at something from
the incorrect perspective, or not working hard enough. All of these
problems can be corrected through effort, leading incrementalist
individuals to continually seek any situation that will intellectually
better themselves. Also they are more likely to engage in remedial
action to correct mistakes if necessary. Those with fixed intelligence
views attribute failure to their own lack of ability.
Self-regulated learning
Individuals
with an incremental mindset will take feedback and channel that into
determination to try new strategies for solving a given problem, a large
part of self-regulated learning (or learning to effectively guide your own studies).
As a result, incrementalist individuals are more effective at
self-regulated learning, ultimately leading them to be more productive
at developing plans for learning and making connections between topics
which promotes deeper processing of information.
Self-esteem
Incrementalist individuals generally have positive and stable self-esteem and do not question their intelligence in the face of failure, instead remaining eager and curious.
Individuals with entity beliefs mostly attribute failure or having to
exert effort to a lack of ability. Therefore, if they do not succeed at
some task, they are unlikely to seek similar tasks or will quit trying.
They believe that putting in effort will undermine their competence
because if they were smart enough to begin with, they would not need to
put in effort.
These individuals will limit themselves to situations where they
believe they will succeed and may limit themselves in the face of
negative feedback, which they will likely interpret as a personal attack
on their ability. These individuals' self-esteem as well as their
enjoyment of a task may suffer when they encounter failure and the
associated feelings of helplessness.
Many children who see failure as a reflection of their intelligence
will even lie about their scores to strangers to preserve their
self-esteem and competence, since they connect their judgments of self
to their performance. Students who see the value of effort do not show
such a tendency. The majority of what are considered "best students" are often concerned with failure.
Students who achieve a great deal of academic success early on might be
most likely to believe their intelligence is fixed because they so
frequently have been praised regarding their intelligence. They may have
faced fewer opportunities for setbacks and do not have much experience
persisting through errors.
Longitudinal research shows that individuals who endorse entity beliefs
experience decreasing self-esteem throughout their college years, while
individuals who endorse incremental beliefs experience an increase.
Development
Implicit theories of intelligence develop at an early age and are subtly influenced by parents and educators and the type of praise
they give for successful work. Typically it has been assumed that any
sort of praise will have a positive impact on a child's self-confidence
and achievement. However, different types of praise can lead to the
development of different views on intelligence. Young children who hear
praise that values high intelligence as a measure of success, such as
"You must be smart at these problems," may link failure with a lack of
intelligence and are more susceptible to developing an entity mindset.
Often children are given high praise for their intelligence after
relatively easy success, which sets them up to develop counterproductive
behaviors in dealing with academic setbacks, rather than fostering
confidence and the enjoyment of learning. Praise for intelligence
connects performance with ability, rather than effort, leading these
individuals to develop "performance" goals to prove competence. However
students who receive praise valuing hard work as a measure of success,
such as "You must have worked hard at these problems," more often pursue
mastery goals that underlie an incremental mindset.
Subtle differences in speech to children that promote non-generic
praise (i.e. "You did a good job drawing") versus generic praise (i.e.
"You are a good drawer"), lead children to respond to later criticism in
a way that demonstrates an incremental mindset.
Shifting from entity to incremental mindset to improve achievement
Understanding
differences between those who believe in entity theory versus
incremental theory allows educators to predict how students will
persevere in a classroom. Then, educators can change behaviors that may
contribute to academic shortcomings for those with entity tendencies and
low confidence in their abilities. While these implicit beliefs
regarding where intelligence comes from are relatively stable across
time and permeate all aspects of behavior, it is possible to change peoples' perspectives on their abilities for a given task with the right priming. Dweck's 2006 book Mindset: How You Can Fill Your Potential focuses on teaching individuals how they can encourage thinking with a growth mindset for a happier and more successful existence.
Elementary-aged students
Given
the opportunity for fifth graders to choose their own learning tasks,
when primed towards gaining skills, they will pick challenging tasks.
When they are primed towards assessment, they will pick tasks that they
think they will be successful at to show off their abilities. Thus, they
will forgo new learning if it means the possibility of making mistakes. If the situation is framed in a manner that emphasizes learning and process rather than success, mindset can be altered.
Middle school-aged children
Transitioning
between elementary school and middle school is a time when many
students with an entity theory of intelligence begin to experience their
first taste of academic difficulty. Transitioning students with low
abilities can be oriented to a growth mentality when taught that their
brains are like muscles that get stronger through hard work and effort.
This lesson can result in a marked improvement in grades compared to
students with similar abilities and resources available to them who do
not receive this information on the brain.
College-aged students
One consequence for individuals experiencing the stereotype threat
(or worrying about conforming to a negative stereotype associated with a
member of one's group) is that they will also experience an entity
mindset. College students are able to overcome this negative impact
after participating in an incremental thinking intervention, afterwards
reporting higher levels of happiness according to the theory.
Predictive power of knowing an individual's theory
Success in school and on tests
An
individual's implicit theory of intelligence can predict future
success, particularly navigating life transitions that are often
associated with challenging situations, such as moving from elementary
to middle school. Students followed throughout their middle school
careers showed that those who possessed growth mindset tendencies made
better grades and had a more positive view on the role of effort than
students who possessed fixed mindset tendencies with similar abilities,
two years following the initial survey.
Those with theoretical entity beliefs worry more about tests even in
situations where they have experienced some success, spend less time
practicing before tests, and thus have shown reduced performance on IQ
tests relative to others in their environment. If the situation is framed in a manner that emphasizes learning and process rather than success, mindset can be altered.
Individuals with fixed mindsets may engage in less practice in order to
allow themselves an excuse besides low ability for potentially poor
performance in order to preserve their egos.
Students who have learning goals (associated with incremental beliefs)
are more internally motivated and successful in the face of a
challenging college course. After the first test in a course, those who
possess learning goals are likely to improve their grades on the next
test whereas those with performance goals did not.
Negotiation skills
Incrementalist
individuals tend to have stronger negotiating skills, believing that
with effort a better deal can be reached. This finding may have
implications for more favorable working conditions for those with
incrementalist beliefs.
"Easily learned = easily remembered" heuristic
According
to theory, individuals who believe their intelligence can grow think
about information in their world differently even outside of academic
challenges, seen by use of a different heuristic when making judgments of learning (JOLs),
or estimates of learning. Those with entity views are generally guided
by the principle "easily learned" means "easily remembered," which means
that when learning information, these individuals will make low JOLs
when a task is difficult. Those with incremental views did not follow
this "easily learned" means "easily remembered" principle and gave
higher judgments to more difficult tasks, perhaps believing that if more
effort is put into the learning because it is harder, those items will
be better remembered.
Other behaviors governed by implicit theories
Research into implicit theories of intelligence has led to additional discoveries expanding the idea of entity vs incremental
mindset to other areas outside of just intelligence. Views about
intelligence are just a single manifestation of a more general entity or
incremental mindset which reveals a great deal about a person's view of
the world and self. Generally those with entity views will see all
characteristics, in addition to intelligence, as innate and static while
those with incremental views see characteristics as malleable. Entity
beliefs lead to more stereotyping, greater rigidity in prejudiced
beliefs, and difficulties during conflict resolution. Incremental views
are connected to more open beliefs and amenability during conflict
resolutions.
In intimate relationships, those who possess an incremental mindset
tend to believe that people can change and exhibit more forgiveness than
those with entity mindsets. Similarly, those with entity beliefs are
likely to endorse the fundamental attribution error
more than those with incremental views, who tend to focus much more on
the situation than internal characteristics of an individual.