If means time, then is frequency in cycles per unit time, but in the abstract, they can be any dual pair of variables (e.g. position and spatial frequency).
The sine transform is necessarily an odd function of frequency, i.e. for all :
The Fourier cosine transform of is:
Fourier cosine transform
The cosine transform is necessarily an even function of frequency, i.e. for all :
Odd and even simplification
The multiplication rules for even and odd functions shown in the overbraces in the following equations dramatically simplify the integrands when transforming even and odd functions. Some authors even only define the cosine transform for even functions . Since cosine is an even function and because the integral of an even function from to is twice its integral from to , the cosine transform of any even function can be simplified to avoid negative :
And because the integral from to of any odd function from is zero, the cosine transform of any odd function is simply zero:
Similarly, because sin is odd, the sine transform of any odd function also simplifies to avoid negative :
and the sine transform of any even function is simply zero:
The sine transform represents the odd part of a function, while the cosine transform represents the even part of a function.
Other conventions
Just like the Fourier transform takes the form of different equations with different constant factors (see Fourier transform § Unitarity and definition for square integrable functions for discussion), other authors also define the cosine transform as
and the sine transform as
Another convention defines the cosine transform as and the sine transform as using as the transformation variable. And while is typically used to represent the time domain, is often instead used to represent a spatial domain when transforming to spatial frequencies.
Fourier inversion
The original function can be recovered from its sine and cosine transforms under the usual hypotheses using the inversion formula:
Fourier inversion (from the sine and cosine transforms)
Simplifications
Note that since both integrands are even functions of , the concept of negative frequency can be avoided by doubling the result of integrating over non-negative frequencies:
Also, if is an odd function, then the cosine transform is zero, so its inversion simplifies to:
Likewise, if the original function is an even function, then the sine transform is zero, so its inversion also simplifies to:
Remarkably, these last two simplified inversion formulas look
identical to the original sine and cosine transforms, respectively,
though with swapped with (and with swapped with or ).
A consequence of this symmetry is that their inversion and transform
processes still work when the two functions are swapped. Two such
functions are called transform pairs.
Overview of inversion proof
Using the addition formula for cosine, the full inversion formula can also be rewritten as Fourier's integral formula:
This theorem is often stated under different hypotheses, that is integrable, and is of bounded variation on an open interval containing the point , in which case
This latter form is a useful intermediate step in proving the
inverse formulae for the since and cosine transforms. One method of
deriving it, due to Cauchy is to insert a into the integral, where is fixed. Then
Now when , the integrand tends to zero except at , so that formally the above is
Relation with complex exponentials
The complex exponential form of the Fourier transform used more often today is
where is the square root of negative one. By applying Euler's formula
it can be shown (for real-valued functions) that the Fourier
transform's real component is the cosine transform (representing the
even component of the original function) and the Fourier transform's
imaginary component is the negative of the sine transform (representing
the odd component of the original function):Because of this relationship, the cosine transform of functions whose Fourier transform is known (e.g. in Fourier transform § Tables of important Fourier transforms) can be simply found by taking the real part of the Fourier transform:while the sine transform is simply the negative of the imaginary part of the Fourier transform:
Pros and cons
An advantage of the modern Fourier transform is that while the sine and cosine transforms together are required to extract the phase information of a frequency, the modern Fourier transform instead compactly packs both phase and
amplitude information inside its complex valued result. But a
disadvantage is its requirement on understanding complex numbers,
complex exponentials, and negative frequency.
The sine and cosine transforms meanwhile have the advantage that
all quantities are real. Since positive frequencies can fully express
them, the non-trivial concept of negative frequency
needed in the regular Fourier transform can be avoided. They may also
be convenient when the original function is already even or odd or can
be made even or odd, in which case only the cosine or the sine transform
respectively is needed. For instance, even though an input may not be
even or odd, a discrete cosine transform may start by assuming an even extension of its input while a discrete sine transform may start by assuming an odd extension of its input, to avoid having to compute the entire discrete Fourier transform.
Numerical evaluation
Using
standard methods of numerical evaluation for Fourier integrals, such as
Gaussian or tanh-sinh quadrature, is likely to lead to completely
incorrect results, as the quadrature sum is (for most integrands of
interest) highly ill-conditioned.
Special numerical methods which exploit the structure of the oscillation
are required, an example of which is Ooura's method for Fourier
integrals. This method attempts to evaluate the integrand at locations which
asymptotically approach the zeros of the oscillation (either the sine or
cosine), quickly reducing the magnitude of positive and negative terms
which are summed.
Salience (also called saliency, from Latin saliō meaning “leap, spring”) is the property by which some thing stands out. Salient events are an attentional mechanism by which organisms learn and survive; those organisms can focus their limited perceptual and cognitive resources on the pertinent (that is, salient) subset of the sensorydata available to them.
Saliency typically arises from contrasts between items and their
neighborhood. They might be represented, for example, by a red dot
surrounded by white dots, or by a flickering message indicator of an
answering machine, or a loud noise in an otherwise quiet environment.
Saliency detection is often studied in the context of the visual
system, but similar mechanisms operate in other sensory systems. Just
what is salient can be influenced by training: for example, for human
subjects particular letters can become salient by training.
There can be a sequence of necessary events, each of which has to be
salient, in turn, in order for successful training in the sequence; the
alternative is a failure, as in an illustrated sequence when tying a bowline; in the list of illustrations, even the first illustration is a salient: the rope in the list must cross over, and not under
the bitter end of the rope (which can remain fixed, and not free to
move); failure to notice that the first salient has not been satisfied
means the knot will fail to hold, even when the remaining salient events
have been satisfied.
When attention deployment is driven by salient stimuli, it is considered to be bottom-up, memory-free,
and reactive. Conversely, attention can also be guided by top-down,
memory-dependent, or anticipatory mechanisms, such as when looking ahead
of moving objects or sideways before crossing streets. Humans and other
animals have difficulty paying attention to more than one item
simultaneously, so they are faced with the challenge of continuously
integrating and prioritizing different bottom-up and top-down
influences.
Neuroanatomy
The brain component named the hippocampus
helps with the assessment of salience and context by using past
memories to filter new incoming stimuli, and placing those that are most
important into long term memory. The entorhinal
cortex is the pathway into and out of the hippocampus, and is an
important part of the brain's memory network; research shows that it is a
brain region that suffers damage early on in Alzheimer's disease, one of the effects of which is altered (diminished) salience.
The pulvinar nuclei (in the thalamus) modulate physical/perceptual salience in attentional selection.
The primary visual cortex (V1) generates a bottom-up saliency map from visual inputs to guide reflexive attentional shifts or gaze shifts. According to V1 Saliency Hypothesis,
the saliency of a location is higher when V1 neurons give higher
responses to that location relative to V1 neurons' responses to other
visual locations.
For example, a unique red item among green items, or a unique vertical
bar among horizontal bars, is salient since it evokes higher V1
responses and attracts attention or gaze. The V1 neural responses are sent to the superior colliculus
to guide gaze shifts to the salient locations. A fingerprint of the
saliency map in V1 is that attention or gaze can be captured by the
location of an eye-of-origin singleton in visual inputs, e.g., a bar
uniquely shown to the left eye in a background of many other bars shown
to the right eye, even when observers cannot tell the difference between
the singleton and the background bars.
In psychology
The
term is widely used in the study of perception and cognition to refer
to any aspect of a stimulus that, for any of many reasons, stands out
from the rest. Salience may be the result of emotional, motivational or
cognitive factors and is not necessarily associated with physical
factors such as intensity, clarity or size. Although salience is thought
to determine attentional selection, salience associated with physical
factors does not necessarily influence selection of a stimulus.
Salience bias
Salience bias (also referred to as perceptual salience) is a cognitive bias that predisposes individuals to focus on or attend to items, information, or stimuli that are more prominent, visible,
or emotionally striking. This is as opposed to stimuli that are
unremarkable, or less salient, even though this difference is often
irrelevant by objective standards. The American Psychological Association
(APA) defines the salience hypothesis as a theory regarding perception
where “motivationally significant” information is more readily perceived
than information with little or less significant motivational
importance.
Perceptual salience (salience bias) is linked to the vividness effect,
whereby a more pronounced response is produced by a more vivid
perception of a stimulus than the mere knowledge of the stimulus.
Salience bias assumes that more dynamic, conspicuous, or distinctive
stimuli engage attention more than less prominent stimuli,
disproportionately impacting decision making, it is a bias which favors more salient information.
Application
Cognitive Psychology
Salience bias, like all other cognitive biases, is an applicable concept to various disciplines. For example, cognitive psychology investigates cognitive functions and processes, such as perception, attention, memory,
problem solving, and decision making, all of which could be influenced
by salience bias. Salience bias acts to combat cognitive overload by
focusing attention on prominent stimuli, which affects how individuals
perceive the world as other, less vivid stimuli that could add to or
change this perception, are ignored. Human attention gravitates towards
novel and relevant stimuli and unconsciously filters out less prominent
information, demonstrating salience bias, which influences behavior as
human behavior is affected by what is attended to.
Behavioral economists Tversky and Kahneman also suggest that the
retrieval of instances is influenced by their salience, such as how
witnessing or experiencing an event first-hand has a greater impact than
when it is less salient, like if it were read about, implying that memory is affected by salience.
Language
It
is also relevant in language understanding and acquisition. Focusing on
more salient phenomena allows people to detect language patterns and
dialect variations more easily, making dialect categorization more
efficient.
Social Behavior
Furthermore,
social behaviors and interactions can also be influenced by perceptual
salience. Changes in the perceptual salience of an individual heavily
influences their social behavior and subjective experience of their
social interactions, confirming a “social salience effect”.Social salience relates to how individuals perceive and respond to other people.
Behavioral Science
The connection between salience bias and other heuristics, like availability and representativeness, links it to the fields of behavioral science and behavioral economics.
Salience bias is closely related to the availability heuristic in
behavioral economics, based on the influence of information vividness
and visibility, such as recency or frequency, on judgements, for example:
Accessibility
and salience are closely related to availability, and they are
important as well. If you have personally experienced a serious
earthquake, you’re more likely to believe that an earthquake is likely
than if you read about it in a weekly magazine. Thus, vivid and easily
imagined causes of death (for example, tornadoes) often receive inflated
estimates of probability, and less-vivid causes (for example, asthma
attacks) receive low estimates, even if they occur with a far greater
frequency (here, by a factor of twenty). Timing counts too: more recent
events have a greater impact on our behavior, and on our fears, than
earlier ones.
— Richard H. Thaler, Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness (2008-04-08)
Humans have bounded rationality,
which refers to their limited ability to be rational in decision
making, due to a limited capacity to process information and cognitive
ability. Heuristics, such as availability, are employed to reduce the
complexity of cognitive and social tasks or judgements, in order to decrease the cognitive load
that result from bounded rationality. Despite the effectiveness of
heuristics in doing so, they are limited by systematic errors
that occur, often the result of influencing biases, such as salience.
This can lead to misdirected or misinformed judgements, based on an
overemphasis or overweighting of certain, more salient information. For
example, the irrational behavior of procrastination
occurs because costs in the present, like sacrificing free time, are
disproportionately salient to future costs, because at that time they
are more vivid.
The more prominent information is more readily available than the less
salient information, and thus has a larger impact on decision making and
behavior, resulting in errors in judgement.
Other fields such as philosophy, economics, finance, and
political science have also investigated the effects of salience, such
as in relation to taxes,
where salience bias is applied to real-world behaviors, affecting
systems like the economy. The existence of salience bias in humans can
make behavior more predictable and this bias can be leveraged to
influence behavior, such as through nudges.
Evaluation
Salience
bias is one of many explanations for why humans deviate from rational
decision making: by being overly focused on or biased to the most
visible data and ignoring other potentially important information that
could result in a more reasonable judgment. As a concept it is supported
in psychological and economic literature, through its relationship with
the availability heuristic outlined by Tversky and Kahneman, and its applicability to behaviors relevant to multiple disciplines, such as economics.
Despite this support, salience bias is limited for various
reasons, one example being its difficulty in quantifying,
operationalizing, and universally defining.
Salience is often confused with other terms in literature, for example,
one article states that salience, which is defined as a cognitive bias
referring to “visibility and prominence”, is often confused with terms
like transparency and complexity in public finance literature.
This limits salience bias as the confusion negates its importance as an
individual term, and therefore the influence it has on tax related
behavior. Likewise, the APA definition of salience refers to
motivational importance,
which is based on subjective judgement, adding to the difficulty.
According to psychologist S. Taylor “some people are more salient than
others” and these differences can further bias judgements.
Biased judgements have far-reaching consequences, beyond poor decision making, such as overgeneralizing and stereotyping.
Studies into solo status or token integration demonstrate this. The
token is an individual in a group different to the other members in that
social environment, like a female in an all-male workplace. The token
is viewed as symbolic of their social group, whereby judgments made
about the solo individual predict judgements of their social group,
which can result in inaccurate perceptions of that group and potential
stereotyping. The distinctiveness of the individual in that environment
“fosters a salience bias” and hence predisposes those generalized judgements, positive or negative.
In interaction design
Salience
in design draws from the cognitive aspects of attention, and applies it
to the making of 2D and 3D objects. When designing computer and screen
interfaces, salience helps draw attention to certain objects like
buttons and signify affordance, so designers can utilize this aspect of perception to guide users.
There are several variables used to direct attention:
Color. Hue, saturation, and value can all be used to call attention to areas or objects within an interface, and de-emphasize others.
Size. Object size and proportion to surrounding elements creates visual hierarchy, both in interactive elements like buttons, but also within informative elements like text.
Position. An object's orientation or spatial arrangement in relation to the surrounding objects creates differentiation to invite action.
Accessibility
A
consideration for salience in interaction design is accessibility. Many
interfaces used today rely on visual salience for guiding user
interaction, and people with disabilities like color-blindness may have
trouble interacting with interfaces using color or contrast to create
salience.
Kapur (2003) proposed that a hyperdopaminergic state,
at a "brain" level of description, leads to an aberrant assignment of
salience to the elements of one's experience, at a "mind" level. These aberrant salience attributions have been associated with altered activities in the mesolimbic system, including the striatum, the amygdala, the hippocampus, the parahippocampal gyrus., the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula.
Dopamine mediates the conversion of the neural representation of an
external stimulus from a neutral bit of information into an attractive
or aversive entity, i.e. a salient event. Symptoms of schizophrenia
may arise out of 'the aberrant assignment of salience to external
objects and internal representations', and antipsychotic medications
reduce positive symptoms by attenuating aberrant motivational salience
via blockade of the dopamine D2 receptors (Kapur, 2003).
Alternative areas of investigation include supplementary motor areas, frontal eye fields
and parietal eye fields. These areas of the brain are involved with
calculating predictions and visual salience. Changing expectations on
where to look restructures these areas of the brain. This cognitive
repatterning can result in some of the symptoms found in such disorders.
Visual saliency modeling
In the domain of psychology,
efforts have been made in modeling the mechanism of human attention,
including the learning of prioritizing the different bottom-up and
top-down influences.
In the domain of computer vision, efforts have been made in modeling the mechanism of human attention, especially the bottom-up attentional mechanism, including both spatial and temporal attention. Such a process is also called visual saliency detection.
Generally speaking, there are two kinds of models to mimic the
bottom-up saliency mechanism. One way is based on the spatial contrast
analysis: for example, a center-surround mechanism is used to define
saliency across scales, which is inspired by the putative neural
mechanism. The other way is based on the frequency domain analysis.
While they used the amplitude spectrum to assign saliency to rarely
occurring magnitudes, Guo et al. use the phase spectrum instead.
Recently, Li et al. introduced a system that uses both the amplitude and the phase information.
A key limitation in many such approaches is their computational
complexity leading to less than real-time performance, even on modern
computer hardware. Some recent work attempts to overcome these issues at the expense of saliency detection quality under some conditions.
Other work suggests that saliency and associated speed-accuracy
phenomena may be a fundamental mechanisms determined during recognition
through gradient descent, needing not be spatial in nature.