Prosocial behavior, or intent to benefit others, is a social behavior that "benefit[s] other people or society as a whole", "such as helping, sharing, donating, co-operating, and volunteering".
Obeying the rules and conforming to socially accepted behaviors (such
as stopping at a "Stop" sign or paying for groceries) are also regarded
as prosocial behaviors. These actions may be motivated by empathy and by concern about the welfare and rights of others, as well as for egoistic or practical concerns, such as one's social status or reputation, hope for direct or indirect reciprocity, or adherence to one's perceived system of fairness. It may also be motivated by altruism,
though the existence of pure altruism is somewhat disputed, and some
have argued that this falls into philosophical rather than psychological
realm of debate.
Evidence suggests that pro sociality is central to the well-being of
social groups across a range of scales, including schools. Prosocial
behavior in the classroom can have a significant impact on a student's motivation for learning and contributions to the classroom and larger community. Empathy is a strong motive in eliciting prosocial behavior, and has deep evolutionary roots.
Prosocial behavior fosters positive traits that are beneficial for children and society. It helps many beneficial functions by bettering production of any league and its organizational scale. Evolutionary psychologists use theories such as kin-selection theory and inclusive fitness as an explanation for why prosocial behavioral tendencies are passed down generationally, according to the evolutionary fitness displayed by those who engaged in prosocial acts. Encouraging prosocial behavior may also require decreasing or eliminating undesirable social behaviors.
Although the term "prosocial behavior" is often associated with developing desirable traits in children, the literature on the topic has grown since the late 1980s to include adult behaviors as well.
Prosocial behavior fosters positive traits that are beneficial for children and society. It helps many beneficial functions by bettering production of any league and its organizational scale. Evolutionary psychologists use theories such as kin-selection theory and inclusive fitness as an explanation for why prosocial behavioral tendencies are passed down generationally, according to the evolutionary fitness displayed by those who engaged in prosocial acts. Encouraging prosocial behavior may also require decreasing or eliminating undesirable social behaviors.
Although the term "prosocial behavior" is often associated with developing desirable traits in children, the literature on the topic has grown since the late 1980s to include adult behaviors as well.
Origin of the term
According to the psychology researcher C. Daniel Batson, the term "was created by social scientists as an antonym for antisocial."
Reciprocity vs. altruism in motivation
The purest forms of prosocial behavior are motivated by altruism, an unselfish interest in helping another person. According to Santrock,
the circumstances most likely to evoke altruism are empathy for an
individual in need, or a close relationship between the benefactor and
the recipient. However, many prosocial behaviors that appear altruistic
are in fact motivated by the norm of reciprocity, which is the
obligation to return a favor with a favor. People feel guilty
when they do not reciprocate and they may feel angry when someone else
does not reciprocate. Reciprocal altruism suggests that "such helping is
driven by a genetic tendency". Thus some professionals argue that altruism may not exist, and is completely motivated by reciprocity. Either reciprocity or altruism may motivate many important prosocial behaviors, including sharing.
Situational and individual factors
Prosocial behavior is mediated by both situational and individual factors.
Situational factors
One of the most common situation factors is the occurrence of the bystander effect.
The bystander effect is the phenomenon that an individual's likelihood
of helping decreases when passive bystanders are present in a critical
situation. For example, when someone drops a stack of papers on a
crowded sidewalk, most people are likely to continue passing him/her by.
This example can be extended to even more urgent situations, such as a
car crash or natural disaster.
The decision model of bystander intervention noted that whether
or not an individual gives aid in a situation depends upon their
analysis of the situation. An individual will consider whether or not
the situation requires their assistance, if the assistance is the
responsibility of the individual, and how to help.
This model, proposed by Latane and Darley, describes five things that must occur in order for a person to intervene:
- Notice the situation
- Construe it as an emergency.
- Develop feelings of responsibility.
- Believe they have skills to succeed.
- Reach a conscious decision to help.
The number of individuals present in the situation requiring help is
also a mediating factor in one's decision to give aid, where the more
individuals are present, the less likely it is for one particular
individual to give aid due to a reduction in perceived personal
responsibility.
This is known as diffusion of responsibility, where the responsibility
one feels for the person(s) in need is divided by the number of
bystanders. Another factor that comes into play is evaluation
apprehension, which simply refers to the fear of being judged by other
bystanders. Finally, pluralistic ignorance may also lead to someone not
intervening. This refers to relying on the reaction of others, before
reacting yourself.
Additionally, Piliavin et al., (1981) noted that individuals are
likely to maximize their rewards and minimize their costs when
determining whether or not to give aid in a situation – that is, that
people are rationally self-motivated. Prosocial behavior is more likely
to occur if the cost of helping is low (i.e. minimal time, or minimal
effort), if helping would actually benefit the individual providing the
help in some way, and if the rewards of providing the help are large. If
it is in an individual's interest to help, they will most likely do so,
especially if the cost of not providing the help is great.
People are also more likely to help those in their social group,
or their "in group". With a sense of shared identity with the individual
requiring assistance, the altruist is more likely to provide help, on
the basis that one allocates more time and energy towards helping
behavior within individuals of their own group. The labeling of another
individual as a member of one's "in-group" leads to greater feelings of
closeness, emotional arousal, and a heightened sense of personal
responsibility for the other's welfare, all of which increase the
motivation to act prosocially.
Researchers have also found that social exclusion
decreases the likelihood of prosocial behavior occurring. In a series
of seven experiments conducted by Twenge et al., (2007) researchers
manipulated social inclusion or exclusion by telling research
participants that other participants had purposefully excluded them, or
that they would probably end up alone later in life. They found that
this preliminary social exclusion caused prosocial behavior to drop
significantly, noting that "Socially excluded people donated less money
to a student fund, were unwilling to volunteer for further lab
experiments, were less helpful after a mishap, and cooperated less in a
mixed-motive game with another student."
This effect is thought to be due to the fact that prosocial behavior,
again, is motivated by a sense of responsibility in caring for and
sharing resources with members of one's own group.
Individual factors
Individuals
can be compelled to act prosocially based on learning and socialization
during childhood. Operant conditioning and social learning positively
reinforces discrete instances of prosocial behaviors. Cognitive
capacities like intelligence for example, are almost always related to
prosocial likings.
Helping skills and a habitual motivation to help others is therefore
socialized, and reinforced as children understand why helping skills
should be used to help others around them.
Social and individual standards and ideals also motivate
individuals to engage in prosocial behavior. Social responsibility
norms, and social reciprocity norms reinforce those who act prosocially.
As an example, consider the child who is positively reinforced for
"sharing" during their early childhood years.
When acting prosocially, individuals reinforce and maintain their
positive self-images or personal ideals, as well as help to fulfill
their own personal needs.
The correlation between a helper's state and helping tendencies are
greatly restricted to the relationship between whomever takes part in
the situation.
Emotional arousal is an additional important motivator for
prosocial behavior in general. Batson's (1987) empathy-altruism model
examines the emotional and motivational component of prosocial behavior.
Feeling empathy towards the individual needing aid increases the
likelihood that the aid will be given. This empathy is called
"empathetic concern" for the other individual, and is characterized by
feelings of tenderness, compassion, and sympathy.
Agreeableness is thought to be the personality trait most
associated with inherent prosocial motivation. Prosocial thoughts and
feelings may be defined as a sense of responsibility for other
individuals, and a higher likelihood of experiencing empathy
("other-oriented empathy") both affectively (emotionally) and
cognitively. These prosocial thoughts and feelings correlate with
dispositional empathy and dispositional agreeableness.
Other factors
In
addition to situational and individualistic factors, there are some
categorical characteristics that can impact prosocial behavior. Several
studies have indicated a positive relationship between prosocial
behavior and religion. In addition, there may be sex differences in prosocial behavior, particularly as youths move into adolescence.
Research suggests that while women and men both engage in prosocial
behaviors, women tend to engage in more communal and relational
prosocial behaviors whereas men tend to engage in more agentic prosocial
behaviors.
A recent study examining workplace charitable giving looked at the role
of both sex and ethnicity. Results showed that women gave significantly
more than men, and Caucasians gave significantly more than minority
groups. However, the percent of minority individuals in the workplace
was positively associated with workplace charitable giving by
minorities.
Culture, sex, and religion are important factors to consider in
understanding prosocial behavior on an individual and group level.
In childhood through early adolescence
Prosocial
behavior in childhood often begins with questions of sharing and
fairness. From age 12–18 months, children begin to display prosocial
behavior in presenting and giving their toys to their parents, without
promoting or being reinforced by praise.
The development of prosocial behavior continues throughout the second
year of life, as children begin to gain a moral understanding of the
world.
As obedience to societal standards becomes important, children's
ability to exhibit prosocial behavior strengthens, with occurrence and
diversity of these behaviors increasing with age and cognitive maturity.
What is important developmentally is that the child has developed a
belief that sharing is an obligatory part of a social relationship and
involves a question of right and wrong.
So, as children move through childhood, their reasoning changes from
being hedonistic and needs-oriented to becoming more concerned with
approval and more involved in complex cognitive forms of perspective
taking and reciprocity reasoning.
Additionally, children's prosocial behavior is typically more centered
around interest in friends and concern for approval, whereas
adolescents begin to develop reasoning that is more concerned with
abstract principles such as guilt and positive affect.
Parents can set examples that children carry into their
interactions and communication with peers, but parents are not present
during all of their children's peer exchanges. The day-to-day
constructions of fairness standards is done by children in collaboration
and negotiation with each other.
Recent research demonstrates that invoking the self using subtle
linguistic cues (e.g. identifying someone as a "helper" versus labeling
the action, "helping") fosters the perception that a behavior reflects
identity, and increases helping, or prosocial, behaviors in children
significantly across tasks.
Another study by Nantel-Vivier et al. used a multi-informant
model to investigate the development of prosocial behaviour in both
Canadian and Italian adolescents aged 10–15.
Their findings have indicated that, in early adolescence, although
empathy and moral reasoning continue to advance, the development of
prosocial behaviors reaches a plateau. Theories for this change in
development suggest that it is the result of more individualized and
selective prosocial behaviors. During adolescence, youth begin to focus
these behaviors toward their peer groups and/or affiliations.
Consistent with previous analyses, this study also found a
tendency toward higher prosocial behaviors in young adolescent girls
compared to their male classmates. The earlier maturation in females
may be a possible explanation for this disparity. A more recent study
that focused on the effects of pubertal timing found that early
maturation in adolescents has a positive impact on prosocial behaviors.
While their findings apply to both genders, this study found a much more
pronounced effect in males. This suggests that earlier onset of
puberty has a positive correlation with the development of prosocial
behaviors.
In many Indigenous American communities, prosocial behavior is a valued means of learning and child rearing. Such behaviors are seen as contributing in an eagerly collaborative and flexible environment, aimed at teaching consideration, responsibility, and skills with the guidance and support of adults. Culturally valued developmental goals are integrally tied to children's participation in these contexts.
It is also helpful for children to learn cultural mores in addition to
individual personality development. Children learn functional life
skills through real-time observation of adults and interactive
participation of these learned skills within their community.
Prosocial development in school
Prosocial
behavior can act as a strong motivator in education, for it provides
students with a purpose beyond themselves and the classroom. This
purpose beyond the self, or self-transcendence,
is an innate human need to be a part of something bigger than
themselves. When learning in isolation, the way Western academics are
traditionally designed, students struggle to make connections to the
material and its greater overarching purpose. This disconnection harms
student learning, motivation, and attitudes about education.
If teachers make space for prosocial behavior in education and social learning,
then they can illustrate that what students are learning will have a
direct impact on the world that they live in. This would be considered a
mutually constituting relationship,
or a relationship in which both individuals and culture develop
interdependently. In other words, what students are learning in a
classroom could be intimately connected with a purpose towards a greater
cause, deepening the learning itself.
Studies by Yeager et al.
test the effects of having a self-transcendent purpose for learning,
with the results showing that such a purpose for learning led to fewer
future college dropouts, increased high school math and science GPAs,
and persistence on boring tasks. This self transcendent purpose may not
only encourage persistence on boring tasks, but may help to make boring
tasks more meaningful and engaging.
A person's ideas and opinions are largely shaped by the world
that they grow up in, which in turn determines what sort of change they
want to instill in the world. For example: a girl who grew up in
poverty becoming a social worker. The environment she grew up in gave
her an awareness of the workings of poverty, motivating her to instill
change in either the institutions that cause it, or help those affected
by poverty.
There aren't many opportunities to make prosocial contributions
in school; which makes school feel isolated and irrelevant. By
encouraging students to find a self-transcendent purpose in their
learning, we enable them to enjoy their learning and make connections to
community contributions.
Influence of media programming and video games on children
Studies have shown that different types of media programming may evoke prosocial behaviors in children.
The channels aimed at younger viewers like Nickelodeon and Disney Channel had significantly more acts of altruism than the general-audience demographic channels like A&E and or TNT,
according to one large-scale study. This study examined the programming
of 18 different channels, including more than 2,000 entertainment
shows, during a randomly selected week on television. The study revealed
that nearly three quarters (73 percent) of programs contained at least
one act of altruism
and on average viewers saw around three acts of altruism an hour.
Around one-third of those behaviors were explicitly rewarded in the
plot, potentially sending the message that these acts of prosocial
behavior can come with positive consequences.
Another study on the topic was conducted by University at Buffalo, Iowa State University and University of Minnesota
professors. They studied children for two years for the purpose of
investigating the role of media exposure on prosocial behavior for young
boys and girls. The study concluded that media exposure could possibly
predict outcomes related to prosocial behavior.
Other experimental research has suggested that prosocial video games may increase prosocial behavior in players although some of this work has proven difficult to replicate.
However other scholars have been critical of this work for tending to
falsely dichotomize video games into prosocial/violent categories
despite significant overlap as well as methodological flaws in the
experimental studies.
For instance a study by Ferguson and Garza found that exposure to
violent video games was associated with increased prosocial behavior,
both on-line as well as volunteering in the real world. The authors
speculated this may be due to the prosocial themes common in many
violent games, as well as team oriented play in many games.
Legislation
In the United States, in an effort to get stations to air education and prosocial programming for children, the Children's Television Act
was adopted in 1990. It states that channels must produce and air
programming developed specifically for children as a condition to renew
broadcast licenses. After discussions as to what the definition of
"specifically designed for children" really means, in 1996 guidelines
were passed to correct this ambiguity.
Influence of observation
People
are generally much more likely to act pro-socially in a public setting
rather than in a private setting. One explanation for this finding has
to do with perceived status, being publicly recognized as a pro-social
individual often enhance one's self-image and desirability to be
considered for inclusion in social groups.
Other research has shown that merely given people the "illusion" that
they are being observed (e.g., by hanging up posters of "staring" human
eyes) can generate significant changes in pro-social acts such as
charitable giving and less littering. Pictures of human eyes trigger an
involuntary neural gaze detection mechanism, which primes people to act
pro-socially.
There are two different forms of prosocial behaviors. Ordinary
prosocial behavior requires, "situational and sociocultural demands."
Extraordinary prosocial behavior doesn't include as much. This indicates
that one form is used for a more selfish result while the other is not.
Influence of perception of responsibility and guilt
Guilt has long been regarded as a motivator for prosocial behavior. Extensive data from a 2012 study conducted by de Hooge,
demonstrates that when a secondary individual repairs a transgressors’
damage caused to victims, the transgressors’ guilt feelings, reparative
intentions, and prosocial behavior drastically diminish. Thus, reduction
of guilt may have more to do with reparative actions broadly, rather
than necessarily prosocial behaviors taken on by oneself.
Social media in natural disasters
Social media can also be a catalyst for prosocial behavior. One example occurred during the relief efforts in the wake of the 2011 Tōhoku earthquake and tsunami off the coast of Japan, when users turned to Facebook and Twitter to provide financial and emotional support via their social networks. Direct donations to Japanese relief were possible on The Red Cross fan page on Facebook, and via online discount sites like Groupon and LivingSocial.
Relation to mood and emotion
Mood
and prosocial behavior are closely linked. People often experience the
"feel good-do good" phenomena, where being in a good mood increases
helping behaviors. Being in a good mood helps us to see the "good" in
other people, and prolongs our own good mood. For example, mood and work
behaviors have frequently been examined in research; studies show that
positive mood at work is associated with more positive work-related
behaviors (e.g., helping co-workers). Similarly, prosocial behaviors increase positive mood. Several studies have shown the benefits of volunteering and other prosocial behaviors on self-esteem, life satisfaction, and overall mental health.
Additionally, negative mood can also impact prosocial behavior. Research
has shown that guilt often leads to prosocial behaviors, whereas other
negative mood states, such as fear, do not lead to the same prosocial
behaviors.
A recent pilot study examined whether an intervention increasing
prosocial behavior (kind acts) in young adults with social anxiety would
both increase positive affect and decrease social anxiety in
participants. Participants randomly assigned to a 4-week Kind Acts
intervention, where individuals were instructed to engage in three kind
acts each day twice a week over the 4 week period, showed both higher
self-reported positive mood and increased satisfaction with
relationships at the end of the intervention. This intervention
demonstrates ways in which prosocial behavior may be beneficial in
improving mood and mental health.
Other research suggests that cultivating positive emotions, such as gratitude,
may also facilitate prosocial behavior. A study by Bartlett &
DeSteno examined the ability of gratitude to shape costly prosocial
behavior, demonstrating that gratitude increases efforts to assist a
benefactor even when such efforts are costly (i.e., hedonically
negative), and that this increase is qualitatively different from
efforts given from just general positive affective state. They also show
that gratitude can increase assistance provided to strangers, not just
close social ties. Awe is another positive emotional state that has been closely linked to inspiring generosity and prosocial behavior. Piff et al.
studied this phenomenon through experiments using economic and
ethical-decision making games and explain, "When people experience awe
they really want to share that experience with other people, suggesting
that it has this particularly viral component to it... awe binds people
together—by causing people to want to share their positive experiences
collectively with one another."
Psychopathy and lack of prosocial behavior
In 1941, Hervey Cleckley described psychopathy as a disorder in which individuals often initially appear intelligent, charming, and even kind but are in fact egocentric, grandiose
and impulsive. He described individuals who would, on a whim, leave
their families to cross the country gambling, drinking and fighting,
only to return and act as if nothing was out of the ordinary.
Today, psychopathy is described as a personality disorder that is characterized by decreased anxiety, fear, and social closeness as well as increased impulsivity, manipulativeness, interpersonal dominance and aggression. These traits lead to numerous types of antisocial behavior including high rates of substance abuse, serial short term relationships, and various forms of criminal behavior.
One common misconception about psychopathy though is that all
psychopaths are serial killers or other vicious criminals. In reality,
many researchers do not consider criminal behavior to be a criterion for
the disorder although the role of criminality in the disorder is
strongly debated.
Additionally, psychopathy is being researched as a dimensional
construct that is one extreme of normal range personality traits instead
of a categorical disorder.
In regards to the lack of prosocial behavior in psychopathy,
there are several theories that have been proposed in the literature.
One theory suggests that psychopaths engage in less prosocial behavior
(and conversely more antisocial behavior) because of a deficit in their
ability to recognize fear in others, particularly fearful facial
expressions.
Because they are unable to recognize that their actions are causing
another distress, they continue that behavior in order to obtain some
goal that benefits them. A second theory proposes that psychopaths have a
sense of "altruistic punishment"
where they are willing to punish other individuals even if it means
they will be harmed in some way. There has also been an evolutionary
theory proposed
stating that psychopaths lack of prosocial behavior is an adaptive
mating strategy in that it allows them to spread more of their genes
while taking less responsibility for their offspring. Finally, there is
some evidence that in some situations psychopaths behavior may not be
antisocial but instead it may be more utilitarian
than other individuals. In a recent study, Bartels & Pizarro (2011)
found that when making decisions about traditional moral dilemmas such
as the trolley problem,
individuals high in psychopathic traits actually make more utilitarian
(and therefore more moral in some views) choices. This finding is
particularly interesting because it suggests that psychopaths, who are
often considered immoral or even evil, may actually make better moral
decisions than non-psychopaths. The authors of this study conclude that
individuals high in psychopathic traits are less influenced by their
emotions and therefore make more "mathematical" decisions and choose the
option that leads to the lowest number of deaths.
The theories discussed above are not intended to be a
comprehensive list but instead to provide a sense of how psychopaths
differ in their approach to social interactions. As with most
psychological/social phenomena, it is likely a combination of these
factors that lead to psychopaths lack of prosocial behavior. Further
research is needed to determine the causal nature of any one of these
individual deficits as well as if there is any way to help these
individuals develop more prosocial patterns of behavior.
Helper's high
Psychologists have shown that helping others can produce "feel-good" neurotransmitters such as oxytocin
and that, similar to any other pleasurable activity, the act of
volunteering, giving and behaving pro-socially can become addictive.
Some work has been done on utilizing this principle through the concept of helper therapy,
in which therapeutic benefits are gleaned from assisting others.
Community health workers have been found to gain helper benefits that
include positive feelings about self, a sense of belonging, valuable
work experience, and access to health information and skills through
their prosocial vocation, which may buffer against the various stressors
inherent in this line of work.
In addition, Helper therapy may also be highly beneficial for
distressed adolescents experiencing suicidal thoughts. Studies indicate
that when help-seeking youth use online community forums, the
help-seekers often begin to provide support for other help seekers, and
develop a reciprocal prosocial community battling depression together.
Prosocial dissidence
Stefano
Passini and Davide Morselli argue that groups will obey authority so
long as its system, basis, and demands are viewed as legitimate. Passini
and Morselli distinguish between anti-social disobedience, which they
see as destructive, and prosocial disobedience, which they see as
constructive. "Disobedience becomes prosocial when it is enacted for the
sake of the whole society, including all its different levels and
groups. In contrast, anti-social disobedience is enacted mainly in
favour of one's own group, in order to attain individual rights." A main
difference between anti-social and pro-social dissidence is the way
that they relate to authority; anti-social dissidents reject authority
and disobey its norms and laws, while pro-social dissidents understand
the important roles that societal laws play in maintaining order, but
also recognize and address the flaws in authoritative reasoning.
Pro-social protests, if viewed in a positive manner, can increase
freedoms and equality for the general public, and improve democratic
institutions.
Labor market outcomes
Recent
scientific research shows that individuals who volunteer have better
labor market outcomes in terms of hiring opportunities and wages.