A Medley of Potpourri is just what it says; various thoughts, opinions, ruminations, and contemplations on a variety of subjects.
Search This Blog
Tuesday, July 31, 2018
Memory implantation
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Memory implantation is a technique used in cognitive psychology to investigate human memory. In memory implantation studies researchers make people believe that they remember an event that actually never happened. The false memories
that have been successfully implanted in people’s memories include
remembering being lost in a mall as a child, taking a hot air balloon
ride, and putting slime in a teacher’s desk in primary school.
Memory implantation techniques were developed in the 1990s as a way of
providing evidence of how easy it is to distort people's memories of
past events. Most of the studies on memory implantation were published
in the context of the debate about repressed memories and the possible danger of digging for lost memories in therapy. The successful implantation of memories in people’s minds has implications for therapy and legal settings.
Successful memory implantation
Published studies
The
first formal studies using memory implantation were published in the
early 1990s, the most famous being "The Formation of False Memories"
(commonly referred to as the "Lost in the Mall" study) by Loftus and Pickrell.[1]
The basic technique used in this study involved asking family members
of a participant to provide narratives of events that happened when they
were young and then add another event that definitely had not happened.
The participants saw these four narratives and were told to try to
remember as much as possible about each event. Across a number of
studies using memory implantation, about 37% of people have come to
remember parts of or entire events that never actually happened.[4]
Other studies have expanded on this paradigm by introducing
photos instead of narratives. Wade and colleagues found that 50% of
people came to remember details of a hot air balloon ride that never
happened, after seeing a manipulated photo depicting the event.[5]
Later it has been argued that photos by themselves do not produce more
false memories than narratives, but that both methods have the power to
successfully implant false memories.[6]
Real photos have also been found to increase the creation of false
memories. In a study by Lindsay and colleagues people were shown a
childhood photo from the same time period as the false event. Seeing the
photo resulted in more false memories, even when the photos did not
depict the actual event.[7]
In a study with children 1999 Pezdek and Hodge found that it was
easier to implant a memory of a plausible event (being lost in a mall)
than an implausible one (receiving a rectal enema).[8]
Later follow up studies, however, show that the perceived plausibility
of a false event can be changed, making the false event easier to
implant.[9][10] Taken together, these findings show that there are many factors that are important for the way people remember events.
Mazzoni et al. also suggest a 3 processes model for the development of false memories through suggestions.[10]
The first process is to make people perceive the event as plausible,
the second is to make people believe it is likely to have happened to
them and the third step is to help people interpret thoughts and
fantasies about the event as memories. Other factors influencing the
likelihood of producing false memories include imagining the events and making a source-monitoring error, specifically reality monitoring.[11]
Legal case
A real life example of memory implantation occurred during the criminal case against Paul Ingram.
Ingram was accused by his daughters of recurring sexual abuse in their
childhood. Ingram denied all allegations at first but after being
interviewed by police and therapists he came to remember multiple
instances of abuse. Psychologist Richard Ofshe
considered this confession a result of suggestive questioning and
decided to test his theory. He told Ingram about a made-up scenario and
said it was another accusation made by his children. Ofshe asked Ingram
to try and remember as much as possible about this new event. Ingram
could not recall anything straight away but after thinking about it for
some time came up with a written confession where he described in detail
what had happened. His children confirmed to Ofshe that the event had
never actually happened, Ingram had created an entirely false memory
of an event after suggestions from Ofshe. Richard Ofshe considered this
successful memory implantation evidence of Paul Ingram’s suggestibility
and in his opinion it questions the accuracy of Ingram's other
confessions.[12]
Whether or not example is generalizable to other legal cases involving
recovered memories of sexual abuse it does show how important it is to
have corroborative evidence even in cases where the suspect confesses.
Implications
The methods used in memory implantation studies are meant to mimic those used by some therapists to recoverrepressed memories of childhood events .[4]
The high rate of people "remembering" false events shows that memories
cannot always be taken at face value. Being told to go home and look at
old photos to jog your memory can help you remember real events, but
paired with suggestions from a therapist it might also lead to false
memories. Memory implantation studies are also similar to recovered
memory therapy in the way that they involve an authoritative figure
claiming to know that the event actually happened and applying pressure
on the participant/patient to remember.[4]
Memory implantation techniques in general also illustrate how people
can relatively easily come to remember things that actually never
happened. This poses a big problem for criminal confessions resulting
from suggestive questioning by police and others and also for the
accuracy associated with eyewitness memory. It has been argued that memory implantation studies are not applicable to real life memories of trauma such as childhood sexual abuse.[13]
As it is not ethical to try to implant false memories of sexual abuse
researchers have tried to get around this by choosing other events that
are seen as negative but not traumatic. Being lost in a shopping mall
for example would be a negative experience for most children.[1]
Hyman and colleagues used memory implantation techniques with emotional
events such as a specific birthday party (positive) and being
hospitalized overnight(negative). They found that using emotional events
did not change the rate of false memory creation significantly compared
with other studies.[14]
Research with children
In 1998 Herrmann and Yoder published an article arguing for the cessation of memory implantation research with children.[15] The criticisms referred to several studies investigating the suggestibility of children written by Ceci and colleagues.[16][17]
Herrmann and Yoer argue that the methods used can have negative
implications for the children used such as lessen their respect for
authority, be damaging for their concept of self (feel incompetent when
it is pointed out that their memories are wrong) and cause stress.[15]
This article created much debate and several commentaries to the
article were published in the same June edition of the journal "Applied
Cognitive Psychology" together with the original article. One of these
was written by Ceci, Bruck and Loftus who disagree with the statements
in Herrmann and Yoder's article. According to these authors there is no
evidence that any children have been harmed in a memory implantation
study and until such evidence exists there is no reason to stop using
these techniques with children as long as it produces good research.[18]
Ornstein and Gordon also replied to Herrmann and Yoder's article saying
that although people conducting research with children have an ethical
responsibility there is much to be gained from memory implantation
research and the benefits outweigh the potential risk for children
involved.[19]
Another commentary written by Goodman, Quas and Redlich argues that
there is reason to believe that children in general enjoy participating
in false memory studies and that the benefits of these studies for
research into eyewitness memory are many. They also refer to several
cases where memory implantation studies have been cited in court and
contributed to convictions being overturned.[20] Thompson and Jackson propose a modified version of the suggestions from
Herrmann and Yoder and say that methods for being more ethically aware
when doing research with children have to be developed.[21] Also Westcott agrees with the general concerns about researchers having to take extra care when working with children.[22]
In their responding article to all the commentaries Yoder and Herrmann
again question whether memory implantation research with children is
necessary and conclude that ethical guidelines should be put in place by
Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (SARMAC).[23]
Although all the researchers involved in these commentaries from this
issue of "Applied Cognitive Psychology" put forward different arguments
the general consensus was that doing research with children calls for
extra care and the benefits from the research must outweigh potential
risk to the participating children.