Piaget's theory of cognitive development is a comprehensive theory about the nature and development of human intelligence. It was first created by the Swiss developmental psychologist Jean Piaget (1896–1980). The theory deals with the nature of knowledge itself and how humans gradually come to acquire, construct, and use it. Piaget's theory is mainly known as a developmental stage theory.
To Piaget, cognitive development
was a progressive reorganization of mental processes resulting from
biological maturation and environmental experience. He believed that
children construct an understanding of the world around them, experience
discrepancies between what they already know and what they discover in
their environment, then adjust their ideas accordingly.
Moreover, Piaget claimed that cognitive development is at the center of
the human organism, and language is contingent on knowledge and
understanding acquired through cognitive development. Piaget's earlier work received the greatest attention.
Child-centered classrooms and "open education" are direct applications of Piaget's views.
Despite its huge success, Piaget's theory has some limitations that
Piaget recognized himself: for example, the theory supports sharp stages
rather than continuous development (horizontal and vertical décalage).
Nature of intelligence: operative and figurative
Piaget noted that reality
is a dynamic system of continuous change. Reality is defined in
reference to the two conditions that define dynamic systems.
Specifically, he argued that reality involves transformations and
states. Transformations refer to all manners of changes that a thing or person can undergo. States
refer to the conditions or the appearances in which things or persons
can be found between transformations. For example, there might be
changes in shape or form (for instance, liquids are reshaped as they are
transferred from one vessel to another, and similarly humans change in
their characteristics as they grow older), in size (a toddler does not
walk and run without falling, but after 7 yrs of age, the child's
sensory motor anatomy is well developed and now acquires skill faster),
or in placement or location in space and time (e.g., various objects or
persons might be found at one place at one time and at a different place
at another time). Thus, Piaget argued, if human intelligence is to be adaptive, it must have functions to represent both the transformational and the static aspects of reality.
He proposed that operative intelligence is responsible for the
representation and manipulation of the dynamic or transformational
aspects of reality, and that figurative intelligence is responsible for
the representation of the static aspects of reality.
Operative intelligence is the active aspect of
intelligence. It involves all actions, overt or covert, undertaken in
order to follow, recover, or anticipate the transformations of the
objects or persons of interest. Figurative intelligence
is the more or less static aspect of intelligence, involving all means
of representation used to retain in mind the states (i.e., successive
forms, shapes, or locations) that intervene between transformations.
That is, it involves perception, imitation, mental imagery, drawing, and language.
Therefore, the figurative aspects of intelligence derive their meaning
from the operative aspects of intelligence, because states cannot exist
independently of the transformations that interconnect them. Piaget
stated that the figurative or the representational aspects of
intelligence are subservient to its operative and dynamic aspects, and
therefore, that understanding essentially derives from the operative
aspect of intelligence.
At any time, operative intelligence frames how the world is
understood and it changes if understanding is not successful. Piaget
stated that this process of understanding and change involves two basic
functions: assimilation and accommodation.
Assimilation and Accommodation
Through his study of the field of education, Piaget focused on two processes, which he named assimilation and accommodation.
To Piaget, assimilation meant integrating external elements into
structures of lives or environments, or those we could have through
experience. Assimilation is how humans perceive and adapt to new information. It is the process of fitting new information into pre-existing cognitive schemas. Assimilation in which new experiences are reinterpreted to fit into, or assimilate with, old ideas.
It occurs when humans are faced with new or unfamiliar information and
refer to previously learned information in order to make sense of it. In
contrast, accommodation is the process of taking new information
in one's environment and altering pre-existing schemas in order to fit
in the new information. This happens when the existing schema
(knowledge) does not work, and needs to be changed to deal with a new
object or situation. Accommodation is imperative because it is how people will continue to interpret new concepts, schemas, frameworks, and more. Piaget believed that the human brain has been programmed through evolution
to bring equilibrium, which is what he believed ultimately influences
structures by the internal and external processes through assimilation
and accommodation.
Piaget's understanding was that assimilation and accommodation cannot exist without the other.
They are two sides of a coin. To assimilate an object into an existing
mental schema, one first needs to take into account or accommodate to
the particularities of this object to a certain extent. For instance, to
recognize (assimilate) an apple as an apple, one must first focus
(accommodate) on the contour of this object. To do this, one needs to
roughly recognize the size of the object. Development increases the
balance, or equilibration, between these two functions. When in balance
with each other, assimilation and accommodation generate mental schemas
of the operative intelligence. When one function dominates over the
other, they generate representations which belong to figurative
intelligence.
Sensorimotor stage
In his theory of Cognitive development, Jean Piaget proposed that humans progress through four developmental stages:
the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational and formal operational period. The first of these, the sensorimotor stage "extends from birth to the acquisition of language."
In this stage, infants progressively construct knowledge and
understanding of the world by coordinating experiences (such as vision
and hearing) with physical interactions with objects (such as grasping,
sucking, and stepping). Infants gain knowledge of the world from the physical actions they perform within it. They progress from reflexive, instinctual action at birth to the beginning of symbolic thought toward the end of the stage.
Children learn that they are separate from the environment. They
can think about aspects of the environment, even though these may be
outside the reach of the child's senses. In this stage, according to
Piaget, the development of object permanence is one of the most important accomplishments. Object permanence is a child's understanding that an object continues to exist even though they cannot see or hear it. Peek-a-boo
is a game in which children who have yet to fully develop object
permanence respond to sudden hiding and revealing of a face. By the
end of the sensorimotor period, children develop a permanent sense of
self and object and will quickly lose interest in Peek-a-boo.
Piaget divided the sensorimotor stage into six sub-stages".
Sub-Stage | Age | Description |
---|---|---|
1 Simple reflexes | Birth-6 weeks | "Coordination of sensation and action through reflexive behaviors". Three primary reflexes are described by Piaget: sucking of objects in the mouth, following moving or interesting objects with the eyes, and closing of the hand when an object makes contact with the palm (palmar grasp). Over the first six weeks of life, these reflexes begin to become voluntary actions. For example, the palmar reflex becomes intentional grasping. |
2 First habits and primary circular reactions phase | 6 weeks-4 months | "Coordination of sensation and two types of schema: habits (reflex) and primary circular reactions (reproduction of an event that initially occurred by chance). The main focus is still on the infant's body". As an example of this type of reaction, an infant might repeat the motion of passing their hand before their face. Also at this phase, passive reactions, caused by classical or operant conditioning, can begin. |
3 Secondary circular reactions phase | 4–8 months | Development of habits. "Infants become more object-oriented, moving beyond self-preoccupation; repeat actions that bring interesting or pleasurable results". This stage is associated primarily with the development of coordination between vision and prehension. Three new abilities occur at this stage: intentional grasping for a desired object, secondary circular reactions, and differentiations between ends and means. At this stage, infants will intentionally grasp the air in the direction of a desired object, often to the amusement of friends and family. Secondary circular reactions, or the repetition of an action involving an external object begin; for example, moving a switch to turn on a light repeatedly. The differentiation between means and ends also occurs. This is perhaps one of the most important stages of a child's growth as it signifies the dawn of logic. |
4 Coordination of secondary circular reactions stages | 8–12 months | "Coordination of vision and touch—hand-eye coordination; coordination of schemas and intentionality". This stage is associated primarily with the development of logic and the coordination between means and ends. This is an extremely important stage of development, holding what Piaget calls the "first proper intelligence". Also, this stage marks the beginning of goal orientation, the deliberate planning of steps to meet an objective. |
5 Tertiary circular reactions, novelty, and curiosity | 12–18 months | "Infants become intrigued by the many properties of objects and by the many things they can make happen to objects; they experiment with new behavior". This stage is associated primarily with the discovery of new means to meet goals. Piaget describes the child at this juncture as the "young scientist," conducting pseudo-experiments to discover new methods of meeting challenges. |
6 Internalization of schemas | 18–24 months | "Infants develop the ability to use primitive symbols and form enduring mental representations". This stage is associated primarily with the beginnings of insight, or true creativity. This marks the passage into the preoperational stage. |
Pre-operational stage
By
observing sequences of play, Piaget was able to demonstrate that,
towards the end of the second year, a qualitatively new kind of
psychological functioning occurs, known as the pre-operational stage,
the second of Piaget's four developmental stages.
It starts when the child begins to learn to speak at age two and lasts
up until the age of seven. During the pre-operational stage of cognitive
development, Piaget noted that children do not yet understand concrete
logic and cannot mentally manipulate information.
Children's increase in playing and pretending takes place in this
stage. However, the child still has trouble seeing things from different
points of view. The children's play is mainly categorized by symbolic
play and manipulating symbols. Such play is demonstrated by the idea of
checkers being snacks, pieces of paper being plates, and a box being a
table. Their observations of symbols exemplifies the idea of play with
the absence of the actual objects involved.
The pre-operational stage is sparse and logically inadequate in
regard to mental operations. The child is able to form stable concepts
as well as magical beliefs. The child, however, is still not able to
perform operations, which are tasks that the child can do mentally,
rather than physically. Thinking in this stage is still egocentric, meaning the child has difficulty seeing the viewpoint of others. The Pre-operational Stage is split into two substages: the symbolic function substage, and the intuitive
thought substage. The symbolic function substage is when children are
able to understand, represent, remember, and picture objects in their
mind without having the object in front of them. The intuitive thought
substage is when children tend to propose the questions of "why?" and
"how come?" This stage is when children want to understand everything.
Symbolic function substage
At
about two to four years of age, children cannot yet manipulate and
transform information in a logical way. However, they now can think in
images and symbols. Other examples of mental abilities are language and
pretend play. Symbolic play is when children develop imaginary friends
or role-play with friends. Children's play becomes more social and they
assign roles to each other. Some examples of symbolic play include
playing house, or having a tea party. The type of symbolic play in which
children engage is connected with their level of creativity and ability
to connect with others.
Additionally, the quality of their symbolic play can have consequences
on their later development. For example, young children whose symbolic
play is of a violent nature tend to exhibit less prosocial behavior and
are more likely to display antisocial tendencies in later years.
In this stage, there are still limitations, such as egocentrism and precausal thinking.
Egocentrism
occurs when a child is unable to distinguish between their own
perspective and that of another person. Children tend to stick to their
own viewpoint, rather than consider the view of others. Indeed, they are
not even aware that such a concept as "different viewpoints" exists. Egocentrism can be seen in an experiment performed by Piaget and Swiss developmental psychologist Bärbel Inhelder, known as the three mountain problem.
In this experiment, three views of a mountain are shown to the child,
who is asked what a traveling doll would see at the various angles. The
child will consistently describe what they can see from the position
from which they are seated, regardless of the angle from which they are
asked to take the doll's perspective. Egocentrism would also cause a
child to believe, "I like Sesame Street, so Daddy must like Sesame Street, too."
Similar to preoperational children's egocentric thinking is their structuring of a cause and effect
relationships. Piaget coined the term "precausal thinking" to describe
the way in which preoperational children use their own existing ideas or
views, like in egocentrism, to explain cause-and-effect relationships.
Three main concepts of causality as displayed by children in the
preoperational stage include: animism, artificialism and transductive reasoning.
Animism is the belief that inanimate objects are capable of
actions and have lifelike qualities. An example could be a child
believing that the sidewalk was mad and made them fall down, or that the
stars twinkle in the sky because they are happy. Artificialism refers
to the belief that environmental characteristics can be attributed to
human actions or interventions. For example, a child might say that it
is windy outside because someone is blowing very hard, or the clouds are
white because someone painted them that color. Finally, precausal
thinking is categorized by transductive reasoning. Transductive
reasoning is when a child fails to understand the true relationships
between cause and effect. Unlike deductive or inductive reasoning
(general to specific, or specific to general), transductive reasoning
refers to when a child reasons from specific to specific, drawing a
relationship between two separate events that are otherwise unrelated.
For example, if a child hears the dog bark and then a balloon popped,
the child would conclude that because the dog barked, the balloon
popped.
Intuitive thought substage
At
between about the ages of 4 and 7, children tend to become very curious
and ask many questions, beginning the use of primitive reasoning. There
is an emergence in the interest of reasoning and wanting to know why
things are the way they are. Piaget called it the "intuitive substage"
because children realize they have a vast amount of knowledge, but they
are unaware of how they acquired it. Centration, conservation, irreversibility,
class inclusion, and transitive inference are all characteristics of
preoperative thought.
Centration is the act of focusing all attention on one characteristic or
dimension of a situation, whilst disregarding all others. Conservation
is the awareness that altering a substance's appearance does not change
its basic properties. Children at this stage are unaware of conservation
and exhibit centration. Both centration and conservation can be more
easily understood once familiarized with Piaget's most famous
experimental task.
In this task, a child is presented with two identical beakers
containing the same amount of liquid. The child usually notes that the
beakers do contain the same amount of liquid. When one of the beakers is
poured into a taller and thinner container, children who are younger
than seven or eight years old typically say that the two beakers no
longer contain the same amount of liquid, and that the taller container
holds the larger quantity (centration), without taking into
consideration the fact that both beakers were previously noted to
contain the same amount of liquid. Due to superficial changes, the child
was unable to comprehend that the properties of the substances
continued to remain the same (conservation).
Irreversibility is a concept developed in this stage which is
closely related to the ideas of centration and conservation.
Irreversibility refers to when children are unable to mentally reverse a
sequence of events. In the same beaker situation, the child does not
realize that, if the sequence of events was reversed and the water from
the tall beaker was poured back into its original beaker, then the same
amount of water would exist. Another example of children's reliance on
visual representations is their misunderstanding of "less than" or "more
than". When two rows containing equal amounts of blocks are placed in
front of a child, one row spread farther apart than the other, the child
will think that the row spread farther contains more blocks.
Class inclusion refers to a kind of conceptual thinking that
children in the preoperational stage cannot yet grasp. Children's
inability to focus on two aspects of a situation at once inhibits them
from understanding the principle that one category or class can contain
several different subcategories or classes.
For example, a four-year-old girl may be shown a picture of eight dogs
and three cats. The girl knows what cats and dogs are, and she is aware
that they are both animals. However, when asked, "Are there more dogs or
animals?" she is likely to answer "more dogs". This is due to her
difficulty focusing on the two subclasses and the larger class all at
the same time. She may have been able to view the dogs as dogs or animals, but struggled when trying to classify them as both, simultaneously. Similar to this is concept relating to intuitive thought, known as "transitive inference".
Transitive inference is using previous knowledge to determine the
missing piece, using basic logic. Children in the preoperational stage
lack this logic. An example of transitive inference would be when a
child is presented with the information "A" is greater than "B" and "B"
is greater than "C". This child may have difficulty here understanding
that "A" is also greater than "C".
Concrete operational stage
The concrete operational stage
is the third stage of Piaget's theory of cognitive development. This
stage, which follows the preoperational stage, occurs between the ages
of 7 and 11 (preadolescence) years,
and is characterized by the appropriate use of logic. During this
stage, a child's thought processes become more mature and "adult like".
They start solving problems in a more logical fashion. Abstract, hypothetical
thinking is not yet developed in the child, and children can only solve
problems that apply to concrete events or objects. At this stage, the
children undergo a transition where the child learns rules such as conservation. Piaget determined that children are able to incorporate Inductive reasoning. Inductive reasoning involves drawing inferences from observations in order to make a generalization. In contrast, children struggle with deductive reasoning,
which involves using a generalized principle in order to try to predict
the outcome of an event. Children in this stage commonly experience
difficulties with figuring out logic in their heads. For example, a
child will understand that "A is more than B" and "B is more than C".
However, when asked "is A more than C?", the child might not be able to
logically figure the question out in his or her head.
Two other important processes in the concrete operational stage are logic and the elimination of egocentrism.
Egocentrism is the inability to consider or understand a
perspective other than one's own. It is the phase where the thought and
morality of the child is completely self focused.
During this stage, the child acquires the ability to view things from
another individual's perspective, even if they think that perspective is
incorrect. For instance, show a child a comic in which Jane puts a doll
under a box, leaves the room, and then Melissa moves the doll to a
drawer, and Jane comes back. A child in the concrete operations stage
will say that Jane will still think it's under the box even though the
child knows it is in the drawer.
Children in this stage can, however, only solve problems that
apply to actual (concrete) objects or events, and not abstract concepts
or hypothetical tasks. Understanding and knowing how to use full common
sense has not yet been completely adapted.
Piaget determined that children in the concrete operational stage
were able to incorporate inductive logic. On the other hand, children
at this age have difficulty using deductive logic, which involves using a
general principle to predict the outcome of a specific event. This
includes mental reversibility. An example of this is being able to
reverse the order of relationships between mental categories. For
example, a child might be able to recognize that his or her dog is a
Labrador, that a Labrador is a dog, and that a dog is an animal, and
draw conclusions from the information available, as well as apply all
these processes to hypothetical situations.
The abstract quality of the adolescent's thought at the formal
operational level is evident in the adolescent's verbal problem solving
ability.
The logical quality of the adolescent's thought is when children are
more likely to solve problems in a trial-and-error fashion. Adolescents begin to think more as a scientist thinks, devising plans to solve problems and systematically test opinions.
They use hypothetical-deductive reasoning, which means that they
develop hypotheses or best guesses, and systematically deduce, or
conclude, which is the best path to follow in solving the problem.
During this stage the adolescent is able to understand love, logical
proofs and values. During this stage the young person begins to
entertain possibilities for the future and is fascinated with what they
can be.
Adolescents also are changing cognitively by the way that they think about social matters.
Adolescent egocentrism governs the way that adolescents think about
social matters, and is the heightened self-consciousness in them as they
are, which is reflected in their sense of personal uniqueness and
invincibility. Adolescent egocentrism can be dissected into two types of social thinking, imaginary audience that involves attention-getting behavior, and personal fable, which involves an adolescent's sense of personal uniqueness and invincibility.
These two types of social thinking begin to affect a child's
egocentrism in the concrete stage. However, it carries over to the
formal operational stage when they are then faced with abstract thought
and fully logical thinking.
Testing for concrete operations
Piagetian
tests are well known and practiced to test for concrete operations. The
most prevalent tests are those for conservation. There are some
important aspects that the experimenter must take into account when
performing experiments with these children.
One example of an experiment for testing conservation is the water level task.
An experimenter will have two glasses that are the same size, fill them
to the same level with liquid, which the child will acknowledge is the
same. Then, the experimenter will pour the liquid from one of the small
glasses into a tall, thin glass. The experimenter will then ask the
child if the taller glass has more liquid, less liquid, or the same
amount of liquid. The child will then give his answer. The experimenter
will ask the child why he gave his answer, or why he thinks that is.
- Justification: After the child has answered the question being posed, the experimenter must ask why the child gave that answer. This is important because the answers they give can help the experimenter to assess the child's developmental age.
- Number of times asking: Some argue that if a child is asked if the amount of liquid in the first set of glasses is equal then, after pouring the water into the taller glass, the experimenter asks again about the amount of liquid, the children will start to doubt their original answer. They may start to think that the original levels were not equal, which will influence their second answer.
- Word choice: The phrasing that the experimenter uses may affect how the child answers. If, in the liquid and glass example, the experimenter asks, "Which of these glasses has more liquid?", the child may think that his thoughts of them being the same is wrong because the adult is saying that one must have more. Alternatively, if the experimenter asks, "Are these equal?", then the child is more likely to say that they are, because the experimenter is implying that they are.
Formal operational stage
The final stage is known as the formal operational stage
(adolescence and into adulthood, roughly ages 11 to approximately
15–20): Intelligence is demonstrated through the logical use of symbols
related to abstract concepts. This form of thought includes "assumptions
that have no necessary relation to reality."
At this point, the person is capable of hypothetical and deductive
reasoning. During this time, people develop the ability to think about
abstract concepts.
Piaget stated that "hypothetico-deductive reasoning"
becomes important during the formal operational stage. This type of
thinking involves hypothetical "what-if" situations that are not always
rooted in reality, i.e. counterfactual thinking. It is often required in science and mathematics.
- Abstract thought emerges during the formal operational stage. Children tend to think very concretely and specifically in earlier stages, and begin to consider possible outcomes and consequences of actions.
- Metacognition, the capacity for "thinking about thinking" that allows adolescents and adults to reason about their thought processes and monitor them.
- Problem-solving is demonstrated when children use trial-and-error to solve problems. The ability to systematically solve a problem in a logical and methodical way emerges.
While children in primary school years mostly used inductive reasoning, drawing general conclusions from personal experiences and specific facts, adolescents become capable of deductive reasoning,
in which they draw specific conclusions from abstract concepts using
logic. This capability results from their capacity to think
hypothetically.
"However, research has shown that not all persons in all cultures
reach formal operations, and most people do not use formal operations
in all aspects of their lives".
Experiments
Piaget and his colleagues conducted several experiments to assess formal operational thought.
In one of the experiments, Piaget evaluated the cognitive
capabilities of children of different ages through the use of a scale
and varying weights. The task was to balance the scale by hooking
weights on the ends of the scale. To successfully complete the task, the
children must use formal operational thought to realize that the
distance of the weights from the center and the heaviness of the weights
both affected the balance. A heavier weight has to be placed closer to
the center of the scale, and a lighter weight has to be placed farther
from the center, so that the two weights balance each other.
While 3- to 5- year olds could not at all comprehend the concept of
balancing, children by the age of 7 could balance the scale by placing
the same weights on both ends, but they failed to realize the importance
of the location. By age 10, children could think about location but
failed to use logic and instead used trial-and-error. Finally, by age 13
and 14, in early adolescence, some children more clearly understood the
relationship between weight and distance and could successfully
implement their hypothesis.
The stages and causation
Piaget sees children's conception of causation
as a march from "primitive" conceptions of cause to those of a more
scientific, rigorous, and mechanical nature. These primitive concepts
are characterized as supernatural, with a decidedly non-natural or non-mechanical tone. Piaget has as his most basic assumption that babies are phenomenists.
That is, their knowledge "consists of assimilating things to schemas"
from their own action such that they appear, from the child's point of
view, "to have qualities which, in fact, stem from the organism".
Consequently, these "subjective conceptions," so prevalent during
Piaget's first stage of development, are dashed upon discovering deeper empirical truths.
Piaget gives the example of a child believing that the moon and
stars follow him on a night walk. Upon learning that such is the case
for his friends, he must separate his self from the object, resulting in
a theory that the moon is immobile, or moves independently of other agents.
The second stage, from around three to eight years of age, is characterized by a mix of this type of magical, animistic,
or "non-natural" conceptions of causation and mechanical or
"naturalistic" causation. This conjunction of natural and non-natural
causal explanations supposedly stems from experience itself, though
Piaget does not make much of an attempt to describe the nature of the
differences in conception. In his interviews with children, he asked
questions specifically about natural phenomena,
such as: "What makes clouds move?", "What makes the stars move?", "Why
do rivers flow?" The nature of all the answers given, Piaget says, are
such that these objects must perform their actions to "fulfill their
obligations towards men". He calls this "moral explanation".
Practical applications
Parents can use Piaget's theory when deciding how to determine what to buy in order to support their child's growth.
Teachers can also use Piaget's theory, for instance, when discussing
whether the syllabus subjects are suitable for the level of students or
not.
For example, recent studies have shown that children in the same grade
and of the same age perform differentially on tasks measuring basic
addition and subtraction fluency. While children in the preoperational
and concrete operational levels of cognitive development perform
combined arithmetic operations (such as addition and subtraction) with
similar accuracy,
children in the concrete operational level of cognitive development
have been able to perform both addition problems and subtraction
problems with overall greater fluency.
The stage of cognitive growth of a person differ from another.
Cognitive development or thinking is an active process from the
beginning to the end of life. Intellectual advancement happens because
people at every age and developmental period looks for cognitive
equilibrium. To achieve this balance, the easiest way is to understand
the new experiences through the lens of the preexisting ideas. Infants
learn that new objects can be grabbed in the same way of familiar
objects, and adults explain the day's headlines as evidence for their
existing worldview.
However, the application of standardized Piagetian theory and
procedures in different societies established widely varying results
that lead some to speculate not only that some cultures produce more
cognitive development than others but that without specific kinds of
cultural experience, but also formal schooling, development might cease
at certain level, such as concrete operational level.
A procedure was done following methods developed in Geneva (i.e. water
level task). Participants were presented with two beakers of equal
circumference and height, filled with equal amounts of water. The water
from one beaker was transferred into another with taller and smaller
circumference. The children and young adults from non-literate
societies of a given age were more likely to think that the taller,
thinner beaker had more water in it. On the other hand, an experiment on
the effects of modifying testing procedures to match local cultural
produced a different pattern of results. In the revised procedures, the
participants explained in their own language and indicated that while
the water was now "more", the quantity was the same. Piaget's water level task has also been applied to the elderly by Formann and results showed an age-associated non-linear decline of performance.
Postulated physical mechanisms underlying schemas and stages
In 1967, Piaget considered the possibility of RNA molecules as likely embodiments of his still-abstract schemas (which he promoted as units of action)—though he did not come to any firm conclusion.
At that time, due to work such as that of Swedish biochemist Holger
Hydén, RNA concentrations had, indeed, been shown to correlate with
learning, so the idea was quite plausible.
However, by the time of Piaget's death in 1980, this notion had lost favor. One main problem was over the protein
which, it was assumed, such RNA would necessarily produce, and that did
not fit in with observation. It was determined that only about 3% of
RNA does code for protein. Hence, most of the remaining 97% (the "ncRNA")
could theoretically be available to serve as Piagetian schemas (or
other regulatory roles in the 2000s under investigation). The issue has
not yet been resolved experimentally, but its theoretical aspects were
reviewed in 2008 — then developed further from the viewpoints of biophysics and epistemology.
Meanwhile, this RNA-based approach also unexpectedly offered
explanations for other several biological issues unresolved, thus
providing some measure of corroboration.
Relation to psychometric theories of intelligence
Piaget
designed a number of tasks to verify hypotheses arising from his
theory. The tasks were not intended to measure individual differences,
and they have no equivalent in psychometric
intelligence tests. Notwithstanding the different research traditions
in which psychometric tests and Piagetian tasks were developed, the
correlations between the two types of measures have been found to be
consistently positive and generally moderate in magnitude. A common
general factor underlies them. It has been shown that it is possible to
construct a battery consisting of Piagetian tasks that is as good a
measure of general intelligence as standard IQ tests.
Challenges to Piagetian stage theory
Piagetian
accounts of development have been challenged on several grounds. First,
as Piaget himself noted, development does not always progress in the
smooth manner his theory seems to predict. Décalage, or
progressive forms of cognitive developmental progression in a specific
domain, suggest that the stage model is, at best, a useful
approximation.
Furthermore, studies have found that children may be able to learn
concepts and capability of complex reasoning that supposedly represented
in more advanced stages with relative ease (Lourenço & Machado,
1996, p. 145). More broadly, Piaget's theory is "domain general,"
predicting that cognitive maturation occurs concurrently across
different domains of knowledge (such as mathematics, logic, and
understanding of physics or language).
Piaget did not take into account variability in a child's performance
notably how a child can differ in sophistication across several domains.
During the 1980s and 1990s, cognitive developmentalists were influenced by "neo-nativist" and evolutionary psychology ideas. These ideas de-emphasized domain general theories and emphasized domain specificity or modularity of mind.
Modularity implies that different cognitive faculties may be largely
independent of one another, and thus develop according to quite
different timetables, which are "influenced by real world experiences".
In this vein, some cognitive developmentalists argued that, rather than
being domain general learners, children come equipped with domain
specific theories, sometimes referred to as "core knowledge," which
allows them to break into learning within that domain. For example, even
young infants appear to be sensitive to some predictable regularities
in the movement and interactions of objects (for example, an object
cannot pass through another object), or in human behavior (for example, a
hand repeatedly reaching for an object has that object, not just a
particular path of motion), as it becomes the building block of which
more elaborate knowledge is constructed.
Piaget's theory has been said to undervalue the influence that
culture has on cognitive development. Piaget demonstrates that a child
goes through several stages of cognitive development and come to
conclusions on their own but in reality, a child's sociocultural
environment plays an important part in their cognitive development.
Social interaction teaches the child about the world and helps them
develop through the cognitive stages, which Piaget neglected to
consider.
More recent work has strongly challenged some of the basic
presumptions of the "core knowledge" school, and revised ideas of domain
generality—but from a newer dynamic systems approach, not from a
revised Piagetian perspective. Dynamic systems approaches harken to
modern neuroscientific
research that was not available to Piaget when he was constructing his
theory. One important finding is that domain-specific knowledge is
constructed as children develop and integrate knowledge. This enables
the domain to improve the accuracy of the knowledge as well as
organization of memories.
However, this suggests more of a "smooth integration" of learning and
development than either Piaget, or his neo-nativist critics, had
envisioned. Additionally, some psychologists, such as Lev Vygotsky and Jerome Bruner, thought differently from Piaget, suggesting that language was more important for cognition development than Piaget implied.
Post-Piagetian and neo-Piagetian stages
In recent years, several theorists attempted to address concerns with
Piaget's theory by developing new theories and models that can
accommodate evidence which violates Piagetian predictions and
postulates.
- The neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development, advanced by Robbie Case, Andreas Demetriou, Graeme S. Halford, Kurt W. Fischer, Michael Lamport Commons, and Juan Pascual-Leone, attempted to integrate Piaget's theory with cognitive and differential theories of cognitive organization and development. Their aim was to better account for the cognitive factors of development and for intra-individual and inter-individual differences in cognitive development. They suggested that development along Piaget's stages is due to increasing working memory capacity and processing efficiency by "biological maturation". Moreover, Demetriou's theory ascribes an important role to hypercognitive processes of "self-monitoring, self-recording, self-evaluation, and self-regulation", and it recognizes the operation of several relatively autonomous domains of thought (Demetriou, 1998; Demetriou, Mouyi, Spanoudis, 2010; Demetriou, 2003, p. 153).
- Piaget's theory stops at the formal operational stage, but other researchers have observed the thinking of adults is more nuanced than formal operational thought. This fifth stage has been named post formal thought or operation. Post formal stages have been proposed. Michael Commons presented evidence for four post formal stages in the model of hierarchical complexity: systematic, meta-systematic, paradigmatic, and cross-paradigmatic (Commons & Richards, 2003, p. 206–208; Oliver, 2004, p. 31). There are many theorists, however, who have criticized "post formal thinking," because the concept lacks both theoretical and empirical verification. The term "integrative thinking" has been suggested for use instead.
- A "sentential" stage, said to occur before the early preoperational stage, has been proposed by Fischer, Biggs and Biggs, Commons, and Richards.
- Searching for a micro-physiological basis for human mental capacity, Robert R. Traill (1978, Section C5.4; 1999, Section 8.4) proposed that there may be "pre-sensorimotor" stages ("M−1L", "M−2L", …), which are developed in the womb and/or transmitted genetically.
- Jerome Bruner has expressed views on cognitive development in a "pragmatic orientation" in which humans actively use knowledge for practical applications, such as problem solving and understanding reality.
- Michael Lamport Commons proposed the model of hierarchical complexity (MHC) in two dimensions: horizontal complexity and vertical complexity (Commons & Richards, 2003, p. 205).
- Kieran Egan has proposed five stages of understanding: "somatic", "mythic", "romantic", "philosophic", and "ironic", which is developed through cognitive tools such as "stories", "binary oppositions", "fantasy" and "rhyme, rhythm, and meter" to enhance memorization to develop a long-lasting learning capacity.
- Lawrence Kohlberg developed three stages of moral development: "Preconventional", "Conventional" and "Postconventional". Each level is composed of two orientation stages, with a total of six orientation stages: (1) "Punishment-Obedience", (2) "Instrumental Relativist", (3) "Good Boy-Nice Girl", (4) "Law and Order", (5) "Social Contract", and (6) "Universal Ethical Principle".
- Andreas Demetriou has expressed neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development.
- Jane Loevinger's stages of ego development occur through "an evolution of stages". "First is the Presocial Stage followed by the Symbiotic Stage, Impulsive Stage, Self-Protective Stage, Conformist Stage, Self-Aware Level: Transition from Conformist to Conscientious Stage, Individualistic Level: Transition from Conscientious to the Autonomous Stage, Conformist Stage, and Integrated Stage".
- Ken Wilber has incorporated Piaget's theory in his multidisciplinary field of integral theory. The human consciousness is structured in hierarchical order and organized in "holon" chains or "great chain of being", which are based on the level of spiritual and psychological development.
- The process of initiation is a modification of Piaget's theory integrating Abraham Maslow's concept of self-actualization.
- Cheryl Armon has proposed five stages of " the Good Life": "Egoistic Hedonism", "Instrumental Hedonism", "Affective/Altruistic Mutuality", "Individuality", and "Autonomy/Community" (Andreoletti & Demick, 2003, p. 284) (Armon, 1984, p. 40–43).
- Christopher R. Hallpike proposed that human evolution of cognitive moral understanding had evolved from the beginning of time from its primitive state to the present time.
- Robert Kegan extended Piaget's developmental model to adults in describing what he called constructive-developmental psychology.