Numeracy is the ability to reason and to apply simple numerical concepts. Basic numeracy skills consist of comprehending fundamental arithmetics
like addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. For example,
if one can understand simple mathematical equations such as, 2 + 2 = 4,
then one would be considered possessing at least basic numeric
knowledge. Substantial aspects of numeracy also include number sense, operation sense, computation, measurement, geometry, probability and statistics. A numerically literate person can manage and respond to the mathematical demands of life.
By contrast, innumeracy (the lack of numeracy) can have a
negative impact. Numeracy has an influence on career decisions, and risk
perception towards health decisions. For example, innumeracy distorts
risk perception towards health decisions and may negatively affect economic choices. "Greater numeracy has been associated with reduced susceptibility to framing effects,
less influence of nonnumerical information such as mood states, and
greater sensitivity to different levels of numerical risk".
Representation of numbers
Humans have evolved to mentally represent numbers in two major ways from observation (not formal math). These representations are often thought to be innate, to be shared across human cultures, to be common to multiple species, and not to be the result of individual learning or cultural transmission. They are:
- Approximate representation of numerical magnitude, and
- Precise representation of the quantity of individual items.
Approximate representations of numerical magnitude imply that one can
relatively estimate and comprehend an amount if the number is large. For example, one experiment showed children and adults arrays of many dots.
After briefly observing them, both groups could accurately estimate the
approximate number of dots. However, distinguishing differences between
large numbers of dots proved to be more challenging.
Precise representations of distinct individuals demonstrate that
people are more accurate in estimating amounts and distinguishing
differences when the numbers are relatively small.
For example, in one experiment, an experimenter presented an infant
with two piles of crackers, one with two crackers the other with three.
The experimenter then covered each pile with a cup. When allowed to
choose a cup, the infant always chose the cup with more crackers because
the infant could distinguish the difference.
Both systems—approximate representation of magnitude and precise
representation quantity of individual items—have limited power. For
example, neither allows representations of fractions or negative numbers.
More complex representations require education. However, achievement in
school mathematics correlates with an individual's unlearned
approximate number sense.
Definitions and assessment
Fundamental (or rudimentary) numeracy skills include understanding of the real number line, time, measurement, and estimation.
Fundamental skills include basic skills (the ability to identify and
understand numbers) and computational skills (the ability to perform
simple arithmetical operations and compare numerical magnitudes).
More sophisticated numeracy skills include understanding of ratio
concepts (notably fractions, proportions, percentages, and
probabilities), and knowing when and how to perform multistep
operations.
Two categories of skills are included at the higher levels: the
analytical skills (the ability to understand numerical information, such
as required to interpret graphs and charts) and the statistical skills
(the ability to apply higher probabilistic and statistical computation,
such as conditional probabilities).
A variety of tests have been developed for assessing numeracy and health numeracy.
Childhood influences
The first couple of years of childhood are considered to be a vital part of life for the development of numeracy and literacy.
There are many components that play key roles in the development of
numeracy at a young age, such as Socioeconomic Status (SES), parenting,
Home Learning Environment (HLE), and age.
Socioeconomic status
Children who are brought up in families with high SES tend to be more engaged in developmentally enhancing activities. These children are more likely to develop the necessary abilities to learn and to become more motivated to learn.
More specifically, a mother's education level is considered to have an
effect on the child's ability to achieve in numeracy. That is, mothers
with a high level of education will tend to have children who succeed
more in numeracy.
A number of studies have, moreover, proved that the education level of mother is strongly correlated with the average age of getting married. To be more precise, females who entered the marriage later, tend to have greater autonomy,
chances for skills premium and level of education (i.e. numeracy).
Hence, they were more likely to share this experience with children.
Parenting
Parents
are suggested to collaborate with their child in simple learning
exercises, such as reading a book, painting, drawing, and playing with
numbers. On a more expressive note, the act of using complex language,
being more responsive towards the child, and establishing warm
interactions are recommended to parents with the confirmation of
positive numeracy outcomes.
When discussing beneficial parenting behaviors, a feedback loop is
formed because pleased parents are more willing to interact with their
child, which in essence promotes better development in the child.
Home-learning environment
Along with parenting and SES, a strong home-learning environment increases the likelihood of the child being prepared for comprehending complex mathematical schooling.
For example, if a child is influenced by many learning activities in
the household, such as puzzles, coloring books, mazes, or books with
picture riddles, then they will be more prepared to face school
activities.
Age
Age is accounted for when discussing the development of numeracy in children. Children under the age of 5 have the best opportunity to absorb basic numeracy skills. After the age of 7, achievement of basic numeracy skills become less influential.
For example, a study was conducted to compare the reading and
mathematic abilities between children, ages 5 and 7, each in three
different mental capacity groups (underachieving, average, and
overachieving). The differences in the amount of knowledge retained
were greater between the three different groups at age 5, than between
the groups at age 7. This reveals that the younger you are the greater
chance you have to retain more information, like numeracy.
Literacy
There seems to be a relationship between literacy and numeracy,
which can be seen in young children. Depending on the level of
literacy or numeracy at a young age, one can predict the growth of
literacy and/ or numeracy skills in future development.
There is some evidence that humans may have an inborn sense of number. In one study for example, five-month-old infants
were shown two dolls, which were then hidden with a screen. The babies
saw the experimenter pull one doll from behind the screen. Without the
child's knowledge, a second experimenter could remove, or add dolls,
unseen behind the screen. When the screen was removed, the infants
showed more surprise at an unexpected number (for example, if there were
still two dolls). Some researchers have concluded that the babies were
able to count, although others doubt this and claim the infants noticed
surface area rather than number.
Employment
Numeracy has a huge impact on employment. In a work environment, numeracy can be a controlling factor affecting career achievements and failures. Many professions require individuals to have a well-developed sense of numeracy, for example: mathematician, physicist, accountant, actuary, Risk Analyst, financial analyst, engineer, and architect.
Even outside these specialized areas, the lack of proper numeracy skills
can reduce employment opportunities and promotions, resulting in
unskilled manual careers, low-paying jobs, and even unemployment. For example, carpenters and interior designers need to be able to measure, use fractions, and handle budgets.
Another example pertaining to numeracy influencing employment was demonstrated at the Poynter Institute. The Poynter Institute has recently included numeracy as one of the skills required by competent journalists. Max Frankel, former executive editor of the New York Times, argues that "deploying numbers skillfully is as important to communication as deploying verbs". Unfortunately, it is evident that journalists often show poor numeracy skills. In a study by the Society of Professional Journalists, 58% of job applicants interviewed by broadcast news directors lacked an adequate understanding of statistical materials.
With regards to assessing applicants for an employment position, psychometric numerical reasoning tests have been created by occupational psychologists,
who are involved in the study of numeracy. These psychometric numerical
reasoning tests are used to assess an applicants' ability to comprehend
and apply numbers. These tests are sometimes administered with a time
limit, resulting in the need for the test-taker to think quickly and
concisely. Research has shown that these tests are very useful in
evaluating potential applicants because they do not allow the applicants
to prepare for the test, unlike interview questions. This suggests
that an applicant's results are reliable and accurate.
These psychometric numerical reasoning tests first became
prevalent during the 1980s, following the pioneering work of
psychologists, such as P. Kline. In 1986 P. Kline's published a book
entitled, "A handbook of test construction: Introduction to psychometric
design", which explained that psychometric testing could provide
reliable and objective results. These findings could then be used to
effectively assess a candidate's abilities in numeracy. In the future,
psychometric numerical reasoning tests will continue to be used in
employment assessments to fairly and accurately differentiate and
evaluate possible employment applicants.
Innumeracy and dyscalculia
Innumeracy is a neologism coined by an analogue with illiteracy. Innumeracy refers to a lack of ability to reason with numbers. The term innumeracy was coined by cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter. However, this term was popularized in 1989 by mathematician John Allen Paulos in his book entitled, Innumeracy: Mathematical Illiteracy and its Consequences.
Developmental dyscalculia
refers to a persistent and specific impairment of basic
numerical-arithmetical skills learning in the context of normal
intelligence.
Patterns and differences
The
root cause of innumeracy varies. Innumeracy has been seen in those
suffering from poor education and childhood deprivation of numeracy.
Innumeracy is apparent in children during the transition of numerical
skills obtained before schooling and the new skills taught in the
education departments because of their memory capacity to comprehend the
material. Patterns of innumeracy have also been observed depending on age, gender, and race.
Older adults have been associated with lower numeracy skills than younger adults. Men have been identified to have higher numeracy skills than women. Some studies seem to indicate young people of African heritage tend to have lower numeracy skills.
The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) in
which children at fourth-grade (average 10 to 11 years) and eighth-grade
(average 14 to 15 years) from 49 countries were tested on mathematical
comprehension. The assessment included tests for number, algebra (also
called patterns and relationships at fourth grade), measurement,
geometry, and data. The latest study, in 2003 found that children from Singapore
at both grade levels had the highest performance. Countries like Hong
Kong SAR, Japan, and Taiwan also shared high levels of numeracy. The
lowest scores were found in countries like South Africa, Ghana, and
Saudi Arabia. Another finding showed a noticeable difference between
boys and girls with some exceptions. For example, girls performed
significantly better in Singapore, and boys performed significantly
better in the United States.
Theory
There is a
theory that innumeracy is more common than illiteracy when dividing
cognitive abilities into two separate categories. David C. Geary, a
notable cognitive developmental and evolutionary psychologist from the University of Missouri, created the terms "biological primary abilities" and "biological secondary abilities".
Biological primary abilities evolve over time and are necessary for
survival. Such abilities include speaking a common language or
knowledge of simple mathematics.
Biological secondary abilities are attained through personal
experiences and cultural customs, such as reading or high level
mathematics learned through schooling.
Literacy and numeracy are similar in the sense that they are both
important skills used in life. However, they differ in the sorts of
mental demands each makes. Literacy consists of acquiring vocabulary and
grammatical sophistication, which seem to be more closely related to
memorization, whereas numeracy involves manipulating concepts, such as
in calculus or geometry, and builds from basic numeracy skills. This could be a potential explanation of the challenge of being numerate.
Innumeracy and risk perception in health decision-making
Health
numeracy has been defined as "the degree to which individuals have the
capacity to access, process, interpret, communicate, and act on
numerical, quantitative, graphical, biostatistical, and probabilistic
health information needed to make effective health decisions". The concept of health numeracy is a component of the concept of health literacy.
Health numeracy and health literacy can be thought of as the
combination of skills needed for understanding risk and making good
choices in health-related behavior.
Health numeracy requires basic numeracy but also more advanced
analytical and statistical skills. For instance, health numeracy also
requires the ability to understand probabilities or relative frequencies
in various numerical and graphical formats, and to engage in Bayesian inference, while avoiding errors sometimes associated with Bayesian reasoning.
Health numeracy also requires understanding terms with definitions that
are specific to the medical context. For instance, although 'survival'
and 'mortality' are complementary in common usage, these terms are not
complementary in medicine (see five-year survival rate).
Innumeracy is also a very common problem when dealing with risk
perception in health-related behavior; it is associated with patients,
physicians, journalists and policymakers.
Those who lack or have limited health numeracy skills run the risk of
making poor health-related decisions because of an inaccurate perception
of information.
For example, if a patient has been diagnosed with breast cancer, being
innumerate may hinder her ability to comprehend her physician's
recommendations or even the severity of the health concern. One study
found that people tended to overestimate their chances of survival or
even to choose lower quality hospitals. Innumeracy also makes it difficult or impossible for some patients to read medical graphs correctly. Some authors have distinguished graph literacy from numeracy.
Indeed, many doctors exhibit innumeracy when attempting to explain a
graph or statistics to a patient. Once again, a misunderstanding between
a doctor and patient due to either the doctor, patient, or both being
unable to comprehend numbers effectively could result in serious health
consequences.
Different presentation formats of numerical information, for
instance natural frequency icon arrays, have been evaluated to assist
both low numeracy and high numeracy individuals.
Evolution of Numeracy
In the field of economic history, numeracy is often used to assess human capital at times when there was no data on schooling or other educational measures. Using a method called age-heaping, researchers like professor Baten study the development and inequalities of numeracy over time and throughout regions. For example, Baten and Hippe find a numeracy gap between regions in West / Central Europe and the rest of Europe for the period 1790 - 1880. At the same time, their data analysis reveals that these differences as well as within country inequality decreased over time. Taking a similar approach, Baten and Fourie find overall high levels of numeracy for people in the Cape Colony (late 17th to early 19th century).
In contrast to these studies comparing numeracy over countries or regions, it is also possible to analyze numeracy within countries. For example, Baten, Crayen and Voth look at the effects of war on numeracy in England and Baten and Priwitzer find a "military bias" in today's West Hungary: people opting for a military career had - on average - better numeracy indicators (1 BCE to 3CE).