Dyscalculia | |
---|---|
Pronunciation | |
Specialty | Pediatrics |
Duration | Lifetime |
Dyscalculia /ˌdɪskælˈkjuːliə/ is difficulty in learning or comprehending arithmetic, such as difficulty in understanding numbers, learning how to manipulate numbers, performing mathematical calculations and learning facts in mathematics. It is generally seen as the mathematical equivalent to dyslexia.
It can occur in people from across the whole IQ range, along with difficulties with time, measurement, and spatial reasoning. Estimates of the prevalence of dyscalculia range between 3 and 6% of the population. In 2015, it was established that 11% of children with dyscalculia also have ADHD. Dyscalculia has also been associated with people who have Turner syndrome and people who have spina bifida.
Mathematical disabilities can occur as the result of some types of brain injury, in which case the proper term, acalculia, is to distinguish it from dyscalculia which is of innate, genetic or developmental origin.
Signs and symptoms
The
earliest appearance of dyscalculia is typically a deficit in the
ability to know, from a brief glance and without counting, how many
objects there are in a small group.
Children as young as 5 can subitize 6 objects, especially looking at a
die. However, children with dyscalculia can subitize fewer objects and
even when correct take longer to identify the number than their
age-matched peers.
Dyscalculia often looks different at different ages. It tends to become
more apparent as kids get older; however, symptoms can appear as early
as preschool. Common
symptoms of dyscalculia are, having difficulty with mental math,
trouble analyzing time and reading an analog clock, struggle with motor
sequencing that involves numbers, and often they will count on their
fingers when adding numbers.
Common symptoms
Dyscalculia is characterized by difficulties with common arithmetic tasks. These difficulties may include:
- Difficulty reading analog clocks
- Difficulty stating which of two numbers is larger
- Sequencing issues
- Inability to comprehend financial planning or budgeting, sometimes even at a basic level; for example, estimating the cost of the items in a shopping basket or balancing a checkbook
- Inconsistent results in addition, subtraction, multiplication and division
- Visualizing numbers as meaningless or nonsensical symbols, rather than perceiving them as characters indicating a numerical value (hence the misnomer, "math dyslexia")
- Difficulty with multiplication, subtraction, addition, and division tables, mental arithmetic, etc.
- Problems with differentiating between left and right
- A "warped" sense of spatial awareness, or an understanding of shapes, distance, or volume that seems more like guesswork than actual comprehension
- Difficulty with time, directions, recalling schedules, sequences of events, keeping track of time, frequently late or early
- Poor memory (retention and retrieval) of math concepts; may be able to perform math operations one day, but draw a blank the next; may be able to do book work but then fails tests
- Ability to grasp math on a conceptual level, but an inability to put those concepts into practice
- Difficulty recalling the names of numbers, or thinking that certain different numbers "feel" the same (e.g. frequently interchanging the same two numbers for each other when reading or recalling them)
- Difficulty reading musical notation
- Difficulty with choreographed dance steps
- Difficulty working backwards in time (e.g. What time to leave if needing to be somewhere at 'X' time)
- Having particular difficulty mentally estimating the measurement of an object or distance (e.g., whether something is 3 or 6 meters (10 or 20 feet) away)
- When writing, reading and recalling numbers, mistakes may occur in the areas such as: number additions, substitutions, transpositions, omissions, and reversals
- Inability to grasp and remember mathematical concepts, rules, formulae, and sequences
- Inability to concentrate on mentally intensive tasks
- Mistaken recollection of names, poor name/face retrieval, may substitute names beginning with same letter.
Persistence in children
Although
many researchers believe dyscalculia to be a persistent disorder,
evidence on the persistence of dyscalculia remains mixed.
For instance, in a study done by Mazzocco and Myers (2003), researchers
evaluated children on a slew of measures and selected their most
consistent measure as their best diagnostic criterion: a stringent
10th-percentile cut-off on the TEMA-2.
Even with their best criterion, they found dyscalculia diagnoses for
children longitudinally did not persist; only 65% of students who were
ever diagnosed over the course of four years were diagnosed for at least
two years. The percentage of children who were diagnosed in two
consecutive years was further reduced. It is unclear whether this was
the result of misdiagnosed children improving in mathematics and spatial
awareness as they progressed as normal, or that the subjects who showed
improvement were accurately diagnosed, but exhibited signs of a
non-persistent learning disability.
Persistence in adults
There
are very few studies of adults with dyscalculia who have had a history
of it growing up, but such studies have shown that it can persist into
adulthood. It can affect major parts of an adult's life. Most
adults with dyscalculia have a hard time processing math at a 4th grade
level. For 1st-4th grade level, many adults will know what to do for
the math problem, but they will often get them wrong because of
"careless errors," although they are not careless when it comes to the
problem. The adults cannot process their errors on the math problems or
may not even recognize that they have made these errors. Visual-spatial
input, auditory input, and touch input will be affected due to these
processing errors. Dyscalculics may have a difficult time adding numbers
in a column format because their mind can mix up the numbers, and it is
possible that they may get the same answer twice due to their mind
processing the problem incorrectly. Dyscalculics can have problems
determining differences in different coins and their size or giving the
correct amount of change and if numbers are grouped together, it is
possible that they cannot determine which has less or more.
If a dyscalculic is asked to choose the greater of two numbers, with
the lesser number in a larger font than the greater number, they may
take the question literally and pick the number with the bigger font.
Adults with dyscalculia have a tough time with directions while driving
and with controlling their finances, which causes difficulties on a
day-to-day basis.
College students or other adult learners
College
students particularly may have a tougher time due to the fast pace and
change in difficulty of the work they are given. As a result of this,
students may develop a lot of anxiety and frustration. After dealing
with their anxiety for a long time, students can become averse to math
and try to avoid it as much as possible, which may result in lower
grades in math courses. However, students with dyscalculia often do
exceptionally in writing, reading, and speaking. Students may try to
succeed through determination and persistence because of their inability
to do well with numbers. They may try to keep a positive attitude even
with the frustration and anxiety because they want to meet their goal in
life. The problem, when it comes to college, is that professors cannot
grade entirely on their persistence, determination, and efforts.
Students need to figure out ways to overcome their difficulties.
There are a lot of services that schools can provide for students. In
the 21st century there is evidence that there will be an increase in
enrollment for students with learning disabilities in community
colleges.
Causes
Both domain-general and domain-specific
causes have been put forth. With respect to pure developmental
dyscalculia, domain-general causes are unlikely as they should not
impair one’s ability in the numerical domain without also affecting
other domains such as reading.
Two competing domain-specific hypotheses about the causes of developmental dyscalculia have been proposed – the magnitude representation (or number module deficit hypothesis) and the access deficit hypothesis.
Magnitude representation deficit
Dehaene's "number sense"
theory suggests that approximate numerosities are automatically ordered
in an ascending manner on a mental number line. The mechanism to
represent and process non-symbolic magnitude (e.g., number of dots) is
often known as the "approximate number system"
(ANS), and a core deficit in the precision of the ANS, known as the
"magnitude representation hypothesis" or "number module deficit
hypothesis", has been proposed as an underlying cause of developmental
dyscalculia.
In particular, the structural features of the ANS is
theoretically supported by a phenomenon called the "numerical distance
effect", which has been robustly observed in numerical comparison tasks.
Typically developing individuals are less accurate and slower in
comparing pairs of numbers closer together (e.g., 7 and 8) than further
apart (e.g., 2 and 9). A related "numerical ratio effect" (in which the
ratio between two numbers varies but the distance is kept constant,
e.g., 2 vs. 5 and 4 vs. 7) based on the Weber's law has also been used to further support the structure of the ANS.
The numerical ratio effect is observed when individuals are less
accurate and slower in comparing pairs of numbers that have a larger
ratio (e.g., 8 and 9, ratio = 8/9) than a smaller ratio (2 and 3; ratio =
2/3). A larger numerical distance or ratio effect with comparison of
sets of objects (i.e., non-symbolic) is thought to reflect a less
precise ANS, and the ANS acuity has been found to correlate with math
achievement in typically developing children and also in adults.
More importantly, several behavioral studies
have found that children with developmental dyscalculia show an
attenuated distance/ratio effect than typically developing children.
Moreover, neuroimaging studies have also provided additional insights
even when behavioral difference in distance/ratio effect might not be
clearly evident. For example, Gavin R. Price and colleagues
found that children with developmental dyscalculia showed no
differential distance effect on reaction time relative to typically
developing children, but they did show a greater effect of distance on
response accuracy. They also found that the right intraparietal sulcus
in children with developmental dyscalculia was not modulated to the
same extent in response to non-symbolic numerical processing as in
typically developing children.
With the robust implication of the intraparietal sulcus in magnitude
representation, it is possible that children with developmental
dyscalculia have a weak magnitude representation in the parietal region.
Yet, it does not rule out an impaired ability to access and manipulate
numerical quantities from their symbolic representations (e.g., Arabic
digits).
Moreover, findings from a cross-sectional study suggest that children
with developmental dyscalculia might have a delayed development in
their numerical magnitude representation by as much as five years.
However, the lack of longitudinal studies still leaves the question
open as to whether the deficient numerical magnitude representation is a
delayed development or impairment.
Access deficit hypothesis
Rousselle & Noël
propose that dyscalculia is caused by the inability to map preexisting
representations of numerical magnitude onto symbolic Arabic digits.
Evidence for this hypothesis is based on research studies that have
found that individuals with dyscalculia are proficient on tasks that
measure knowledge of non-symbolic numerical magnitude (i.e.,
non-symbolic comparison tasks) but show an impaired ability to process
symbolic representations of number (i.e., symbolic comparison tasks). Neuroimaging studies also report increased activation in the right intraparietal sulcus during tasks that measure symbolic but not non-symbolic processing of numerical magnitude. However, support for the access deficit hypothesis is not consistent across research studies.
Diagnosis
At its most basic level, dyscalculia is a learning disability affecting the normal development of arithmetic skills.
A consensus has not yet been reached on appropriate diagnostic criteria for dyscalculia.
Mathematics is a specific domain that is complex (i.e. includes many
different processes, such as arithmetic, algebra, word problems,
geometry, etc.) and cumulative (i.e. the processes build on each other
such that mastery of an advanced skill requires mastery of many basic
skills). Thus dyscalculia can be diagnosed using different criteria, and
frequently is; this variety in diagnostic criteria leads to variability
in identified samples, and thus variability in research findings
regarding dyscalculia.
Other than using achievement tests as diagnostic criteria,
researchers often rely on domain-specific tests (i.e. tests of working
memory, executive function, inhibition, intelligence, etc.) and teacher
evaluations to create a more comprehensive diagnosis. Alternatively,
fMRI research has shown that the brains of the neurotypical
children can be reliably distinguished from the brains of the
dyscalculic children based on the activation in the prefrontal cortex.
However, due to the cost and time limitations associated with brain and
neural research, these methods will likely not be incorporated into
diagnostic criteria despite their effectiveness.
Types
Research on
subtypes of dyscalculia has begun without consensus; preliminary
research has focused on comorbid learning disorders as subtyping
candidates. The most common comorbidity in individuals with dyscalculia
is dyslexia.
Most studies done with comorbid samples versus dyscalculic-only samples
have shown different mechanisms at work and additive effects of
comorbidity, indicating that such subtyping may not be helpful in
diagnosing dyscalculia. But there is variability in results at present.
Due to high comorbidity with other disabilities such as dyslexia and ADHD,
some researchers have suggested the possibility of subtypes of
mathematical disabilities with different underlying profiles and causes.
Whether a particular subtype is specifically termed "dyscalculia" as
opposed to a more general mathematical learning disability is somewhat
under debate in the scientific literature.
- Semantic memory: This subtype often coexists with reading disabilities such as dyslexia and is characterized by poor representation and retrieval from long-term memory. These processes share a common neural pathway in the left angular gyrus, which has been shown to be selective in arithmetic fact retrieval strategies and symbolic magnitude judgments. This region also shows low functional connectivity with language-related areas during phonological processing in adults with dyslexia. Thus, disruption to the left angular gyrus can cause both reading impairments and difficulties in calculation. This has been observed in individuals with Gerstmann syndrome, of which dyscalculia is one of constellation of symptoms.
- Procedural concepts: Research by Geary has shown that in addition to increased problems with fact retrieval, children with math disabilities may rely on immature computational strategies. Specifically, children with mathematical disabilities showed poor command of counting strategies unrelated to their ability to retrieve numeric facts. This research notes that it is difficult to discern whether poor conceptual knowledge is indicative of a qualitative deficit in number processing or simply a delay in typical mathematical development.
- Working memory: Studies have found that children with dyscalculia showed impaired performance on working memory tasks compared to neurotypical children. Furthermore, research has shown that children with dyscalculia have weaker activation of the intraparietal sulcus during visuospatial working memory tasks. Brain activity in this region during such tasks has been linked to overall arithmetic performance, indicating that numerical and working memory functions may converge in the intraparietal sulcus. However, working memory problems are confounded with domain-general learning difficulties, thus these deficits may not be specific to dyscalculia but rather may reflect a greater learning deficit. Dysfunction in prefrontal regions may also lead to deficits in working memory and other executive function, accounting for comorbidity with ADHD.
Studies have also shown indications of causes due to congenital or hereditary disorders, but evidence of this is not yet concrete.
Treatment
To
date, very few interventions have been developed specifically for
individuals with dyscalculia. Concrete manipulation activities have been
used for decades to train basic number concepts for remediation
purposes.
This method facilitates the intrinsic relationship between a goal, the
learner’s action, and the informational feedback on the action.
A one-to-one tutoring paradigm designed by Lynn Fuchs and colleagues
which teaches concepts in arithmetic, number concepts, counting, and
number families using games, flash cards, and manipulables has proven
successful in children with generalized math learning difficulties, but
intervention has yet to be tested specifically on children with
dyscalculia.
These methods require specially trained teachers working directly with
small groups or individual students. As such, instruction time in the
classroom is necessarily limited. For this reason, several research
groups have developed computer adaptive training programs designed to
target deficits unique to dyscalculic individuals.
Software intended to remediate dyscalculia has been developed.
While computer adaptive training programs are modeled after one-to-one
type interventions, they provide several advantages. Most notably,
individuals are able to practice more with a digital intervention than
is typically possible with a class or teacher.
As with one-to-one interventions, several digital interventions have
also proven successful in children with generalized math learning
difficulties. Räsänen and colleagues have found that games such as The
Number Race and Graphogame-math can improve performance on number
comparison tasks in children with generalized math learning
difficulties.
Several digital interventions have been developed for
dyscalculics specifically. Each attempts to target basic processes that
are associated with maths difficulties. Rescue Calcularis was one early
computerized intervention that sought to improve the integrity of and
access to the mental number line. Other digital interventions for dyscalculia adapt games, flash cards, and manipulables to function through technology.
While each intervention claims to improve basic numerosity
skills, the authors of these interventions do admit that repetition and
practice effects may be a factor involved in reported performance gains. An additional criticism is that these digital interventions lack the option to manipulate numerical quantities.
While the previous two games provide the correct answer, the individual
using the intervention cannot actively determine, through manipulation,
what the correct answer should be. Butterworth and colleagues argued
that games like The Number Bonds, which allows an individual to compare
different sized rods, should be the direction that digital interventions
move towards. Such games use manipulation activities to provide
intrinsic motivation towards content guided by dyscalculia research. One
of these serious games is Meister Cody – Talasia, an online training that includes the CODY Assessment
– a diagnostic test for detecting dyscalculia. Based on these findings,
Rescue Calcluaris was extended by adaptation algorithms and game forms
allowing manipulation by the learners. It was found to improve addition, subtraction and number line tasks, and was made available as Dybuster Calcularis.
A study used transcranial direct current stimulation
(TDCS) to the parietal lobe during numerical learning and demonstrated
selective improvement of numerical abilities that was still present six
months later in typically developing individuals.
Improvement were achieved by applying anodal current to the right
parietal lobe and cathodal current to the left parietal lobe and
contrasting it with the reverse setup. When the same research group used
tDCS in a training study with two dyscalculic individuals, the reverse
setup (left anodal, right cathodal) demonstrated improvement of
numerical abilities.
Epidemiology
Dyscalculia is thought to be present in 3–6% of the general population, but estimates by country and sample vary somewhat. Many studies have found prevalence rates by gender to be equivalent.
Those that find gender difference in prevalence rates often find
dyscalculia higher in females, but some few studies have found
prevalence rates higher in males.
History
The term 'dyscalculia' was coined in the 1940s, but it was not completely recognized until 1974 by the work of Czechoslovakian
researcher Ladislav Kosc. Kosc defined dyscalculia as "a structural
disorder of mathematical abilities." His research proved that the
learning disability was caused by impairments to certain parts of the
brain that control mathematical calculations and not because symptomatic
individuals were 'mentally handicapped'. Researchers now sometimes use
the terms “math dyslexia” or “math learning disability” when they
mention the condition.
Cognitive disabilities specific to mathematics were originally
identified in case studies with patients who experienced specific
arithmetic disabilities as a result of damage to specific regions of the
brain. More commonly, dyscalculia occurs developmentally as a
genetically linked learning disability which affects a person's ability
to understand, remember, or manipulate numbers or number facts (e.g.,
the multiplication tables).
The term is often used to refer specifically to the inability to
perform arithmetic operations, but is also defined by some educational
professionals and cognitive psychologists such as Stanislas Dehaene and Brian Butterworth as a more fundamental inability to conceptualize numbers as abstract concepts of comparative quantities (a deficit in "number sense"),
which these researchers consider to be a foundational skill upon which
other mathematics abilities build. Symptoms of dyscalculia include the
delay of simple counting, inability to memorize simple arithmetic facts
such as adding, subtracting, etc. There are few known symptoms because
little research has been done on the topic.
Etymology
The term dyscalculia dates back to at least 1949.
Dyscalculia comes from Greek and Latin and means "counting badly". The prefix "dys-" comes from Greek and means "badly". The root "calculia" comes from the Latin "calculare", which means "to count" and which is also related to "calculation" and "calculus".