An argument from authority (argumentum ab auctoritate), also called an appeal to authority, or argumentum ad verecundiam, is a form of defeasible argument in which the opinion of an authority on a topic is used as evidence to support an argument. It is well known as a fallacy,
though some consider that it is used in a cogent form when all sides of
a discussion agree on the reliability of the authority in the given
context. Other authors consider it a fallacy to cite an authority on the discussed topic as the primary means of supporting an argument.
Forms
Appeals to authorities
Historically,
opinion on the appeal to authority has been divided: it is listed as a
valid argument as often as a fallacious argument in various sources, with some holding that it is a strong or at least valid argument and others that it is weak or an outright fallacy.
If all parties agree on the reliability of an authority in the given context it forms a valid inductive argument.
Use in science
Scientific knowledge is best established by evidence and experiment rather than argued through authority as authority has no place in science. Carl Sagan wrote of arguments from authority:
One of the great commandments of science is, "Mistrust arguments from authority." ... Too many such arguments have proved too painfully wrong. Authorities must prove their contentions like everybody else.
One example of the use of the appeal to authority in science dates to 1923, when leading American zoologist Theophilus Painter declared, based on poor data and conflicting observations he had made, that humans had 24 pairs of chromosomes. From the 1920s until 1956, scientists propagated this "fact" based on Painter's authority, despite subsequent counts totaling the correct number of 23. Even textbooks with photos showing 23 pairs incorrectly declared the number to be 24 based on the authority of the then-consensus of 24 pairs.
This seemingly established number generated confirmation bias among researchers, and "most cytologists, expecting to detect Painter's number, virtually always did so". Painter's "influence was so great that many scientists preferred to believe his count over the actual evidence", and scientists who obtained the accurate number modified or discarded their data to agree with Painter's count.
A more recent example involved the "When contact changes minds: An experiment on transmission of support for gay equality"
paper, published in 2014. The paper was a fraud based on forged data,
yet concerns about it were ignored in many cases due to appeals to
authority. One analysis of the affair notes that "Over and over again,
throughout the scientific community and the media, LaCour’s
impossible-seeming results were treated as truth, in part because of the
weight [the study's co-author] Green's name carried".
One psychologist stated his reaction to the paper was "that's very
surprising and doesn't fit with a huge literature of evidence. It
doesn't sound plausible to me... [then I pull it up and] I see Don Green
is an author. I trust him completely, so I'm no longer doubtful". The
forger, LaCour, would use appeals to authority to defend his research:
"if his responses sometimes seemed to lack depth when he was pressed for
details, his impressive connections often allayed concerns", with one
of his partners stating "when he and I really had a disagreement, he
would often rely on the kind of arguments where he’d basically invoke
authority, right? He's the one with advanced training, and his adviser
is this very high-powered, very experienced person...and they know a lot
more than we do".
Much like the erroneous chromosome number taking decades to
refute until microscopy made the error unmistakable, the one who would
go on to debunk this paper "was consistently told by friends and
advisers to keep quiet about his concerns lest he earn a reputation as a
troublemaker", up until "the very last moment when multiple 'smoking
guns' finally appeared", and he found that "There was almost no
encouragement for him to probe the hints of weirdness he’d uncovered".
Appeals to non-authorities
Fallacious arguments from authority are also frequently the result of citing a non-authority as an authority. The philosophers Irving Copi and Carl Cohen
characterized it as a fallacy "when the appeal is made to parties
having no legitimate claim to authority in the matter at hand".
An example of the fallacy of appealing to an authority in an unrelated field would be citing Albert Einstein as an authority for a determination on religion when his primary expertise was in physics.
It is also a fallacious ad hominem argument to argue that a person presenting statements lacks authority and thus their arguments do not need to be considered.
As appeals to a perceived lack of authority, these types of argument
are fallacious for much the same reasons as an appeal to authority.
Other related fallacious arguments assume that a person without status or authority is inherently reliable. For instance, the appeal to poverty is the fallacy of thinking that someone is more likely to be correct because they are poor.
When an argument holds that a conclusion is likely to be true precisely
because the one who holds or is presenting it lacks authority, it is a
fallacious appeal to the common man.
Cognitive bias
The
argument from authority is based on the idea that a perceived authority
must know better and that the person should conform to their opinion.
This has its roots in psychological cognitive biases such as the Asch effect. In repeated and modified instances of the Asch conformity experiments,
it was found that high-status individuals create a stronger likelihood
of a subject agreeing with an obviously false conclusion, despite the
subject normally being able to clearly see that the answer was
incorrect.
Further, humans have been shown to feel strong emotional pressure
to conform to authorities and majority positions. A repeat of the
experiments by another group of researchers found that "Participants
reported considerable distress
under the group pressure", with 59% conforming at least once and
agreeing with the clearly incorrect answer, whereas the incorrect answer
was much more rarely given when no such pressures were present.
Another study shining light on the psychological basis of the fallacy as it relates to perceived authorities are the Milgram experiments, which demonstrated that people are more likely to go along with something when it is presented by an authority.
In a variation of a study where the researchers did not wear lab coats,
thus reducing the perceived authority of the tasker, the obedience
level dropped to 20% from the original rate, which had been higher than
50%. Obedience is encouraged by reminding the individual of what a
perceived authority states and by showing them that their opinion goes
against this authority.
Scholars have noted that certain environments can produce an ideal situation for these processes to take hold, giving rise to groupthink.
In groupthink, individuals in a group feel inclined to minimize
conflict and encourage conformity. Through an appeal to authority, a
group member might present that opinion as a consensus and encourage the
other group members to engage in groupthink by not disagreeing with
this perceived consensus or authority. One paper about the philosophy of mathematics notes that, within academia,
If...a person accepts our discipline, and goes through two or three years of graduate study in mathematics, he absorbs our way of thinking, and is no longer the critical outsider he once was...If the student is unable to absorb our way of thinking, we flunk him out, of course. If he gets through our obstacle course and then decides that our arguments are unclear or incorrect, we dismiss him as a crank, crackpot, or misfit.
Corporate environments are similarly vulnerable to appeals to perceived authorities and experts leading to groupthink, as are governments and militaries.