According to the historical use of the term, people tend to
select specific aspects of exposed information which they incorporate
into their mindset. These selections are made based on their
perspectives, beliefs, attitudes and decisions.
People can mentally dissect the information they are exposed to and
select favorable evidence, while ignoring the unfavorable. The
foundation of this theory is rooted in the cognitive dissonance theory, which asserts that when individuals are confronted with contrasting ideas, certain mental defense mechanisms
are activated to produce harmony between new ideas and pre-existing
beliefs, which results in cognitive equilibrium. Cognitive equilibrium,
which is defined as a state of balance between a person's mental
representation of the world and his or her environment, is crucial to
understanding selective exposure theory. According to Jean Piaget, when a mismatch occurs, people find it to be "inherently dissatisfying".
Selective exposure relies on the assumption that one will continue to seek out information on an issue even after an individual has taken a stance on it. The position that a person has taken will be colored by various factors of that issue that are reinforced during the decision-making process.
Selective exposure has been displayed in various contexts such as self-serving situations and situations in which people hold prejudices regarding outgroups, particular opinions, and personal and group-related issues. Perceived usefulness of information, perceived norm of fairness, and curiosity of valuable information are three factors that can counteract selective exposure.
Selective exposure relies on the assumption that one will continue to seek out information on an issue even after an individual has taken a stance on it. The position that a person has taken will be colored by various factors of that issue that are reinforced during the decision-making process.
Selective exposure has been displayed in various contexts such as self-serving situations and situations in which people hold prejudices regarding outgroups, particular opinions, and personal and group-related issues. Perceived usefulness of information, perceived norm of fairness, and curiosity of valuable information are three factors that can counteract selective exposure.
Effect on decision-making
Individual versus group decision-making
Selective exposure can affect the decisions people make as
individuals or as groups because they may be unwilling to change their
views and beliefs either collectively or on their own. A historical
example of the disastrous effects of selective exposure and its effects
on group dynamics is the series of events leading up to the Bay of Pigs Invasion in 1961. President John F. Kennedy
was given the go ahead by his advisers to authorize the invasion of
Cuba by poorly trained expatriates despite overwhelming evidence that it
was a foolish and ill-conceived tactical maneuver. The advisers were so
eager to please the President that they confirmed their cognitive bias
for the invasion rather than challenging the faulty plan. Changing beliefs about one's self, other people, and the world are three variables as to why people fear new information.
A variety of studies has shown that selective exposure effects can
occur in the context of both individual and group decision making. Numerous situational variables have been identified that increase the tendency toward selective exposure. Social psychology,
specifically, includes research with a variety of situational factors
and related psychological processes that eventually persuade a person to
make a quality decision. Additionally, from a psychological
perspective, the effects of selective exposure can both stem from
motivational and cognitive accounts.
Effect of information quantity
According
to research study by Fischer, Schulz-Hardt, et al. (2008), the quantity
of decision-relevant information that the participants were exposed to
had a significant effect on their levels of selective exposure. A group
for which only two pieces of decision-relevant information were given
had experienced lower levels of selective exposure than the other group
who had ten pieces of information to evaluate. This research brought
more attention to the cognitive processes of individuals when they are
presented with a very small amount of decision-consistent and
decision-inconsistent information. The study showed that in situations
such as this, an individual becomes more doubtful of their initial
decision due to the unavailability of resources. They begin to think
that there is not enough data or evidence in this particular field in
which they are told to make a decision about. Because of this, the
subject becomes more critical of their initial thought process and
focuses on both decision-consistent and inconsistent sources, thus
decreasing his level of selective exposure. For the group who had
plentiful pieces of information, this factor made them confident in
their initial decision because they felt comfort from the fact that
their decision topic was well-supported by a large number of resources.
Therefore, the availability of decision-relevant and irrelevant
information surrounding individuals can influence the level of selective
exposure experienced during the process of decision-making.
Selective exposure is prevalent within singular individuals and
groups of people and can influence either to reject new ideas or
information that is not commensurate with the original ideal. In Jonas
et al. (2001) empirical studies were done on four different experiments
investigating individuals' and groups' decision making. This article
suggests that confirmation bias
is prevalent in decision making. Those who find new information often
draw their attention towards areas where they hold personal attachment.
Thus, people are driven toward pieces of information that are coherent
with their own expectations or beliefs as a result of this selective
exposure theory occurring in action. Throughout the process of the four
experiments, generalization is always considered valid and confirmation
bias is always present when seeking new information and making
decisions.
Accuracy motivation and defense motivation
Fischer and Greitemeyer (2010) explored individuals' decision making in terms of selective exposure to confirmatory information.
Selective exposure posed that individuals make their decisions based on
information that is consistent with their decision rather than
information that is inconsistent. Recent research has shown that
"Confirmatory Information Search" was responsible for the 2008
bankruptcy of the Lehman Brothers Investment Bank which then triggered the Global Financial Crisis.
In the zeal for profit and economic gain, politicians, investors and
financial advisors ignored the mathematical evidence that foretold the
housing market crash in favor of flimsy justifications for upholding the
status quo.
Researchers explain that subjects have the tendency to seek and select
information using their integrative model. There are two primary
motivations for selective exposure: Accuracy Motivation and Defense
Motivation. Accuracy Motivation explains that an individual is motivated
to be accurate in their decision making and Defense Motivation explains
that one seeks confirmatory information to support their beliefs and
justify their decisions. Accuracy motivation is not always beneficial
within the context of selective exposure and can instead be
counterintuitive, increasing the amount of selective exposure. Defense
motivation can lead to reduced levels of selective exposure.
Personal attributes
Selective exposure avoids information inconsistent with one's beliefs and attitudes. For example, former Vice President Dick Cheney would only enter a hotel room after the television was turned on and tuned to a conservative television channel. When analyzing a person's decision-making
skills, his or her unique process of gathering relevant information is
not the only factor taken into account. Fischer et al. (2010) found it
important to consider the information source itself, otherwise explained
as the physical being that provided the source of information.
Selective exposure research generally neglects the influence of
indirect decision-related attributes, such as physical appearance. In
Fischer et al. (2010) two studies hypothesized that physically
attractive information sources resulted in decision makers to be more
selective in searching and reviewing decision-relevant information.
Researchers explored the impact of social information and its level of
physical attractiveness. The data was then analyzed and used to support
the idea that selective exposure existed for those who needed to make a
decision.
Therefore, the more attractive an information source was, the more
positive and detailed the subject was with making the decision. Physical
attractiveness affects an individual's decision because the perception
of quality improves. Physically attractive information sources
increased the quality of consistent information needed to make decisions
and further increased the selective exposure in decision-relevant
information, supporting the researchers' hypothesis.
Both studies concluded that attractiveness is driven by a different
selection and evaluation of decision-consistent information. Decision
makers allow factors such as physical attractiveness to affect everyday
decisions due to the works of selective exposure.
In another study, selective exposure was defined by the amount of
individual confidence. Individuals can control the amount of selective
exposure depending on whether they have a low self-esteem or high
self-esteem. Individuals who maintain higher confidence levels reduce
the amount of selective exposure.
Albarracín and Mitchell (2004) hypothesized that those who displayed
higher confidence levels were more willing to seek out information both
consistent and inconsistent with their views. The phrase
"decision-consistent information" explains the tendency to actively seek
decision-relevant information. Selective exposure occurs when
individuals search for information and show systematic preferences
towards ideas that are consistent, rather than inconsistent, with their
beliefs.
On the contrary, those who exhibited low levels of confidence were more
inclined to examine information that did not agree with their views.
The researchers found that in three out of five studies participants
showed more confidence and scored higher on the Defensive Confidence Scale, which serves as evidence that their hypothesis was correct.
Bozo et al. (2009) investigated the anxiety of fearing death and
compared it to various age groups in relation to health-promoting
behaviors. Researchers analyzed the data by using the terror management theory
and found that age had no direct effect on specific behaviors. The
researchers thought that a fear of death would yield health-promoting
behaviors in young adults. When individuals are reminded of their own
death, it causes stress and anxiety, but eventually leads to positive
changes in their health behaviors. Their conclusions showed that older
adults were consistently better at promoting and practicing good health
behaviors, without thinking about death, compared to young adults.
Young adults were less motivated to change and practice
health-promoting behaviors because they used the selective exposure to
confirm their prior beliefs. Selective exposure thus creates barriers
between the behaviors in different ages, but there is no specific age at
which people change their behaviors.
Though physical appearance will impact one's personal decision
regarding an idea presented, a study conducted by Van Dillen, Papies,
and Hofmann (2013) suggests a way to decrease the influence of personal
attributes and selective exposure on decision-making.
The results from this study showed that people do pay more attention to
physically attractive or tempting stimuli; however, this phenomenon can
be decreased through increasing the "cognitive load." In this study,
increasing cognitive activity led to a decreased impact of physical
appearance and selective exposure on the individual's impression of the
idea presented. This is explained by acknowledging that we are
instinctively drawn to certain physical attributes, but if the required
resources for this attraction are otherwise engaged at the time, then we
might not notice these attributes to an equal extent. For example, if a
person is simultaneously engaging in a mentally challenging activity
during the time of exposure, then it is likely that less attention will
be paid to appearance, which leads to a decreased impact of selective
exposure on decision-making.
Theories accounting for selective exposure
Cognitive dissonance theory
Leon Festinger
is widely considered as the father of modern social psychology and as
an important figure to that field of practice as Freud was to clinical
psychology and Piaget was to developmental psychology.
He was considered to be one of the most significant social
psychologists of the 20th century. His work demonstrated that it is
possible to use the scientific method to investigate complex and
significant social phenomena without reducing them to the mechanistic
connections between stimulus and response that were the basis of behaviorism. Festinger proposed the groundbreaking theory of cognitive dissonance
that has become the foundation of selective exposure theory today
despite the fact that Festinger was considered as an "avant-garde"
psychologist when he had first proposed it in 1957.
In an ironic twist, Festinger realized that he himself was a victim of
the effects of selective exposure. He was a heavy smoker his entire
life and when he was diagnosed with terminal cancer in 1989, he was said
to have joked, "Make sure that everyone knows that it wasn't lung
cancer!"
Cognitive dissonance theory explains that when a person either
consciously or unconsciously realizes conflicting attitudes, thoughts,
or beliefs, they experience mental discomfort. Because of this, an
individual will avoid such conflicting information in the future since
it produces this discomfort, and they will gravitate towards messages
sympathetic to their own previously held conceptions.
Decision makers are unable to evaluate information quality
independently on their own (Fischer, Jonas, Dieter & Kastenmüller,
2008).
When there is a conflict between pre-existing views and information
encountered, individuals will experience an unpleasant and
self-threatening state of aversive-arousal which will motivate them to
reduce it through selective exposure. They will begin to prefer
information that supports their original decision and neglect
conflicting information. Individuals will then exhibit confirmatory
information to defend their positions and reach the goal of dissonance
reduction. Cognitive dissonance theory insists that dissonance is a psychological state of tension that people are motivated to reduce (Festinger 1957). Dissonance causes feelings of unhappiness, discomfort, or distress. Festinger (1957,
p. 13) asserted the following: "These two elements are in a dissonant
relation if, considering these two alone, the obverse of one element
would follow from the other." To reduce dissonance, people add consonant
cognition or change evaluations for one or both conditions in order to
make them more consistent mentally.
In Festinger's theory, there are two basic hypotheses:
- The existence of dissonance, being psychologically uncomfortable, will motivate the person to try to reduce the dissonance and achieve consonance.
- When dissonance is present, in addition to trying to reduce it, the person will actively avoid situations and information which would likely increase the dissonance (Festinger 1957, p. 3). The theory of cognitive dissonance was developed in the mid-1950s to explain why people of strong convictions are so resistant in changing their beliefs even in the face of undeniable contradictory evidence. It occurs when people feel an attachment to and responsibility for a decision, position or behavior. It increases the motivation to justify their positions through selective exposure to confirmatory information (Fischer, 2011). Fischer suggested that people have an inner need to ensure that their beliefs and behaviors are consistent. In an experiment that employed commitment manipulations, it impacts perceived decision certainty. Participants were free to choose attitude-consistent and inconsistent information to write an essay. Those who wrote an attitude-consistent essay showed higher levels of confirmatory information search (Fischer, 2011). The levels and magnitude of dissonance also play a role. Selective exposure to consistent information is likely under certain levels of dissonance. At high levels, a person is expected to seek out information that increases dissonance because the best strategy to reduce dissonance would be to alter one's attitude or decision (Smith et al., 2008).
Subsequent research on selective exposure within the dissonance
theory produced weak empirical support until the dissonance theory was
revised and new methods, more conducive to measuring selective exposure,
were implemented.
To date, scholars still argue that empirical results supporting the
selective exposure hypothesis are still mixed. This is possibly due to
the problems with the methods of the experimental studies conducted. Another possible reason for the mixed results may be the failure to simulate an authentic media environment in the experiments.
According to Festinger, the motivation to seek or avoid
information depends on the magnitude of dissonance experienced (Smith et
al., 2008).
It is observed that there is a tendency for people to seek new
information or select information that supports their beliefs in order
to reduce dissonance.
There exist three possibilities which will affect extent of dissonance (Festinger 1957, pp. 127–131):
- Relative absence of dissonance.
When little or no dissonance exists, there is little or no motivation
to seek new information. For example, when there is an absence of
dissonance, the lack of motivation to attend or avoid a lecture on 'The
Advantages of Automobiles with Very High Horsepower Engines' will be
independent of whether the car a new owner has recently purchased has a
high or low horsepower engine. However, it is important to note the
difference between a situation when there is no dissonance and when the
information has no relevance to the present or future behavior. For the
latter, accidental exposure, which the new car owner does not avoid,
will not introduce any dissonance; while for the former individual, who
also does not avoid information, dissonance may be accidentally
introduced.
- The presence of moderate amounts of dissonance.
The existence of dissonance and consequent pressure to reduce it will
lead to an active search of information, which will then lead people to
avoid information that will increase dissonance. However, when faced
with a potential source of information, there will be an ambiguous
cognition to which a subject will react in terms of individual
expectations about it. If the subject expects the cognition to increase
dissonance, they will avoid it. In the event that one's expectations are
proven wrong, the attempt at dissonance reduction may result in
increasing it instead. It may in turn lead to a situation of active
avoidance.
- The presence of extremely large amounts of dissonance.
If two cognitive elements exist in a dissonant relationship, the
magnitude of dissonance matches the resistance to change. If the
dissonance becomes greater than the resistance to change, then the least
resistant elements of cognition will be changed, reducing dissonance.
When dissonance is close to the maximum limit, one may actively seek out
and expose oneself to dissonance-increasing information. If an
individual can increase dissonance to the point where it is greater than
the resistance to change, he will change the cognitive elements
involved, reducing or even eliminating dissonance. Once dissonance is
increased sufficiently, an individual may bring himself to change, hence
eliminating all dissonance (Festinger 1957, pp. 127–131).
The reduction in cognitive dissonance
following a decision can be achieved by selectively looking for
decision-consonant information and avoiding contradictory information.
The objective is to reduce the discrepancy between the cognitions, but
the specification of which strategy will be chosen is not explicitly
addressed by the dissonance theory. It will be dependent on the quantity
and quality of the information available inside and outside the
cognitive system.
Klapper's selective exposure
In the early 1960s, Columbia University researcher Joseph T. Klapper asserted in his book The Effects Of Mass Communication
that audiences were not passive targets of political and commercial
propaganda from mass media but that mass media reinforces previously
held convictions. Throughout the book, he argued that the media has a
small amount of power to influence people and, most of the time, it just
reinforces our preexisting attitudes and beliefs. He argued that the
media effects of relaying or spreading new public messages or ideas were
minimal because there is a wide variety of ways in which individuals
filter such content. Due to this tendency, Klapper argued that media
content must be able to ignite some type of cognitive activity in an
individual in order to communicate its message.
Prior to Klapper's research, the prevailing opinion was that mass media
had a substantial power to sway individual opinion and that audiences
were passive consumers of prevailing media propaganda. However, by the time of the release of The Effects of Mass Communication,
many studies led to a conclusion that many specifically targeted
messages were completely ineffective. Klapper's research showed that
individuals gravitated towards media messages that bolstered previously
held convictions that were set by peer groups, societal influences, and
family structures and that the accession of these messages over time did
not change when presented with more recent media influence. Klapper
noted from the review of research in the social science that given the
abundance of content within the mass media, audiences were selective to
the types of programming that they consumed. Adults would patronize
media that was appropriate for their demographics and children would
eschew media that was boring to them. So individuals would either
accept or reject a mass media message based upon internal filters that
were innate to that person.
The following are Klapper's five mediating factors and conditions to affect people:
- Predispositions and the related processes of selective exposure, selective perception, and selective retention.
- The groups, and the norms of groups, to which the audience members belong.
- Interpersonal dissemination of the content of communication
- The exercise of opinion leadership
- The nature of mass media in a free enterprise society.
Three basic concepts:
- Selective exposure – people keep away from communication of opposite hue.
- Selective perception – If people are confronting unsympathetic material, they do not perceive it, or make it fit for their existing opinion.
- Selective retention – refers to the process of categorizing and interpreting information in a way that favors one category or interpretation over another. Furthermore, they just simply forget the unsympathetic material.
Groups and group norms work as mediators. For example, one can be
strongly disinclined to change to the Democratic Party if their family
has voted Republican for a long time. In this case, the person's
predisposition to the political party is already set, so they don't
perceive information about Democratic Party or change voting behavior
because of mass communication.
Klapper's third assumption is inter-personal dissemination of mass
communication. If someone is already exposed by close friends, which
creates predisposition toward something, it will lead to an increase in
exposure to mass communication and eventually reinforce the existing
opinion. An opinion leader
is also a crucial factor to form one's predisposition and can lead
someone to be exposed by mass communication. The nature of commercial
mass media also leads people to select certain types of media contents.
Cognitive economy model
This
new model combines the motivational and cognitive processes of
selective exposure. In the past, selective exposure had been studied
from a motivational standpoint. For instance, the reason behind the
existence of selective exposure was that people felt motivated to
decrease the level of dissonance they felt while encountering
inconsistent information. They also felt motivated to defend their
decisions and positions, so they achieved this goal by exposing
themselves to consistent information only. However, the new cognitive
economy model not only takes into account the motivational aspects, but
it also focuses on the cognitive processes of each individual. For
instance, this model proposes that people cannot evaluate the quality of
inconsistent information objectively and fairly because they tend to
store more of the consistent information and use this as their reference
point. Thus, inconsistent information is often observed with a more
critical eye in comparison to consistent information. According to this
model, the levels of selective exposure experienced during the
decision-making process are also dependent on how much cognitive energy
people are willing to invest. Just as people tend to be careful with
their finances, cognitive energy or how much time they are willing to
spend evaluating all the evidence for their decisions works the same
way. People are hesitant to use this energy; they tend to be careful so
they don't waste it. Thus, this model suggests that selective exposure
does not happen in separate stages. Rather, it is a combined process of
the individuals' certain acts of motivations and their management of the
cognitive energy.
Implications
Media
Recent studies have shown relevant empirical evidence for the
pervasive influence of selective exposure on the greater population at
large due to mass media.
Researchers have found that individual media consumers will seek out
programs to suit their individual emotional and cognitive needs.
Individuals will seek out palliative forms of media during the recent
times of economic crisis to fulfill a "strong surveillance need" and to
decrease chronic dissatisfaction with life circumstances as well as
fulfill needs for companionship.
Consumers tend to select media content that exposes and confirms their
own ideas while avoiding information that argues against their opinion. A
study conducted in 2012 has shown that this type of selective exposure
affects pornography consumption as well. Individuals with low levels of life satisfaction
are more likely to have casual sex after consumption of pornography
that is congruent with their attitudes while disregarding content that
challenges their inherently permissive 'no strings attached' attitudes.
Music selection is also affected by selective exposure. A 2014
study conducted by Christa L. Taylor and Ronald S. Friedman at the SUNY
University at Albany, found that mood congruence was effected by
self-regulation of music mood choices. Subjects in the study chose
happy music when feeling angry or neutral but listened to sad music when
they themselves were sad. The choice of sad music given a sad mood was
due less to mood-mirroring but as a result of subjects having an
aversion to listening to happy music that was cognitively dissonant with
their mood.
Politics are more likely to inspire selective exposure among
consumers as opposed to single exposure decisions. For example, in their
2009 meta-analysis of Selective Exposure Theory, Hart et al. reported
that "A 2004 survey by The Pew Research Center for the People & the
Press (2006) found that Republicans are about 1.5 times more likely to
report watching Fox News
regularly than are Democrats (34% for Republicans and 20% of
Democrats). In contrast, Democrats are 1.5 times more likely to report
watching CNN
regularly than Republicans (28% of Democrats vs. 19% of Republicans).
Even more striking, Republicans are approximately five times more likely
than Democrats to report watching "The O'Reilly Factor" regularly and are seven times more likely to report listening to "Rush Limbaugh" regularly."
As a result, when the opinions of Republicans who only tune into
conservative media outlets were compared to those of their fellow
conservatives in a study by Stroud (2010), their beliefs were considered
to be more polarized. The same result was retrieved from the study of
liberals as well.
Due to our greater tendency toward selective exposure, current
political campaigns have been characterized as being extremely partisan
and polarized. As Bennett and Iyengar (2008) commented, "The new, more
diversified information environment makes it not only more feasible for
consumers to seek out news they might find agreeable but also provides a
strong economic incentive for news organizations to cater to their
viewers' political preferences." Selective exposure thus plays a role in shaping and reinforcing individuals' political attitudes.
In early research, selective exposure originally provided an
explanation for limited media effects. The "limited effects" model of
communication emerged in the 1940s with a shift in the media effects
paradigm. This shift suggested that while the media has effects on
consumers' behavior such as their voting behavior, these effects are
limited and influenced indirectly by interpersonal discussions and the
influence of opinion leaders.
Selective exposure was considered one necessary function in the early
studies of media's limited power over citizens' attitudes and behaviors.
Political ads deal with selective exposure as well because people are
more likely to favor a politician that agrees with their own beliefs.
Another significant effect of selective exposure comes from Stroud
(2010) who analyzed the relationship between partisan selective exposure
and political polarization. Using data from the 2004 National Annenberg Election Survey, analysts found that over time partisan selective exposure leads to polarization.
This process is plausible because people can easily create or have
access to blogs, websites, chats, and online forums where those with
similar views and political ideologies can congregate. Much of the
research has also shown that political interaction online tends to be
polarized. Further evidence for this polarization in the political
blogosphere can be found in the Lawrence et al. (2010)'s
study on blog readership that people tend to read blogs that reinforce
rather than challenge their political beliefs. According to Cass
Sunstein's book, Republic.com, the presence of selective exposure
on the web creates an environment that breeds political polarization
and extremism. Due to easy access to social media and other online
resources, people are " likely to hold even stronger views than the ones
they started with, and when these views are problematic, they are
likely to manifest increasing hatred toward those espousing contrary
beliefs."
This illustrates how selective exposure can influence an individual's
political beliefs and subsequently his participation in the political
system.
One of the major academic debates on the concept of selective
exposure is whether selective exposure contributes to people's exposure
to diverse viewpoints or polarization. Scheufele and Nisbet (2012)
discuss the effects of encountering disagreement on democratic
citizenship. Ideally, true civil deliberation among citizens would be
the rational exchange of non-like-minded views (or disagreement).
However, many of us tend to avoid disagreement on a regular basis
because we do not like to confront with others who hold views that are
strongly opposed to our own. In this sense, the authors question about
whether exposure to non-like-minded information brings either positive
or negative effects on democratic citizenship. While there are mixed
findings of peoples' willingness to participate in the political
processes when they encounter disagreement, the authors argue that the
issue of selectivity needs to be further examined in order to understand
whether there is a truly deliberative discourse in online media
environment.