The unconscious mind (or the unconscious) consists of the processes in the mind which occur automatically and are not available to introspection and include thought processes, memories, interests and motivations.
Even though these processes exist well under the surface of conscious awareness, they are theorized to exert an impact on behavior. The term was coined by the 18th-century German Romantic philosopher Friedrich Schelling and later introduced into English by the poet and essayist Samuel Taylor Coleridge.
Empirical evidence suggests that unconscious phenomena include repressed feelings, automatic skills, subliminal perceptions, and automatic reactions, and possibly also complexes, hidden phobias and desires.
The concept was popularized by the Austrian neurologist and psychoanalyst Sigmund Freud. In psychoanalytic theory, unconscious processes are understood to be directly represented in dreams, as well as in slips of the tongue and jokes.
Thus the unconscious mind can be seen as the source of dreams and automatic thoughts (those that appear without any apparent cause), the repository of forgotten memories (that may still be accessible to consciousness at some later time), and the locus of implicit knowledge (the things that we have learned so well that we do them without thinking).
It has been argued that consciousness is influenced by other parts of the mind. These include unconsciousness as a personal habit, being unaware and intuition. Phenomena related to semi-consciousness include awakening, implicit memory, subliminal messages, trances, hypnagogia and hypnosis. While sleep, sleepwalking, dreaming, delirium and comas may signal the presence of unconscious processes, these processes are seen as symptoms rather than the unconscious mind itself.
Some critics have doubted the existence of the unconscious.
Even though these processes exist well under the surface of conscious awareness, they are theorized to exert an impact on behavior. The term was coined by the 18th-century German Romantic philosopher Friedrich Schelling and later introduced into English by the poet and essayist Samuel Taylor Coleridge.
Empirical evidence suggests that unconscious phenomena include repressed feelings, automatic skills, subliminal perceptions, and automatic reactions, and possibly also complexes, hidden phobias and desires.
The concept was popularized by the Austrian neurologist and psychoanalyst Sigmund Freud. In psychoanalytic theory, unconscious processes are understood to be directly represented in dreams, as well as in slips of the tongue and jokes.
Thus the unconscious mind can be seen as the source of dreams and automatic thoughts (those that appear without any apparent cause), the repository of forgotten memories (that may still be accessible to consciousness at some later time), and the locus of implicit knowledge (the things that we have learned so well that we do them without thinking).
It has been argued that consciousness is influenced by other parts of the mind. These include unconsciousness as a personal habit, being unaware and intuition. Phenomena related to semi-consciousness include awakening, implicit memory, subliminal messages, trances, hypnagogia and hypnosis. While sleep, sleepwalking, dreaming, delirium and comas may signal the presence of unconscious processes, these processes are seen as symptoms rather than the unconscious mind itself.
Some critics have doubted the existence of the unconscious.
Historical overview
The term "unconscious" (German: Unbewusste) was coined by the 18th-century German Romantic philosopher Friedrich Schelling (in his System of Transcendental Idealism, ch. 6, § 3) and later introduced into English by the poet and essayist Samuel Taylor Coleridge (in his Biographia Literaria). Some rare earlier instances of the term "unconsciousness" (Unbewußtseyn) can be found in the work of the 18th-century German physician and philosopher Ernst Platner.
Influences on thinking that originate from outside of an
individual's consciousness were reflected in the ancient ideas of
temptation, divine inspiration, and the predominant role of the gods in
affecting motives and actions. The idea of internalised unconscious
processes in the mind was also instigated in antiquity and has been
explored across a wide variety of cultures. Unconscious aspects of
mentality were referred to between 2,500 and 600 BC in the Hindu texts
known as the Vedas, found today in Ayurvedic medicine.
Paracelsus is credited as the first to make mention of an unconscious aspect of cognition in his work Von den Krankheiten
(translates as "About illnesses", 1567), and his clinical methodology
created a cogent system that is regarded by some as the beginning of
modern scientific psychology. William Shakespeare explored the role of the unconscious in many of his plays, without naming it as such. In addition, Western philosophers such as Arthur Schopenhauer, Baruch Spinoza, Gottfried Leibniz, Johann Gottlieb Fichte, Georg Wilhelm Friedrich Hegel, Eduard von Hartmann, Søren Kierkegaard, and Friedrich Nietzsche used the word unconscious.
In 1880, Edmond Colsenet supports at the Sorbonne, a philosophy thesis on the unconscious. Elie Rabier and Alfred Fouillee perform syntheses of the unconscious "at a time when Freud was not interested in the concept".
Psychology
Psychologist
Jacques Van Rillaer points out that, "the unconscious was not
discovered by Freud. In 1890, when psychoanalysis was still unheard of, William James, in his monumental treatise on psychology (The Principles of Psychology), examined the way Schopenhauer, von Hartmann, Janet, Binet and others had used the term 'unconscious' and 'subconscious'".
Historian of psychology Mark Altschule observes that, "It is
difficult—or perhaps impossible—to find a nineteenth-century
psychologist or psychiatrist who did not recognize unconscious
cerebration as not only real but of the highest importance."
Eduard von Hartmann published a book dedicated to the topic, Philosophy of the Unconscious, in 1869.
Furthermore, 19th century German psychologists, Gustav Fechner and Wilhelm Wundt, had begun to use the term in their experimental psychology, in the context of manifold, jumbled sense data that the mind organizes at an unconscious level before revealing it as a cogent totality in conscious form."[24]
Freud's view
Sigmund Freud and his followers developed an account of the unconscious mind. It plays an important role in psychoanalysis.
Freud divided the mind into the conscious mind (or the ego) and the unconscious mind. The latter was then further divided into the id (or instincts and drive) and the superego (or conscience). In this theory, the unconscious refers to the mental processes of which individuals make themselves unaware. Freud proposed a vertical and hierarchical architecture of human consciousness: the conscious mind, the preconscious, and the unconscious mind—each
lying beneath the other. He believed that significant psychic events
take place "below the surface" in the unconscious mind, like hidden messages from the unconscious. He interpreted such events as having both symbolic and actual significance.
In psychoanalytic terms, the unconscious does not include all
that is not conscious, but rather what is actively repressed from
conscious thought or what a person is averse to knowing consciously.
Freud viewed the unconscious as a repository for socially unacceptable
ideas, wishes or desires, traumatic memories, and painful emotions put
out of mind by the mechanism of psychological repression.
However, the contents did not necessarily have to be solely negative.
In the psychoanalytic view, the unconscious is a force that can only be
recognized by its effects—it expresses itself in the symptom.
In a sense, this view places the conscious self as an adversary to its
unconscious, warring to keep the unconscious hidden. Unconscious
thoughts are not directly accessible to ordinary introspection, but are supposed to be capable of being "tapped" and "interpreted" by special methods and techniques such as meditation, free association (a method largely introduced by Freud), dream analysis, and verbal slips (commonly known as a Freudian slip), examined and conducted during psychoanalysis.
Seeing as these unconscious thoughts are normally cryptic,
psychoanalysts are considered experts in interpreting their messages.
Freud based his concept of the unconscious on a variety of
observations. For example, he considered "slips of the tongue" to be
related to the unconscious in that they often appeared to show a
person's true feelings on a subject. For example, "I decided to take a
summer curse". This example shows a slip of the word "course" where the
speaker accidentally used the word curse which would show that they have
negative feelings about having to do this. Freud noticed that also his
patient's dreams expressed important feelings they were unaware of.
After these observations, he came to the conclusion that psychological
disturbances are largely caused by personal conflicts existing at the
unconscious level. His psychoanalytic theory acts to explain
personality, motivation and mental disorders by focusing on unconscious determinants of behavior.
Freud later used his notion of the unconscious in order to explain certain kinds of neurotic behavior. The theory of the unconscious was substantially transformed by later psychiatrists, among them Carl Jung and Jacques Lacan.
In his 1932/1933 conferences, Freud "proposes to abandon the notion of the unconscious that ambiguous judge".
Jung's view
Carl Gustav Jung, a Swiss psychiatrist,
developed the concept further. He agreed with Freud that the
unconscious is a determinant of personality, but he proposed that the
unconscious be divided into two layers: the personal unconscious and the collective unconscious.
The personal unconscious is a reservoir of material that was once
conscious but has been forgotten or suppressed, much like Freud's
notion. The collective unconscious, however, is the deepest level of the
psyche, containing the accumulation of inherited psychic structures and
archetypal
experiences. Archetypes are not memories but energy centers or
psychological functions that are apparent in the culture's use of
symbols. The collective unconscious is therefore said to be inherited
and contain material of an entire species rather than of an individual.
Every person shares the collective unconscious with the entire human
species, as Jung puts it: "[the] whole spiritual heritage of mankind's
evolution, born anew in the brain structure of every individual".
In addition to the structure of the unconscious, Jung differed from Freud in that he did not believe that sexuality was at the base of all unconscious thoughts.
Controversy
The notion that the unconscious mind exists at all has been disputed.
Franz Brentano rejected the concept of the unconscious in his 1874 book Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint, although his rejection followed largely from his definitions of consciousness and unconsciousness.
Jean-Paul Sartre offers a critique of Freud's theory of the unconscious in Being and Nothingness,
based on the claim that consciousness is essentially self-conscious.
Sartre also argues that Freud's theory of repression is internally
flawed. Philosopher Thomas Baldwin argues that Sartre's argument is based on a misunderstanding of Freud.
Erich Fromm
contends that, "The term 'the unconscious' is actually a mystification
(even though one might use it for reasons of convenience, as I am guilty
of doing in these pages). There is no such thing as the unconscious; there are only experiences of which we are aware, and others of which we are not aware, that is, of which we are unconscious.
If I hate a man because I am afraid of him, and if I am aware of my
hate but not of my fear, we may say that my hate is conscious and that
my fear is unconscious; still my fear does not lie in that mysterious
place: 'the' unconscious."
John Searle
has offered a critique of the Freudian unconscious. He argues that the
Freudian cases of shallow, consciously held mental states would be best
characterized as 'repressed consciousness,' while the idea of more
deeply unconscious mental states is more problematic. He contends that
the very notion of a collection of "thoughts" that exist in a privileged
region of the mind such that they are in principle never accessible
to conscious awareness, is incoherent. This is not to imply that there
are not "nonconscious" processes that form the basis of much of
conscious life. Rather, Searle simply claims that to posit the existence
of something that is like a "thought" in every way except for the fact
that no one can ever be aware of it (can never, indeed, "think" it) is
an incoherent concept. To speak of "something" as a "thought" either
implies that it is being thought by a thinker or that it could be
thought by a thinker. Processes that are not causally related to the
phenomenon called thinking are more appropriately called the
nonconscious processes of the brain.
Other critics of the Freudian unconscious include David Stannard, Richard Webster, Ethan Watters, Richard Ofshe, and Eric Thomas Weber.
David Holmes
examined sixty years of research about the Freudian concept of
"repression", and concluded that there is no positive evidence for this
concept. Given the lack of evidence for many Freudian hypotheses, some
scientific researchers proposed the existence of unconscious mechanisms
that are very different from the Freudian ones. They speak of a
"cognitive unconscious" (John Kihlstrom), an "adaptive unconscious" (Timothy Wilson), or a "dumb unconscious" (Loftus and Klinger), which executes automatic processes but lacks the complex mechanisms of repression and symbolic return of the repressed, and Robert Langs deep unconscious wisdom system.
In modern cognitive psychology,
many researchers have sought to strip the notion of the unconscious
from its Freudian heritage, and alternative terms such as "implicit" or
"automatic" have been used. These traditions emphasize the degree to
which cognitive processing happens outside the scope of cognitive
awareness, and show that things we are unaware of can nonetheless
influence other cognitive processes as well as behavior. Active research traditions related to the unconscious include implicit memory (see priming, implicit attitudes), and nonconscious acquisition of knowledge (see Lewicki, see also the section on cognitive perspective below).
Dreams
Freud
In
terms of the unconscious, the purpose of dreams, as stated by Freud, is
to fulfill repressed wishes through the process of dreaming, since they
cannot be fulfilled in real life. For example, if someone were to rob a
store and to feel guilty about it, they might dream about a scenario in
which their actions were justified and renders them blameless. Freud
asserted that the wish-fulfilling aspect of the dream may be disguised
due to the difficulty in distinguishing between manifest content and latent content. The manifest content consists of the plot of a dream at the surface level.
The latent content refers to the hidden or disguised meaning of the
events in the plot. The latent content of the dream is what supports the
idea of wish fulfillment. It represents the intimate information in the
dreamer's current issues and childhood conflict.
Opposing theories
In
response to Freud's theory on dreams, other psychologists have come up
with theories to counter his argument. Theorist Rosalind Cartwright
proposed that dreams provide people with the opportunity to act out and
work through everyday problems and emotional issues in a non-real
setting with no consequences. According to her cognitive
problem solving view, a large amount of continuity exists between our
waking thought and the thoughts that exist in dreams. Proponents of this
view believe that dreams allow participation in creative thinking and
alternate ways to handle situations when dealing with personal issues
because dreams are not restrained by logic or realism.
In addition to this, Allan Hobson and colleagues came up with the activation-synthesis hypothesis which proposes that dreams are simply the side effects of the neural activity in the brain that produces beta brain waves during REM sleep
that are associated with wakefulness. According to this hypothesis,
neurons fire periodically during sleep in the lower brain levels and
thus send random signals to the cortex.
The cortex then synthesizes a dream in reaction to these signals in
order to try to make sense of why the brain is sending them. However,
the hypothesis does not state that dreams are meaningless, it just
downplays the role that emotional factors play in determining dreams.
Contemporary cognitive psychology
Research
While,
historically, the psychoanalytic research tradition was the first to
focus on the phenomenon of unconscious mental activity, there is an
extensive body of conclusive research and knowledge in contemporary cognitive psychology devoted to the mental activity that is not mediated by conscious awareness.
Most of that (cognitive) research on unconscious processes has
been done in the mainstream, academic tradition of the information
processing paradigm. As opposed to the psychoanalytic tradition, driven
by the relatively speculative (in the sense of being hard to empirically
verify) theoretical concepts such as the Oedipus complex or Electra complex,
the cognitive tradition of research on unconscious processes is based
on relatively few theoretical assumptions and is very empirically
oriented (i.e., it is mostly data driven). Cognitive research has
revealed that automatically, and clearly outside of conscious awareness,
individuals register and acquire more information than what they can
experience through their conscious thoughts.
Unconscious processing of information about frequency
For example, an extensive line of research conducted by Hasher and Zacks
has demonstrated that individuals register information about the
frequency of events automatically (i.e., outside of conscious awareness
and without engaging conscious information processing resources).
Moreover, perceivers do this unintentionally, truly "automatically,"
regardless of the instructions they receive, and regardless of the
information processing goals they have. The ability to unconsciously and
relatively accurately tally the frequency of events appears to have
little or no relation to the individual's age,
education, intelligence, or personality, thus it may represent one of
the fundamental building blocks of human orientation in the environment
and possibly the acquisition of procedural knowledge and experience, in general.