Behavior-altering parasites are parasites with two or more hosts, capable of causing changes in the behavior of one of their hosts to enhance their transmission, sometimes directly affecting the hosts' decision-making and behavior control mechanisms. They do this by making the intermediate host, where they may reproduce asexually, more likely to be eaten by a predator at a higher trophic level which becomes the definitive host where the parasite reproduces sexually; the mechanism is therefore sometimes called parasite increased trophic facilitation or parasite increased trophic transmission. Examples can be found in bacteria, protozoa, viruses, and animals. Parasites may also alter the host behaviour to increase the protection to the parasites or their offspring. The term bodyguard manipulation is used for such mechanisms.
Among the behavioral changes caused by parasites is carelessness, making their hosts easier prey. The protozoan Toxoplasma gondii, for example, infects small rodents and causes them to become careless and attracted to the smell of feline urine, which increases their risk of predation and the parasite's chance of infecting a cat, its definitive host.
Parasites may alter the host's behavior by infecting the host's central nervous system, or by altering its neurochemical communication, studied in neuro-parasitology.
Among the behavioral changes caused by parasites is carelessness, making their hosts easier prey. The protozoan Toxoplasma gondii, for example, infects small rodents and causes them to become careless and attracted to the smell of feline urine, which increases their risk of predation and the parasite's chance of infecting a cat, its definitive host.
Parasites may alter the host's behavior by infecting the host's central nervous system, or by altering its neurochemical communication, studied in neuro-parasitology.
Behavioral change
Types
Parasite manipulations can be either direct or indirect. Indirect manipulation is the most frequent method used by behavior-altering parasites,
while the direct approach is far less common. Direct manipulation is
when the parasite itself affects the host and induces a behavioral
response, for example by creating neuroactive compounds that stimulate a
response in the host's central nervous system (CNS), a method mostly
practiced by parasites that reside within the CNS. Affecting the host's neural system is complicated and manipulation includes initiating immune cascades in the host.
However, determination of the causative factor is difficult, especially
whether the behavioral change is the result of direct manipulation from
the parasite, or an indirect response of the host's immune system. A direct approach to behavioral manipulation is often very costly for the parasite,
which results in a trade-off between the benefits of the manipulation
(e.g., fitness increase) and the energy it costs. The more common
approach for parasites is to indirectly induce behavioral responses by
interacting with the host's immune system to create the necessary neuroactive compounds to induce a desired behavioral response. Parasites can also indirectly affect the behavior of their hosts by disturbing their metabolism, development or immunity. Parasitic castrators
drastically modify their hosts' metabolism and reproduction, sometimes
by secreting castrating hormones, changing their behavior and physiology
to benefit the parasite.
Parasites may alter hosts' behaviors in ways that increase their likelihood of transmission
(e.g. by the host being ingested by a predator); result in the
parasite's release at appropriate sites (e.g. by changes in the host's
preferences for habitats); increase parasite survival or increase the host's likelihood of being infected with more parasites.
By viruses
Rabies
causes the host to be aggressive and prone to biting others. This along
with increased salivation, which carries the virus, increases the
chances of it spreading to new hosts.
Viruses from the family Baculoviridae
induce in their hosts changes to both feeding behavior and environment
selection. They infect moth and butterfly caterpillars, who some time
following infection begin to eat incessantly, providing nutrients for
the virus's replication.
When the virions (virus "units") are ready to leave the host, the
caterpillar climbs higher and higher, until its cells are made to
secrete enzymes that "dissolve the animal into goo", raining down clumps
of tissue and viral material for ingestion by future hosts.
By protozoa
The protozoan Toxoplasma gondii infects animals from the family Felidae (its definitive host), and its oocysts are shed with the host's feces. When a rodent consumes the fecal matter it gets infected with the parasite (becoming its intermediate host). The rodent subsequently becomes more extroverted and less fearful of cats, increasing its chance of predation and the parasite's chance of completing its lifecycle. There is some evidence that T. gondii, when infecting humans, alters their behavior in similar ways to rodents; it has also been linked to cases of schizophrenia. Other parasites that increase their host's risk of predation include Euhaplorchis californiensis, Dicrocoelium dendriticum, Myrmeconema neotropicum and Diplostomum pseudospathaceum.
The malaria parasite Plasmodium falciparum, carried by the Anopheles gambiae mosquito, changes its host's attraction to sources of nectar in order to increase its sugar intake and enhance the parasite's chance of survival. It also decreases the host's attraction to human blood while gestating, only to increase it when it is ready to transmit to a human host.
By helminths
Making the host careless increases the risk of it being eaten by a
non-host predator, interrupting the parasite's life-cycle. Some
parasites manipulate their intermediate host to reduce this risk. For
example, the parasitic trematode Microphallus sp., uses the snail Potamopyrgus antipodarum
as an intermediate host. The parasite manipulates the snail's foraging
behavior to increase the chance of it being preyed upon by the
parasite's definitive hosts (waterfowl).
The infected snail forages on the upper side of rocks during the period
of the day when waterfowl feed most intensely. During the rest of the
day, the snail forages at the bottom of rocks to reduce the risk of
being eaten by fish (non-hosts for the parasitic trematode).
The lancet liver fluke (Dicrocoelium dendriticum) is a parasitic trematode with a complex life cycle. In its adult state it occurs in the liver of its definitive host (ruminants), where it reproduces. The parasite eggs are passed with the feces
of the host, which then are eaten by a terrestrial snail (first
intermediate host). The fluke matures into a juvenile stage in the
snail, which in an attempt to protect itself excretes the parasites in
"slime-balls". The "slime-balls" are then consumed by ants (second
intermediate hosts). The fluke manipulates the ant to move up to the top
of grass, where they have a higher chance of being eaten by grazing
ruminants.
The parasitic nematode Myrmeconema neotropicum infects the intermediate ant host Cephalotes atratus. The nematode then induces a morphological change in the ant, which turns the gaster color from black to red, making it resemble fruit. This color transition makes the ant susceptible to predation by frugivorous
birds, which act as the parasite's definitive hosts. The parasitic eggs
are deposited in the bird's feces and are eaten by ants, which complete
the cycle.
Crickets infected by horsehair worms exhibit light-seeking
behavior and increased walking speed, leading them to open spaces and
ponds (the surface of which reflects moonlight); the crickets will
eventually find and enter a body of water, where the worm will wiggle
out of the cricket's abdomen and swim away. While crickets often drown
in the process, those who survive exhibit a partial recovery and return
to normal activities in as little as 20 hours.
The trematode Leucochloridium paradoxum matures inside snails of the genus Succinea.
When ready to switch to its definitive host, a bird, the parasite
travels to the eye stalks of its host and begins to pulsate, attracting
birds with its striking resemblance to an insect larva.
It also influences the normally nocturnal snail to climb out into the
open during the day for an increased chance of being consumed by a bird.
Schistocephalus solidus
is a parasitic tapeworm with three different hosts, two intermediate
and one definitive. In its adult stage the tapeworm resides in the
intestine of piscivorous
birds, where they reproduce and release eggs through the bird's feces.
Free-swimming larvae hatch from the eggs, which are in turn ingested by copepods
(the first intermediate host). The parasite grows and develops in the
crustacean into a stage that can infect the second intermediate host,
the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus).
The parasite's definitive host, a bird, then consumes the infected
three-spined stickleback and the cycle is complete. It has been observed
that S. solidus alters the behavior of the fish in a manner that impedes its escape response when faced with a predatorial bird.
This parasite-induced behavioral manipulation effectively increases the
chance of it being consumed by its definitive bird host. It has also
been observed that the parasite does not induce this behavior until it
has reached a developed stage that can survive in the host bird and therefore effectively reduce its own mortality rate, due to premature transmission.
By insects
The emerald cockroach wasp (Ampulex compressa) parasitises its host, the American cockroach (Periplaneta americana) as a food source and for its growing larvae. The wasp stings the cockroach twice: First in the thoracic ganglion,
paralyzing its front legs and enabling the wasp to deliver a second,
more difficult sting, directly into the cockroach's brain; this second sting makes the cockroach groom itself excessively before sinking into a state of hypokinesia – "a... lethargy characterized by lack of spontaneous movement or response to external stimuli".
The wasp then pulls the idle cockroach into its burrow, where it
deposits an egg onto its abdomen and buries it for the growing larva to
feed on. Keeping the cockroach in a hypokinetic state at this stage,
rather than simply killing it, allows it to stay "fresh" for longer for
the larva to feed on. The adult wasp emerges after 6 weeks, leaving behind nothing but an empty cockroach "shell".
The parasitic wasp Hymenoepimecis argyraphaga grows its larvae on spiders of the species Leucauge argyra. Shortly before killing its host the larva injects it with a chemical that changes its weaving behavior, causing it to weave a strong, cocoon-like structure. The larva then kills the spider and enters the cocoon to pupate.
The wasp Dinocampus coccinellae is both an endoparasite and ectoparasite of ladybugs. The wasp injects an egg into the beetle's abdomen, where the larva feeds on its haemolymph.
When grown and ready to pupate the larva exits its host, which remains
immobile, and weaves a cocoon on its underside, where it pupates. Were a
predator to approach, the beetle would twitch its limbs, scaring the
predator off. This use of the host as a protection has been termed as
bodyguard manipulation. Similarly several parasitic wasps induce their spider hosts to build stronger webs to protect the growing parasites.
Strepsiptera of the family Myrmecolacidae
can cause their ant host to linger on the tips of grass leaves,
increasing the chance of being found by the parasite's males (in case of
females) and putting them in a good position for male emergence (in
case of males). A similar, but much more intricate behavior is exhibited by ants infected with the fungus Ophiocordyceps unilateralis: irregularly-timed body convulsions cause the ant to drop to the forest floor, from which it climbs a plant up to a certain height
before locking its jaws into the vein of one of its leaves answering
certain criteria of direction, temperature and humidity. After several
days the fruiting body of the fungus grows from the ant's head and ruptures, releasing the fungus's spores.
Several species of fly in the family Phoridae parasitise fire ants.
The fly injects an egg into the ant's thorax; upon hatching, the larva
migrates into the ant's head, where it feeds on the ant's haemolymph,
muscle and nerve tissue. During this period some larvae direct the ant
up to 50 meters away from the nest and towards a moist, leafy place
where they can hatch safely.
Eventually the larva completely devours the ant's brain, which often
falls off (hence the species nickname: "decapitating fly"). The larva then pupates in the empty head capsule, emerging as an adult fly after two weeks.
By crustaceans
Members of the order Rhizocephala such as S. carcini
alter male hosts' hormonal balance, to encourage nurturing behavior
similar to that seen in females. The parasite usually spends its entire
life within the host; however, if it is removed from the host in a
laboratory setting, male hosts will subsequently grow partial or
complete female gonads.
Evolutionary perspective
Addition of intermediate hosts
For
complex life cycles to emerge in parasites, the addition of
intermediate host species must be beneficial, e.g., result in a higher fitness. It is probable that most parasites with complex life cycles evolved from simple life cycles.
The transfer from simple to complex life cycles has been analyzed
theoretically, and it has been shown that trophically transmitted
parasites can be favored by the addition of an intermediate prey host if
the population density of the intermediate host is higher than that of
the definitive host.
Additional factors that catalyze this transfer are high predation
rates, and a low natural mortality rate of the intermediate host.
Parasites with a single host species are faced with the problem
of not being able to survive in higher trophic levels and therefore
dying with its prey host. The development of complex life cycles is most
likely an adaptation of the parasite to survive in the predator.
The development of parasite increased trophic transmission is a further
adaptation in relation to a complex life cycle, where the parasite
increases its transmission to a definitive host by manipulating its
intermediate host.
Evolution of induced behaviors
The adaptive manipulation hypothesis posits that specific behavioral alterations induced in a host can be used by parasites to increase their fitness. Under this hypothesis, induced behaviors are the result of natural selection acting upon the parasite's extended phenotype (in this case its host's behavior). Many behaviors induced by obligate parasites
to complete their lifecycles are examples of adaptive manipulation
because of their clear relationship to parasite fitness. For example,
evidence has shown that infection by the parasitic worm Pomphorhynchus laevis leads to altered drifting behavior in its intermediate host, the amphipod Gammarus pulex; this altered behavior increases its host's predation risk by fish which are P. laevis's
definitive hosts. The induced behavioral change in the host thus leads
to the parasite's increased success in completing its life cycle.
In general, whether a specific behavioral change serves an adaptive
purpose for the parasite, the host, or both, depends on the entire
"host-parasite system": The life cycle of the pathogen, its virulence and the host's immune response.
Conversely, evolved behaviors of the host may be a result of adaptations to parasitism.
Mechanisms
The
way in which parasites induce behavioral changes in hosts has been
compared to the way a neurobiologist would effect a similar change in a
lab. A scientist may stimulate a certain pathway
in order to produce a specific behavior, such as increased appetite or
lowered anxiety; parasites also produce specific behavioral changes in
their hosts, but rather than stimulate specific neurological pathways,
they appear to target broader areas of the central nervous system. While the proximate mechanisms underlying this broad targeting have not been fully characterized, two mechanisms used by parasites to alter behavior in vertebrate hosts have been identified: infection of the central nervous system and altered neurochemical communication.
Infection of the central nervous system
Some parasites alter host behavior by infecting neurons in the host's central nervous system. The host's central nervous system responds to the parasite as it would to any other infection. The hallmarks of such response include local inflammation and the release of chemicals such as cytokines.
The immune response itself is responsible for induced behavioral
changes in many cases of parasitic infection. Parasites that are known
to induce behavioral changes through central nervous system inflammation
in their hosts include Toxoplasma gondii in rats, Trypanosoma cruzi in mice and Plasmodium mexicanum in the Mexican lizard.
While some parasites exploit their hosts' typical immune responses,
others seem to alter the immune response itself. For example, the
typical immune response in rodents is characterized by heightened
anxiety. Infection with Toxoplasma gondii inhibits this response, increasing the risk of predation by T. gondii's subsequent hosts. Research suggests that the inhibited anxiety-response could be the result of immunological damage to the limbic system.
Altered neurochemical communication
Parasites that induce behavioral changes in their hosts often exploit the regulation of social behavior in the brain. Social behavior is regulated by neurotransmitters, such as dopamine and serotonin, in the emotional centers of the brain – primarily the amygdala and the hypothalamus,
and although parasites may be capable of stimulating specific
neurochemical pathways to induce behavioral changes, evidence suggests
that they alter neurochemical communication through broad rather than
specific targeting. For example, Toxoplasma gondii
attaches to the hypothalamus rather than target a specific cellular
pathway; this broad targeting leads to a widespread increase in host
dopamine levels, which may in turn account for the loss of aversion to
cat odor. In some cases, T. gondii is believed to cause increases in dopamine levels by secreting another compound, L-Dopa, which may trigger a rise in dopamine levels, though concrete evidence for this mechanism has not yet been demonstrated. This rise in dopamine levels induces a loss of aversion to cat odor in the rats, increasing the risk of predation by cats, T. gondii's definitive host.
The mechanistic details underlying the increase in dopamine levels and
the way it affects the rat's behavioral change remain elusive.
The emerald cockroach wasp alters behavior through the injection of venom directly into the host's brain, causing hypokinesia.
This is achieved by a reduction in dopamine and octopamine activity,
which affects the transmission of interneurons involved in the escape
response;
so while the host's brain circuitry responsible for movement control is
still functional – and indeed it will slog along when pulled by the
wasp – the nervous system is in a depressed state. Put differently: the
wasp's toxin affects not the host's ability to move, but its motivation to do so.
The original function of such secretions may have been to
suppress the immune system of the host, as described above. The
trematode Schistosoma mansoni secretes opioid peptides into the host's bloodstream, influencing both its immune response and neural function. Other sources suggest a possible origin in molecular mimicry.
Other mechanisms
Mermithid nematodes infect arthropods, residing in their haemocoel (circulatory cavity) and manipulating their hemolymph osmolality to trigger water-seeking behavior. The means by which they do so are unknown.