Expressive aphasia | ||
---|---|---|
Other names | Broca's aphasia, non-fluent aphasia, agrammatic aphasia | |
Broca's area and Wernicke's area | ||
Specialty | Neurology |
Expressive aphasia, also known as Broca's aphasia, is a type of aphasia characterized by partial loss of the ability to produce language (spoken, manual, or written), although comprehension generally remains intact. A person with expressive aphasia will exhibit effortful speech. Speech generally includes important content words but leaves out function words that have only grammatical significance and not real-world meaning, such as prepositions and articles. This is known as "telegraphic speech". The person's intended message may still be understood, but their sentence will not be grammatically correct. In very severe forms of expressive aphasia, a person may only speak using single word utterances. Typically, comprehension is mildly to moderately impaired in expressive aphasia due to difficulty understanding complex grammar.
It is caused by acquired damage to the anterior regions of the brain, such as Broca's area. It is one subset of a larger family of disorders known collectively as aphasia. Expressive aphasia contrasts with receptive aphasia, in which patients are able to speak in grammatical sentences that lack semantic significance and generally also have trouble with comprehension. Expressive aphasia differs from dysarthria, which is typified by a patient's inability to properly move the muscles of the tongue and mouth to produce speech. Expressive aphasia also differs from apraxia of speech, which is a motor disorder characterized by an inability to create and sequence motor plans for speech.
Signs and symptoms
Broca's
(expressive) aphasia is a type of non-fluent aphasia in which an
individual's speech is halting and effortful. Misarticulations or
distortions of consonants and vowels,
namely phonetic dissolution, are common. Individuals with expressive
aphasia may only produce single words, or words in groups of two or
three.
Long pauses between words are common and multi-syllabic words may be
produced one syllable at a time with pauses between each syllable. The prosody of a person with Broca's aphasia is compromised by shortened length of utterances and the presence of self-repairs and disfluencies. Intonation and stress patterns are also deficient.
For example, in the following passage, a patient with Broca's aphasia is
trying to explain how he came to the hospital for dental surgery:
Yes... ah... Monday... er... Dad and Peter H... (his own name), and Dad.... er... hospital... and ah... Wednesday... Wednesday, nine o'clock... and oh... Thursday... ten o'clock, ah doctors... two... an' doctors... and er... teeth... yah.
The speech of a person with expressive aphasia contains mostly content words such as nouns, verbs, and some adjectives. However, function words like conjunctions,
articles, and prepositions are rarely used except for “and” which is
prevalent in the speech of most patients with aphasia. The omission of
function words makes the person's speech agrammatic. A communication partner of a person with aphasia may say that the person's speech sounds telegraphic due to poor sentence construction and disjointed words. For example, a person with expressive aphasia might say "Smart... university... smart... good... good..."
Self-monitoring is typically well preserved in patients with Broca's aphasia. They are usually aware of their communication deficits, and are more prone to depression and outbursts from frustration than are patients with other forms of aphasia.
In general, word comprehension is preserved, allowing patients to have functional receptive language skills.
Individuals with Broca's aphasia understand most of the everyday
conversation around them, but higher-level deficits in receptive
language can occur.
Because comprehension is substantially impaired for more complex
sentences, it is better to use simple language when speaking with an
individual with expressive aphasia. This is exemplified by the
difficulty to understand phrases or sentences with unusual structure. A
typical patient with Broca's aphasia will misinterpret "the man is
bitten by the dog" by switching the subject and object to “the dog is
bitten by the man.”
Typically, people with expressive aphasia can understand speech and read better than they can produce speech and write. The person's writing will resemble their speech and will be effortful, lacking cohesion, and containing mostly content words.
Letters will likely be formed clumsily and distorted and some may even
be omitted. Although listening and reading are generally intact, subtle
deficits in both reading and listening comprehension are almost always
present during assessment of aphasia.
Because Broca's area is anterior to the primary motor cortex, which is responsible for movement of the face, hands, and arms, a lesion affecting Broca's areas may also result in hemiparesis (weakness of both limbs on the same side of the body) or hemiplegia (paralysis of both limbs on the same side of the body).
The brain is wired contralaterally, which means the limbs on right side
of the body are controlled by the left hemisphere and vice versa.
Therefore, when Broca's area or surrounding areas in the left
hemisphere are damaged, hemiplegia or hemiparesis often occurs on the
right side of the body in individuals with Broca's aphasia.
Severity of expressive aphasia varies among patients. Some people
may only have mild deficits and detecting problems with their language
may be difficult. In the most extreme cases, patients may be able to
produce only a single word. Even in such cases, over-learned and
rote-learned speech patterns may be retained for instance, some patients can count from one to ten, but cannot produce the same numbers in novel conversation.
Manual language and aphasia
In deaf patients who use manual language (such as American Sign Language), damage to the left hemisphere of the brain leads to disruptions in their signing ability. Paraphasic
errors similar to spoken language have been observed; whereas in spoken
language a phonemic substitution would occur (e.g. "tagle" instead of
"table"), in ASL case studies errors in movement, hand position, and
morphology have been noted. Agrammatism, or the lack of grammatical
morphemes in sentence production, has also been observed in lifelong
users of ASL who have left hemisphere damage. The lack of syntactic
accuracy shows that the errors in signing are not due to damage to the
motor cortex, but rather are a manifestation of the damage to the
language-producing area of the brain. Similar symptoms have been seen in
a patient with left hemisphere damage whose first language was British Sign Language,
further showing that damage to the left hemisphere primarily hinders
linguistic ability, not motor ability. In contrast, patients who have
damage to non-linguistic areas on the left hemisphere have been shown to
be fluent in signing, but are unable to comprehend written language.
Overlap with receptive aphasia
In
addition to difficulty expressing oneself, individuals with expressive
aphasia are also noted to commonly have trouble with comprehension in
certain linguistic areas. This agrammatism overlaps with receptive
aphasia, but can be seen in patients who have expressive aphasia without
being diagnosed as having receptive aphasia. The most well-noted of
these are object-relative clauses, object Wh- questions, and topicalized
structures (placing the topic at the beginning of the sentence).
These three concepts all share phrasal movement, which can cause words
to lose their thematic roles when they change order in the sentence.
This is often not an issue for people without agrammatic aphasias, but
many people with aphasia rely heavily on word order to understand roles
that words play within the sentence.
Causes
More common
Less common
- Autoimmune disease
- Paraneoplastic syndrome
- Micrometastasis
- neurodegenerative disorders
- Certain infections (e.g., Bartonella henselae)
- Metabolic disease (e.g., hyperosmolar hyperglycemic state)
Common causes
The most common cause of expressive aphasia is stroke. A stroke is caused by hypoperfusion (lack of oxygen) to an area of the brain, which is commonly caused by thrombosis or embolism. Some form of aphasia occurs in 34 to 38% of stroke patients. Expressive aphasia occurs in approximately 12% of new cases of aphasia caused by stroke.
In most cases, expressive aphasia is caused by a stroke in
Broca's area or the surrounding vicinity. Broca's area is in the lower
part of the premotor cortex
in the language dominant hemisphere and is responsible for planning
motor speech movements. However, cases of expressive aphasia have been
seen in patients with strokes in other areas of the brain.
Patients with classic symptoms of expressive aphasia in general have
more acute brain lesions, whereas patients with larger, widespread
lesions exhibit a variety of symptoms that may be classified as global aphasia or left unclassified.
Expressive aphasia can also be caused by trauma to the brain, tumor, cerebral hemorrhage and by extradural abscess.
Understanding lateralization of brain function
is important for understanding which areas of the brain cause
expressive aphasia when damaged. In the past, it has been believed that
the area for language production differs between left and right-handed
individuals. If this were true, damage to the homologous region of
Broca's area in the right hemisphere should cause aphasia in a
left-handed individual. More recent studies have shown that even
left-handed individuals typically have language functions only in the
left hemisphere. However, left-handed individuals are more likely to
have a dominance of language in the right hemisphere.
Uncommon causes
Less common causes of expressive aphasia include primary autoimmune phenomenon and autoimmune phenomenon that are secondary to cancer (as a paraneoplastic syndrome)
have been listed as the primary hypothesis for several cases of
aphasia, especially when presenting with other psychiatric disturbances
and focal neurological deficits. Many case reports exist describing
paraneoplastic aphasia, and the reports that are specific tend to
describe expressive aphasia.
Although most cases attempt to exclude micrometastasis, it is likely
that some cases of paraneoplastic aphasia are actually extremely small
metastasis to the vocal motor regions.
Neurodegenerative disorders may present with aphasia. Alzheimer's disease may present with either fluent aphasia or expressive aphasia. There are case reports of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease presenting with expressive aphasia.
Diagnosis
Expressive aphasia is classified as non-fluent aphasia, as opposed to fluent aphasia. Diagnosis is done on a case-by-case basis, as lesions often affect the surrounding cortex and deficits are highly variable among patients with aphasia.
A physician is typically the first person to recognize aphasia in
a patient who is being treated for damage to the brain. Routine
processes for determining the presence and location of lesion in the
brain include magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and computed tomography
(CT) scans. The physician will complete a brief assessment of the
patient's ability to understand and produce language. For further
diagnostic testing, the physician will refer the patient to a
speech-language pathologist, who will complete a comprehensive
evaluation.
In order to diagnose a patient who is suffering from Broca's aphasia, there are certain commonly used tests and procedures. The Western Aphasia Battery
(WAB) classifies individuals based on their scores on the subtests;
spontaneous speech, auditory comprehension, repetition, and naming. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
(BDAE) can inform users what specific type of aphasia they may have,
infer the location of lesion, and assess current language abilities. The
Porch Index of Communication Ability (PICA) can predict potential
recovery outcomes of the patients with aphasia. Quality of life measurement is also an important assessment tool.
Tests such as the Assessment for Living with Aphasia (ALA) and the
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) allow for therapists to target
skills that are important and meaningful for the individual.
In addition to formal assessments, patient and family interviews
are valid and important sources of information. The patient's previous
hobbies, interests, personality, and occupation are all factors that
will not only impact therapy but may motivate them throughout the
recovery process.
Patient interviews and observations allow professionals to learn the
priorities of the patient and family and determine what the patient
hopes to regain in therapy. Observations of the patient may also be
beneficial to determine where to begin treatment. The current behaviors
and interactions of the patient will provide the therapist with more
insight about the client and their individual needs.
Other information about the patient can be retrieved from medical
records, patient referrals from physicians, and the nursing staff.
In non-speaking patients who use manual languages, diagnosis is
often based on interviews from the patient's acquaintances, noting the
differences in sign production pre- and post-damage to the brain.
Many of these patients will also begin to rely on non-linguistic
gestures to communicate, rather than signing since their language
production is hindered.
Treatment
Currently,
there is no standard treatment for expressive aphasia. Most aphasia
treatment is individualized based on a patient's condition and needs as
assessed by a speech language pathologist. Patients go through a period
of spontaneous recovery following brain injury in which they regain a
great deal of language function.
In the months following injury or stroke, most patients receive
traditional treatment for a few hours per day. Among other exercises,
patients practice the repetition of words and phrases. Mechanisms are
also taught in traditional treatment to compensate for lost language
function such as drawing and using phrases that are easier to pronounce.
Emphasis is placed on establishing a basis for communication with
family and caregivers in everyday life. Treatment is individualized
based on the patient's own priorities, along with the family's input.
A patient may have the option of individual or group treatment.
Although less common, group treatment has been shown to have
advantageous outcomes. Some types of group treatments include family
counseling, maintenance groups, support groups and treatment groups.
Melodic intonation therapy
Melodic intonation therapy
was inspired by the observation that individuals with non-fluent
aphasia sometimes can sing words or phrases that they normally cannot
speak. "Melodic Intonation Therapy was begun as an attempt to use the
intact melodic/prosodic processing skills of the right hemisphere in
those with aphasia to help cue retrieval words and expressive language."
It is believed that this is because singing capabilities are stored in
the right hemisphere of the brain, which is likely to remain unaffected
after a stroke in the left hemisphere.
However, recent evidence demonstrates that the capability of
individuals with aphasia to sing entire pieces of text may actually
result from rhythmic features and the familiarity with the lyrics.
The goal of Melodic Intonation Therapy is to utilize singing to
access the language-capable regions in the right hemisphere and use
these regions to compensate for lost function in the left hemisphere.
The natural musical component of speech was used to engage the patients'
ability to produce phrases. A clinical study revealed that singing and
rhythmic speech may be similarly effective in the treatment of
non-fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech. Moreover, evidence from randomized controlled trials
is still needed to confirm that Melodic Intonation Therapy is suitable
to improve propositional utterances and speech intelligibility in
individuals with (chronic) non-fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech.
Melodic Intonation Therapy appears to work particularly well in
patients who have had a unilateral, left hemisphere stroke, show poor
articulation, are non-fluent or have severely restricted speech output,
have moderately preserved auditory comprehension, and show good
motivation. MIT therapy on average lasts for 1.5 hours per day for five
days per week. At the lowest level of therapy, simple words and phrases
(such as "water" and "I love you") are broken down into a series of
high- and low-pitch syllables. With increased treatment, longer phrases
are taught and less support is provided by the therapist. Patients are
taught to say phrases using the natural melodic component of speaking
and continuous voicing is emphasized. The patient is also instructed to
use the left hand to tap the syllables of the phrase while the phrases
are spoken. Tapping is assumed to trigger the rhythmic component of
speaking to utilize the right hemisphere.
FMRI
studies have shown that Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) uses both
sides of the brain to recover lost function, as opposed to traditional
therapies that utilize only the left hemisphere. In MIT, individuals
with small lesions in the left hemisphere seem to recover by activation
of the left hemisphere perilesional cortex. Meanwhile, individuals with
larger left-hemisphere lesions show a recruitment of the use of
language-capable regions in the right hemisphere.
The interpretation of these results is still a matter of debate. For
example, it remains unclear whether changes in neural activity in the
right hemisphere result from singing or from the intensive use of common
phrases, such as "thank you", "how are you?" or "I am fine." This type
of phrases falls into the category of formulaic language and is known to be supported by neural networks of the intact right hemisphere.
A pilot study reported positive results when comparing the
efficacy of a modified form of MIT to no treatment in people with
nonfluent aphasia with damage to their left-brain. A randomized
controlled trial was conducted and the study reported benefits of
utilizing modified MIT treatment early in the recovery phase for people
with nonfluent aphasia.
Melodic Intonation Therapy is used by music therapists,
board-certified professionals that use music as a therapeutic tool to
effect certain non-musical outcomes in their patients. Speech language
pathologists can also use this therapy for individuals who have had a
left hemisphere stroke and non-fluent aphasias such as Broca's or even
apraxia of speech.
Constraint-induced therapy
Constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) is based on similar principles as constraint-induced movement therapy developed by Dr. Edward Taub at the University of Alabama at Birmingham.
Constraint-induced movement therapy is based on the idea that a person
with an impairment (physical or communicative) develops a "learned
nonuse" by compensating for the lost function with other means such as
using an unaffected limb by a paralyzed individual or drawing by a
patient with aphasia.
In constraint-induced movement therapy, the alternative limb is
constrained with a glove or sling and the patient is forced to use the
affected limb. In constraint-induced aphasia therapy the interaction is
guided by communicative need in a language game context, picture cards,
barriers making it impossible to see other players' cards, and other
materials, so that patients are encouraged ("constrained") to use the
remaining verbal abilities to succeed in the communication game.
Two important principles of constraint-induced aphasia therapy
are that treatment is very intense, with sessions lasting for up to 6
hours over the course of 10 days and that language is used in a
communication context in which it is closely linked to (nonverbal)
actions. These principles are motivated by neuroscience insights about learning
at the level of nerve cells (synaptic plasticity) and the coupling
between cortical systems for language and action in the human brain.
Constraint-induced therapy contrasts sharply with traditional therapy
by the strong belief that mechanisms to compensate for lost language
function, such as gesturing or writing, should not be used unless
absolutely necessary, even in everyday life.
It is believed that CIAT works by the mechanism of increased neuroplasticity.
By constraining an individual to use only speech, it is believed that
the brain is more likely to reestablish old neural pathways and recruit
new neural pathways to compensate for lost function.
The strongest results of CIAT have been seen in patients with
chronic aphasia (lasting over 6 months). Studies of CIAT have confirmed
that further improvement is possible even after a patient has reached a
"plateau" period of recovery.
It has also been proven that the benefits of CIAT are retained long
term. However, improvements only seem to be made while a patient is
undergoing intense therapy.
Recent work has investigated combining constraint-induced aphasia
therapy with drug treatment, which led to an amplification of therapy
benefits.
Medication
In
addition to active speech therapy, pharmaceuticals have also been
considered as a useful treatment for expressive aphasia. This area of
study is relatively new and much research continues to be conducted.
The following drugs have been suggested for use in treating aphasia and their efficacy has been studied in control studies.
- Bromocriptine – acts on Catecholamine Systems
- Piracetam – mechanism not fully understood, but most likely interacts with cholinergic and glutamatergic receptors, among others
- Cholinergic drugs (Donepezil, Aniracetam, Bifemelane) – acts on acetylcholine systems
- Dopaminergic psychostimulants: (Dexamphetamine, Methylphenidate)
The most effect has been shown by piracetam and amphetamine, which may increase cerebral plasticity
and result in an increased capability to improve language function. It
has been seen that piracetam is most effective when treatment is begun
immediately following stroke. When used in chronic cases it has been
much less efficient.
Bromocriptine has been shown by some studies to increase verbal
fluency and word retrieval with therapy than with just therapy alone. Furthermore, its use seems to be restricted to non-fluent aphasia.
Donepezil has shown a potential for helping chronic aphasia.
No study has established irrefutable evidence that any drug is an effective treatment for aphasia therapy. Furthermore, no study has shown any drug to be specific for language recovery.
Comparison between the recovery of language function and other motor
function using any drug has shown that improvement is due to a global
increase plasticity of neural networks.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation
In transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), magnetic fields are used to create electrical currents in specified cortical
regions. The procedure is a painless and noninvasive method of
stimulating the cortex. TMS works by suppressing the inhibition process
in certain areas of the brain.
By suppressing the inhibition of neurons by external factors, the
targeted area of the brain may be reactivated and thereby recruited to
compensate for lost function. Research has shown that patients can
demonstrate increased object naming ability with regular transcranial
magnetic stimulation than patients not receiving TMS. Furthermore, research suggests this improvement is sustained upon the completion of TMS therapy.
However, some patients fail to show any significant improvement from
TMS which indicates the need for further research of this treatment.
Treatment of underlying forms
Described
as the linguistic approach to the treatment of expressive aphasia,
treatment begins by emphasizing and educating patients on the thematic
roles of words within sentences.
Sentences that are usually problematic will be reworded into
active-voiced, declarative phrasings of their non-canonical
counterparts.
The simpler sentence phrasings are then transformed into variations
that are more difficult to interpret. For example, many individuals who
have expressive aphasia struggle with Wh- sentences. "What" and "who"
questions are problematic sentences that this treatment method attempts
to improve, and they are also two interrogative particles that are
strongly related to each other because they reorder arguments from the
declarative counterparts.
For instance, therapists have used sentences like, "Who is the boy
helping?" and "What is the boy fixing?" because both verbs are
transitive- they require two arguments in the form of a subject and a
direct object, but not necessarily an indirect object.
In addition, certain question particles are linked together based on
how the reworded sentence is formed. Training "who" sentences increased
the generalizations of non-trained "who" sentences as well as untrained
"what" sentences, and vice versa.
Likewise, "where" and "when" question types are very closely linked.
"What" and "who" questions alter placement of arguments, and "where" and
"when" sentences move adjunct phrases. Training is in the style of: "The man parked the car in the driveway. What did the man park in the driveway?" Sentence training goes on in this manner for more domains, such as clefts and sentence voice.
Results: Patients’ use of sentence types used in the TUF
treatment will improve, subjects will generalize sentences of similar
category to those used for treatment in TUF, and results are applied to
real-world conversations with others.
Generalization of sentence types used can be improved when the
treatment progresses in the order of more complex sentences to more
elementary sentences. Treatment has been shown to affect on-line
(real-time) processing of trained sentences and these results can be
tracked using fMRI mappings.
Training of Wh- sentences has led improvements in three main areas of
discourse for aphasics: increased average length of utterances, higher
proportions of grammatical sentences, and larger ratios of numbers of
verbs to nouns produced.
Patients also showed improvements in verb argument structure
productions and assigned thematic roles to words in utterances with more
accuracy.
In terms of on-line sentence processing, patients having undergone this
treatment discriminate between anomalous and non-anomalous sentences
with more accuracy than control groups and are closer to levels of
normalcy than patients not having participated in this treatment.
Mechanisms of recovery
Mechanisms
for recovery differ from patient to patient. Some mechanisms for
recovery occur spontaneously after damage to the brain, whereas others
are caused by the effects of language therapy. FMRI
studies have shown that recovery can be partially attributed to the
activation of tissue around the damaged area and the recruitment of new
neurons in these areas to compensate for the lost function. Recovery may
also be caused in very acute lesions by a return of blood flow and
function to damaged tissue that has not died around an injured area.
It has been stated by some researchers that the recruitment and
recovery of neurons in the left hemisphere opposed to the recruitment of
similar neurons in the right hemisphere is superior for long-term
recovery and continued rehabilitation.
It is thought that, because the right hemisphere is not intended for
full language function, using the right hemisphere as a mechanism of
recovery is effectively a "dead-end" and can lead only to partial
recovery.
It has been proven that, among all types of therapies, one of the
most important factors and best predictors for a successful outcome is
the intensity of the therapy. By comparing the length and intensity of
various methods of therapies, it was proven that intensity is a better
predictor of recovery than the method of therapy used.
Prognosis
In
most individuals with expressive aphasia, the majority of recovery is
seen within the first year following a stroke or injury. The majority of
this improvement is seen in the first four weeks in therapy following a
stroke and slows thereafter.
However, this timeline will vary depending upon the type of stroke
experienced by the patient. Patients who experienced an ischemic stroke
may recover in the days and weeks following the stroke, and then
experience a plateau and gradual slowing of recovery. On the contrary,
patients who experienced a hemorrhagic stroke experience a slower
recovery in the first 4–8 weeks, followed by a faster recovery which
eventually stabilizes.[57]
Numerous factors impact the recovery process and outcomes. Site
and extent of lesion greatly impacts recovery. Other factors that may
affect prognosis are age, education, gender, and motivation. Occupation, handedness, personality, and emotional state may also be associated with recovery outcomes.
Studies have also found that prognosis of expressive aphasia correlates strongly with the initial severity of impairment. However, it has been seen that continued recovery is possible years after a stroke with effective treatment.
Timing and intensity of treatment is another factor that impacts
outcomes. Research suggests that even in later stages of recovery,
intervention is effective at improving function, as well as, preventing
loss of function.
Unlike receptive aphasia, patients with expressive aphasia are
aware of their errors in language production. This may further motivate a
person with expressive aphasia to progress in treatment, which would
affect treatment outcomes.
On the other hand, awareness of impairment may lead to higher levels of
frustration, depression, anxiety, or social withdrawal, which have been
proven to negatively affect a person's chance of recovery.
History
Expressive aphasia was first identified by the French neurologist Paul Broca.
By examining the brains of deceased individuals having acquired
expressive aphasia in life, he concluded that language ability is
localized in the ventroposterior region of the frontal lobe. One of the
most important aspects of Paul Broca's discovery was the observation
that the loss of proper speech in expressive aphasia is due to the
brain's loss of ability to produce language, as opposed to the mouth's
loss of ability to produce words.
The discoveries of Paul Broca were made during the same period of time as the German Neurologist Carl Wernicke, who was also studying brains of aphasiacs post-mortem and identified the region now known as Wernicke's area.
Discoveries of both men contributed to the concept of localization,
which states that specific brain functions are all localized to a
specific area of the brain. While both men made significant
contributions to the field of aphasia, it was Carl Wernicke who realized
the difference between patients with aphasia that could not produce
language and those that could not comprehend language (the essential
difference between expressive and receptive aphasia).