Prosopagnosia | |
---|---|
Other names | Face blindness |
The fusiform face area, the part of the brain associated with facial recognition | |
Pronunciation | |
Specialty | Neurology |
Prosopagnosia (from Greek prósōpon, meaning "face", and agnōsía, meaning "non-knowledge"), also called face blindness, is a cognitive disorder of face perception in which the ability to recognize familiar faces, including one's own face (self-recognition), is impaired, while other aspects of visual processing (e.g., object discrimination) and intellectual functioning (e.g., decision-making) remain intact. The term originally referred to a condition following acute brain damage (acquired prosopagnosia), but a congenital or developmental form of the disorder also exists, with a prevalence rate of 2.5%. The specific brain area usually associated with prosopagnosia is the fusiform gyrus, which activates specifically in response to faces. The functionality of the fusiform gyrus allows most people to recognize faces in more detail than they do similarly complex inanimate objects. For those with prosopagnosia, the new method for recognizing faces depends on the less sensitive object-recognition system. The right hemisphere fusiform gyrus is more often involved in familiar face recognition than the left. It remains unclear whether the fusiform gyrus is only specific for the recognition of human faces or if it is also involved in highly trained visual stimuli.
Acquired prosopagnosia results from occipito-temporal lobe damage
and is most often found in adults. This is further subdivided into
apperceptive and associative prosopagnosia. In congenital prosopagnosia,
the individual never adequately develops the ability to recognize
faces.
Though there have been several attempts at remediation, no therapies have demonstrated lasting real-world improvements across a group of prosopagnosics. Prosopagnosics often learn to use "piecemeal" or "feature-by-feature" recognition strategies. This may involve secondary clues such as clothing, gait, hair color, skin color, body shape, and voice. Because the face seems to function as an important identifying feature in memory, it can also be difficult for people with this condition to keep track of information about people, and socialize normally with others. Prosopagnosia has also been associated with other disorders that are associated with nearby brain areas: left hemianopsia (loss of vision from left side of space, associated with damage to the right occipital lobe), achromatopsia (a deficit in color perception often associated with unilateral or bilateral lesions in the temporo-occipital junction) and topographical disorientation (a loss of environmental familiarity and difficulties in using landmarks, associated with lesions in the posterior part of the parahippocampal gyrus and anterior part of the lingual gyrus of the right hemisphere). It is from the Greek: prosopon = "face" and agnosia = "not knowing".
The opposite of prosopagnosia is the skill of superior face recognition ability. People with this ability are called "super recognizers".
Though there have been several attempts at remediation, no therapies have demonstrated lasting real-world improvements across a group of prosopagnosics. Prosopagnosics often learn to use "piecemeal" or "feature-by-feature" recognition strategies. This may involve secondary clues such as clothing, gait, hair color, skin color, body shape, and voice. Because the face seems to function as an important identifying feature in memory, it can also be difficult for people with this condition to keep track of information about people, and socialize normally with others. Prosopagnosia has also been associated with other disorders that are associated with nearby brain areas: left hemianopsia (loss of vision from left side of space, associated with damage to the right occipital lobe), achromatopsia (a deficit in color perception often associated with unilateral or bilateral lesions in the temporo-occipital junction) and topographical disorientation (a loss of environmental familiarity and difficulties in using landmarks, associated with lesions in the posterior part of the parahippocampal gyrus and anterior part of the lingual gyrus of the right hemisphere). It is from the Greek: prosopon = "face" and agnosia = "not knowing".
The opposite of prosopagnosia is the skill of superior face recognition ability. People with this ability are called "super recognizers".
Types
Apperceptive
Apperceptive prosopagnosia
has typically been used to describe cases of acquired prosopagnosia
with some of the earliest processes in the face perception system. The
brain areas thought to play a critical role in apperceptive
prosopagnosia are right occipital temporal regions.
People with this disorder cannot make any sense of faces and are
unable to make same–different judgments when they are presented with
pictures of different faces. They are unable to recognize both familiar
and unfamiliar faces. In addition, apperceptive sub-types of
prosopagnosia struggle recognizing facial emotion. However, they may be able to recognize people based on non-face clues such as their clothing, hairstyle, skin color, or voice. Apperceptive prosopagnosia is believed to be associated with impaired fusiform gyrus.
It is interesting that experiments on the formation of new face
detectors in adults on face-like stimuli (learning to distinguish the
faces of cats) indicate that such new detectors are formed not in the
fusiform, but in the lingual gyrus.
Associative
Associative prosopagnosia
has typically been used to describe cases of acquired prosopagnosia
with spared perceptual processes but impaired links between early face
perception processes and the semantic information we hold about people
in our memories. Right anterior temporal regions may also play a
critical role in associative prosopagnosia.
People with this form of the disorder may be able to tell whether
photos of people's faces are the same or different and derive the age
and sex from a face (suggesting they can make sense of some face
information) but may not be able to subsequently identify the person or
provide any information about them such as their name, occupation, or
when they were last encountered. Associative prosopagnosia is thought to be due to impaired functioning of the parahippocampal gyrus.
Developmental
Developmental prosopagnosia (DP), also called congenital prosopagnosia
(CP), is a face-recognition deficit that is lifelong, manifesting in
early childhood, and that cannot be attributed to acquired brain damage.
A number of studies have found functional deficits in DP both on the
basis of EEG measures and fMRI.
It has been suggested that a genetic factor is responsible for the
condition. The term "hereditary prosopagnosia" was introduced if DP
affected more than one family member, essentially accenting the possible
genetic contribution of this condition. To examine this possible
genetic factor, 689 randomly selected students were administered a
survey in which seventeen developmental prosopagnosics were quantifiably
identified. Family members of fourteen of the DP individuals were
interviewed to determine prosopagnosia-like characteristics, and in all
fourteen families, at least one other affected family member was found.
In 2005, a study led by Ingo Kennerknecht showed support for the
proposed congenital disorder form of prosopagnosia. This study provides
epidemiological evidence that congenital prosopagnosia is a frequently
occurring cognitive disorder that often runs in families. The analysis
of pedigree trees formed within the study also indicates that the
segregation pattern of hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA) is fully
compatible with autosomal dominant inheritance. This mode of inheritance
explains why HPA is so common among certain families (Kennerknecht et
al. 2006).
Cause
Prosopagnosia can be caused by lesions in various parts of the inferior occipital areas (occipital face area), fusiform gyrus (fusiform face area), and the anterior temporal cortex. Positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI
scans have shown that, in individuals without prosopagnosia, these
areas are activated specifically in response to face stimuli.
The inferior occipital areas are mainly involved in the early stages of
face perception and the anterior temporal structures integrate specific
information about the face, voice, and name of a familiar person.
Acquired prosopagnosia can develop as the result of several
neurologically damaging causes. Vascular causes of prosopagnosia include
posterior cerebral artery infarcts (PCAIs) and hemorrhages in the
infero-medial part of the temporo-occipital area. These can be either
bilateral or unilateral, but if they are unilateral, they are almost
always in the right hemisphere.
Recent studies have confirmed that right hemisphere damage to the
specific temporo-occipital areas mentioned above is sufficient to induce
prosopagnosia. MRI scans of patients with prosopagnosia showed lesions
isolated to the right hemisphere, while fMRI scans showed that the left
hemisphere was functioning normally.
Unilateral left temporo-occipital lesions result in object agnosia, but
spare face recognition processes, although a few cases have been
documented where left unilateral damage resulted in prosopagnosia. It
has been suggested that these face recognition impairments caused by
left hemisphere damage are due to a semantic defect blocking retrieval
processes that are involved in obtaining person-specific semantic
information from the visual modality.
Other less common etiologies include carbon monoxide poisoning, temporal lobectomy, encephalitis, neoplasm, right temporal lobe atrophy, injury, Parkinson's disease, and Alzheimer's disease.
Diagnosis
There
are few neuropsychological assessments that can definitively diagnose
prosopagnosia. One commonly used test is the famous faces tests, where
individuals are asked to recognize the faces of famous persons. However,
this test is difficult to standardize. The Benton Facial Recognition
Test (BFRT) is another test used by neuropsychologists to assess face
recognition skills. Individuals are presented with a target face above
six test faces and are asked to identify which test face matches the
target face. The images are cropped to eliminate hair and clothes, as
many people with prosopagnosia use hair and clothing cues to recognize
faces. Both male and female faces are used during the test. For the
first six items only one test face matches the target face; during the
next seven items, three of the test faces match the target faces and the
poses are different. The reliability of the BFRT was questioned when a
study conducted by Duchaine and Nakayama showed that the average score
for 11 self-reported prosopagnosics was within the normal range.
The test may be useful for identifying patients with apperceptive
prosopagnosia, since this is mainly a matching test and they are unable
to recognize both familiar and unfamiliar faces. They would be unable
to pass the test. It would not be useful in diagnosing patients with
associative prosopagnosia since they are able to match faces.
The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) was developed by Duchaine
and Nakayama to better diagnose people with prosopagnosia. This test
initially presents individuals with three images each of six different
target faces. They are then presented with many three-image series,
which contain one image of a target face and two distracters. Duchaine
and Nakayama showed that the CFMT is more accurate and efficient than
previous tests in diagnosing patients with prosopagnosia. Their study
compared the two tests and 75% of patients were diagnosed by the CFMT,
while only 25% of patients were diagnosed by the BFRT. However, similar
to the BFRT, patients are being asked to essentially match unfamiliar
faces, as they are seen only briefly at the start of the test. The test
is not currently widely used and will need further testing before it can
be considered reliable.
The 20-item Prosopagnosia Index (PI20)
is a freely available and validated self-report questionnaire that can
be used alongside computer-based face recognition tests to help
identify individuals with prosopagnosia. It has been validated using
objective measures of face perception ability including famous face
recognition tests and the Cambridge Face Memory Test. Less than 1.5% of
the general population score above 65 on the PI20 and less than 65% on
the CFMT.
Prognosis
Management
strategies for acquired prosopagnosia, such as a person who has
difficulty recognizing people's faces after a stroke, generally have a
low rate of success. Acquired prosopagnosia sometimes spontaneously resolves on its own.
History
Selective inabilities to recognize faces were documented as early as the 19th century, and included case studies by Hughlings Jackson and Charcot. However, it was not named until the term prosopagnosia was first used in 1947 by Joachim Bodamer ,
a German neurologist. He described three cases, including a 24-year-old
man who suffered a bullet wound to the head and lost his ability to
recognize his friends, family, and even his own face. However, he was
able to recognize and identify them through other sensory modalities
such as auditory, tactile, and even other visual stimuli patterns (such
as gait and other physical mannerisms). Bodamer gave his paper the title Die Prosop-Agnosie, derived from Classical Greek πρόσωπον (prósōpon) meaning "face" and αγνωσία (agnōsía) meaning "non-knowledge". In October 1996, Bill Choisser began popularizing the term face blindness for this condition; the earliest-known use of the term is in an 1899 medical paper.
A case of a prosopagnosia is "Dr P." in Oliver Sacks' 1985 book The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat, though this is more properly considered to be one of a more general visual agnosia.
Although Dr P. could not recognize his wife from her face, he was able
to recognize her by her voice. His recognition of pictures of his family
and friends appeared to be based on highly specific features, such as
his brother's square jaw and big teeth. Oliver Sacks himself suffered
from prosopagnosia, but did not know it for much of his life.
The study of prosopagnosia has been crucial in the development of theories of face perception.
Because prosopagnosia is not a unitary disorder (i.e., different people
may show different types and levels of impairment), it has been argued
that face perception involves a number of stages, each of which can
cause qualitative differences in impairment that different persons with
prosopagnosia may exhibit.
This sort of evidence has been crucial in supporting the theory
that there may be a specific face perception system in the brain. Most
researchers agree that the facial perception process is holistic rather
than featural, as it is for perception of most objects. A holistic
perception of the face does not involve any explicit representation of
local features (i.e., eyes, nose, mouth, etc.), but rather considers the
face as a whole.
Because the prototypical face has a specific spatial layout (eyes are
always located above nose, and nose located above mouth), it is
beneficial to use a holistic approach to recognize individual/specific
faces from a group of similar layouts. This holistic processing of the
face is exactly what is damaged in prosopagnosics.
They are able to recognize the specific spatial layout and
characteristics of facial features, but they are unable to process them
as one entire face. This is counterintuitive to many people, as not
everyone believes faces are "special" or perceived in a different way
from other objects in the rest of the world. Though evidence suggests
that other visual objects are processed in a holistic manner (e.g., dogs
in dog experts), the size of these effects are smaller and are less
consistently demonstrated than with faces. In a study conducted by
Diamond and Carey, they showed this to be true by performing tests on
dog-show judges. They showed pictures of dogs to the judges and to a
control group and they then inverted those same pictures and showed them
again. The dog-show judges had greater difficulty in recognizing the
dogs once inverted compared to the control group; the inversion effect,
the increased difficulty in recognizing a picture once inverted, was
shown to be in effect. It was previously believed that the inversion
effect was associated only with faces, but this study shows that it may
apply to any category of expertise.
It has also been argued that prosopagnosia may be a general
impairment in understanding how individual perceptual components make up
the structure or gestalt of an object. Psychologist Martha Farah has been particularly associated with this view.
Children
Developmental
prosopagnosia can be a difficult thing for a child to both understand
and cope with. Many adults with developmental prosopagnosia report that
for a long time they had no idea that they had a deficit in face
processing, unaware that others could distinguish people solely on
facial differences.
Prosopagnosia in children may be overlooked; they may just appear
to be very shy or slightly odd due to their inability to recognize
faces. They may also have a hard time making friends, as they may not
recognize their classmates. They often make friends with children who
have very clear, distinguishing features.
Children with prosopagnosia may also have difficulties following
the plots of television shows and movies, as they have trouble
recognizing the different characters. They tend to gravitate towards
cartoons, in which characters have simple but well-defined
characteristics, and tend to wear the same clothes, may be strikingly
different colours or even different species. Prosopagnosiac children
even have a hard time telling family members apart, or recognizing
people out of context (e.g., the teacher in a grocery store). Some have difficulty recognising themselves in group photographs.
Additionally, children with prosopagnosia can have a difficult
time at school, as many school professionals are not well versed in
prosopagnosia, if they are aware of the disorder at all.
Notable people with prosopagnosia
- Robert Gascoyne-Cecil, 3rd Marquess of Salisbury (1830–1903)
- Oliver Sacks (1933–2015), British neurologist
- Jane Goodall (born 1934), English primatologist
- Chuck Close (born 1949), American painter
- Duncan Bannatyne (born 1949), Scottish entrepreneur
- Steve Wozniak (born 1950), American computer engineer and co-founder of Apple Inc.
- John Hickenlooper (born 1952), former Governor of Colorado
- Jim Woodring (born 1952), American cartoonist
- Stephen Fry (born 1957), English actor
- Mary Ann Sieghart (born 1961), English journalist and radio presenter
- Margaret Kerry (born 1929), American actress and reference model for Tinker Bell
- Markos Moulitsas (born 1971), American blogger and former member of the military
- Victoria, Crown Princess of Sweden (born 1977)
- Karl Kruszelnicki, Australian science communicator