Jealousy generally refers to the thoughts or feelings of insecurity, fear, concern, and envy over relative lack of possessions, status or something of great personal value, particularly in reference to a comparator, a rival, or a competitor.
Jealousy can consist of one or more emotions such as anger, resentment, inadequacy, helplessness or disgust. In its original meaning, jealousy is distinct from envy, though the two terms have popularly become synonymous in the English language, with jealousy now also taking on the definition originally used for envy alone.
Jealousy is a typical experience in human relationships, and it has been observed in infants as young as five months. Some researchers claim that jealousy is seen in all cultures. However, others claim jealousy is a culture-specific emotion.
Jealousy can either be suspicious or reactive, and it is often reinforced as a series of particularly strong emotions and constructed as a universal human experience. Psychologists have proposed several models to study the processes underlying jealousy and have identified factors that result in jealousy. Sociologists have demonstrated that cultural beliefs and values play an important role in determining what triggers jealousy and what constitutes socially acceptable expressions of jealousy. Biologists have identified factors that may unconsciously influence the expression of jealousy.
Throughout history, artists have also explored the theme of jealousy in photographs, paintings, films, songs, plays, poems, and books, and theologians have offered religious views of jealousy based on the scriptures of their respective faiths.
Jealousy is a typical experience in human relationships, and it has been observed in infants as young as five months. Some researchers claim that jealousy is seen in all cultures. However, others claim jealousy is a culture-specific emotion.
Jealousy can either be suspicious or reactive, and it is often reinforced as a series of particularly strong emotions and constructed as a universal human experience. Psychologists have proposed several models to study the processes underlying jealousy and have identified factors that result in jealousy. Sociologists have demonstrated that cultural beliefs and values play an important role in determining what triggers jealousy and what constitutes socially acceptable expressions of jealousy. Biologists have identified factors that may unconsciously influence the expression of jealousy.
Throughout history, artists have also explored the theme of jealousy in photographs, paintings, films, songs, plays, poems, and books, and theologians have offered religious views of jealousy based on the scriptures of their respective faiths.
Sexual jealousy
Sexual jealousy may be triggered when a person's significant other displays sexual interest in another person. The feeling of jealousy may be just as powerful if one partner suspects the other is guilty of infidelity.
Fearing that their partner will experience sexual jealousy the person
who has been unfaithful may lie about their actions in order to protect
their partner. Experts often believe that sexual jealousy is in fact a
biological imperative. It may be part of a mechanism by which humans and
other animals ensure access to the best reproductive partners.
It seems that male jealousy in heterosexual relationships may be
influenced by their female partner's phase in her menstrual cycle. In
the period around and shortly before ovulation, males are found to
display more mate-retention tactics, which are linked to jealousy.
Furthermore, a male is more likely to employ mate-retention tactics if
their partner shows more interest in other males, which is more likely
to occur in the pre-ovulation phase.
Romantic jealousy
Romantic jealousy arises as a result of romantic interest. It is defined as
... a complex of thoughts, feelings, and actions that follow threats to self-esteem and/or threats to the existence or quality of the relationship when those threats are generated by the perception of a real or potential romantic attraction between one's partner and a (perhaps imaginary) rival.
Different from sexual jealousy, romantic jealousy is triggered by
threats to self and relationship (rather than sexual interest in another
person). Factors, such as feelings of inadequacy as a partner, sexual
exclusivity, and having put relatively more effort into the
relationship, are positively related to relationship jealousy in both
genders.
Communicative responses
As
romantic jealousy is a complicated reaction that has multiple
components, i.e., thoughts, feelings, and actions, one aspect of
romantic jealousy that is under study is communicative responses.
Communicative responses serve three critical functions in a romantic
relationship, i.e., reducing uncertainty, maintaining or repairing
relationship, and restoring self-esteem. If done properly, communicative responses can lead to more satisfying relationships after experiencing romantic jealousy.
There are two subsets of communicative responses: interactive
responses and general behavior responses. Interactive responses is
face-to-face and partner-directed while general behavior responses may
not occur interactively.
Guerrero and colleagues further categorize multiple types of
communicative responses of romantic jealousy. Interactive responses can
be broken down to six types falling in different places on continua of
threat and directness:
- Avoidance/Denial (low threat and low directness). Example: becoming silent; pretending nothing is wrong.
- Integrative Communication (low threat and high directness). Example: explaining feelings; calmly questioning partner.
- Active Distancing (medium threat and medium directness). Example: decreasing affection.
- Negative Affect Expression (medium threat and medium directness). Example: venting frustration; crying or sulking.
- Distributive Communication (high threat and high directness). Example: acting rude; making hurtful or abrasive comments.
- Violent Communication/Threats (high threat and high directness). Example: using physical force.
Guerrero and colleagues have also proposed five general behavior
responses. The five sub-types differ in whether a response is 1)
directed at partner or rival(s), 2) directed at discovery or repair, and
3) positively or negatively valenced:
- Surveillance/ Restriction (rival-targeted, discovery-oriented, commonly negatively valenced). Example: observing rival; trying to restrict contact with partner.
- Rival Contacts (rival-targeted, discovery-oriented/repair-oriented, commonly negatively valenced). Example: confronting rival.
- Manipulation Attempts (partner-targeted, repair-oriented, negatively valenced). Example: tricking partner to test loyalty; trying to make partner feel guilty.
- Compensatory Restoration (partner-targeted, repair-oriented, commonly positively valenced). Example: sending flowers to partner.
- Violent Behavior (-, -, negatively valenced). Example: slamming doors.
While some of these communicative responses are destructive and
aggressive, e.g., distributive communication and active distancing, some
individuals respond to jealousy in a more constructive way.
Integrative communication, compensatory restoration, and negative
affect expression have been shown to lead to positive relation outcomes.
One factor that affects the type of communicative responses elicited in
an individual is emotions. Jealousy anger is associated with more
aggressive communicative response while irritation tends to lead to more
constructive communicative behaviors.
Gender-based differences
According to the Parental Investment Model based on parental investment
theory, more men than women ratify sex differences in jealousy. In
addition, more women over men consider emotional infidelity (fear of
abandonment) as more distressing than sexual infidelity.
According to the attachment theory, sex and attachment style makes
significant and unique interactive contributions to the distress
experienced. Security within the relationship also heavily contributes
to one’s level of distress. These findings imply that psychological and
cultural mechanisms regarding sex differences may play a larger role
than expected (Levy, Blatt, Schachner.) The attachment theory also
claims to reveal how infants' attachment patterns are the basis for
self-report measures of adult attachment. (Levy, Blatt & Shaner,
1998). Although there are no sex differences in childhood attachment,
individuals with dismissing behavior were more concerned with the sexual
aspect of relationships (Schachner & shaer, 2004). As a coping
mechanism these individuals would report sexual infidelity as more
harmful. Moreover, research shows that audit attachment styles strongly
conclude with the type of infidelity that occurred. Thus psychological
and cultural mechanisms are implied as unvarying differences in
jealousy that play a role in sexual attachment.
Emotional jealousy was predicted to be nine times more responsive
in females than in males. The emotional jealousy predicted in females
also held turn to state that females experiencing emotional jealousy are
more violent than men experiencing emotional jealousy.
There are distinct emotional responses to gender differences in
romantic relationships (Buss, Green & Saboni 2004). For example, due
to paternity uncertainty in males, jealousy increases in males over
sexual infidelity rather than emotional. According to research more
women are likely to be upset by signs of resource withdraw (i.e. another
female) than by sexual infidelity. A large amount of data
supports this notion. However, one must consider for jealousy the life
stage or experience one encounters in reference to the diverse
responses to infidelity available. Research states that a componential
view of jealousy consist of specific set of emotions that serve the
reproductive role.
However, research shows that both men and women would be equally angry
and point the blame for sexual infidelity, but women would be more hurt
by emotional infidelity. Despite this fact, anger surfaces when both
parties involved is responsible for some type of uncontrollable
behavior, sexual conduct is not exempt. (Sabbini and Silver, Averill
1995). Some behavior and actions are controllable such as sexual
behavior. However hurt feelings are activated by relationship deviation.
No evidence is known to be sexually dimorphic in both college and adult
convenience samples. The Jealousy Specific Innate Model (JSIM) proved
to not be innate, but may be sensitive to situational factors. As a
result, it may only activate at stages in on. One study discovered
serious relationships are reserved for older adults rather than
undergraduates. For example, Buss et al. (1992) predicted that male jealousy decreases as females reproductive values decreases.
A second possibility that the JSIM effect is not innate but is from one culture (Desieno et al.,
2002) Kitayana (2004) have highlighted differences in socio-economic
status specific such as the divide between high school and collegiate
individuals. Moreover, individuals of both genders were angrier and
blamed their partners more for sexual infidelities but were more hurt by
emotional (Sabini & Green 2004). Jealousy is composed of
lower-level emotional states (e.g., anger and hurt) which may be
triggered by a variety of events, not by differences in individuals'
life stage. Although research has recognized the importance of early
childhood experiences for the development of competence in intimate
relationships, early family environment is recently being examined as
well (Richardson and Guyer, 1998). Research on self-esteem and
attachment theory suggest that individuals internalize early
experiences within the family which subconsciously translates into their
personal view of worth of themselves and the value of being close to
other individuals, especially in an interpersonal relationship
(Steinberg, Davila, & Fincham, 2006).
In animals
A
study by researches at the University of California, San Diego,
replicated jealousy studies done on humans on canines. They reported,
in a paper published in PLOS ONE in 2014, that a significant number of
dogs exhibited jealous behaviors when their human companions paid
attention to dog-like toys, compared to when their human companions paid
attention to nonsocial objects.
Etymology
The word stems from the French jalousie, formed from jaloux (jealous), and further from Low Latin zelosus (full of zeal), in turn from the Greek word ζήλος (zēlos), sometimes "jealousy", but more often in a positive sense "emulation, ardour, zeal"
(with a root connoting "to boil, ferment";
or "yeast"). The "biblical language" zeal would be known as "tolerating
no unfaithfulness" while in middle English zealous is good. One origin word gelus meant "Possessive and suspicious" the word then turned into jelus.
Since William Shakespeare's use of terms like "green-eyed monster", the color green has been associated with jealousy and envy, from which the expressions "green with envy", are derived.
Theories
Scientific definitions
People do not express jealousy through a single emotion or a single behavior.
They instead express jealousy through diverse emotions and behaviors,
which makes it difficult to form a scientific definition of jealousy.
Scientists instead define jealousy in their own words, as illustrated by
the following examples:
- "Romantic jealousy is here defined as a complex of thoughts, feelings, and actions which follow threats to self-esteem and/or threats to the existence or quality of the relationship, when those threats are generated by the perception of potential attraction between one's partner and a (perhaps imaginary) rival." (White, 1981, p. 24)
- "Jealousy, then, is any aversive reaction that occurs as the result of a partner's extradyadic relationship that is considered likely to occur." (Bringle & Buunk, 1991, page 135)
- "Jealousy is conceptualized as a cognitive, emotional, and behavioral response to a relationship threat. In the case of sexual jealousy, this threat emanates from knowing or suspecting that one's partner has had (or desires to have) sexual activity with a third party. In the case of emotional jealousy, an individual feels threatened by her or his partner's emotional involvement with and/or love for a third party." (Guerrero, Spitzberg, & Yoshimura, 2004, page 311)
- "Jealousy is defined as a defensive reaction to a perceived threat to a valued relationship, arising from a situation in which the partner's involvement with an activity and/or another person is contrary to the jealous person's definition of their relationship." (Bevan, 2004, page 195)
- "Jealousy is triggered by the threat of separation from, or loss of, a romantic partner, when that threat is attributed to the possibility of the partner's romantic interest in another person." (Sharpteen & Kirkpatrick, 1997, page 628)
These definitions of jealousy share two basic themes. First, all the
definitions imply a triad composed of a jealous individual, a partner,
and a perception of a third party or rival. Second, all the definitions
describe jealousy as a reaction to a perceived threat to the
relationship between two people, or a dyad. Jealous reactions typically
involve aversive emotions and/or behaviors that are assumed to be
protective for their attachment relationships. These themes form the
essential meaning of jealousy in most scientific studies.
Comparison with envy
Popular culture uses the word jealousy as a synonym for envy.
Many dictionary definitions include a reference to envy or envious
feelings. In fact, the overlapping use of jealousy and envy has a long
history.
The terms are used indiscriminately in such popular 'feelgood' books as Nancy Friday's Jealousy, where the expression 'jealousy' applies to a broad range of passions, from envy to lust and greed. While this kind of usage blurs the boundaries between categories that are intellectually valuable and psychologically justifiable, such confusion is understandable in that historical explorations of the term indicate that these boundaries have long posed problems. Margot Grzywacz's fascinating etymological survey of the word in Romance and Germanic languages asserts, indeed, that the concept was one of those that proved to be the most difficult to express in language and was therefore among the last to find an unambiguous term. Classical Latin used invidia, without strictly differentiating between envy and jealousy. It was not until the postclassical era that Latin borrowed the late and poetic Greek word zelotypia and the associated adjective zelosus. It is from this adjective that are derived French jaloux, Provençal gelos, Italian geloso, and Spanish celoso. (Lloyd, 1995, page 4)
Perhaps the overlapping use of jealousy and envy occurs because
people can experience both at the same time. A person may envy the
characteristics or possessions of someone who also happens to be a
romantic rival. In fact, one may even interpret romantic jealousy as a form of envy.
A jealous person may envy the affection that his or her partner gives
to a rival — affection the jealous person feels entitled to himself or
herself. People often use the word jealousy as a broad label that
applies to both experiences of jealousy and experiences of envy.
Although popular culture often uses jealousy and envy as
synonyms, modern philosophers and psychologists have argued for
conceptual distinctions between jealousy and envy. For example,
philosopher John Rawls
distinguishes between jealousy and envy on the ground that jealousy
involves the wish to keep what one has, and envy the wish to get what
one does not have. Thus, a child is jealous of her parents' attention to
a sibling, but envious of her friend's new bicycle. Psychologists Laura
Guerrero and Peter Andersen have proposed the same distinction.
They claim the jealous person "perceives that he or she possesses a
valued relationship, but is in danger of losing it or at least of having
it altered in an undesirable manner," whereas the envious person "does
not possess a valued commodity, but wishes to possess it." Gerrod
Parrott draws attention to the distinct thoughts and feelings that occur
in jealousy and envy.
The common experience of jealousy for many people may involve:
- Fear of loss
- Suspicion of or anger about a perceived betrayal
- Low self-esteem and sadness over perceived loss
- Uncertainty and loneliness
- Fear of losing an important person to another
- Distrust
The experience of envy involves:
- Feelings of inferiority
- Longing
- Resentment of circumstances
- Ill will towards envied person often accompanied by guilt about these feelings
- Motivation to improve
- Desire to possess the attractive rival's qualities
- Disapproval of feelings
Parrot acknowledges that people can experience envy and jealousy at
the same time. Feelings of envy about a rival can even intensify the
experience of jealousy.
Still, the differences between envy and jealousy in terms of thoughts
and feelings justify their distinction in philosophy and science.
In psychology
Jealousy
involves an entire "emotional episode," including a complex
"narrative": the circumstances that lead up to jealousy, jealousy itself
as emotion, any attempt at self regulation,
subsequent actions and events and the resolution of the episode
(Parrott, 2001, p. 306). The narrative can originate from experienced
facts, thoughts, perceptions, memories, but also imagination, guess and
assumptions. The more society and culture matter in the formation of
these factors, the more jealousy can have a social and cultural origin.
By contrast, Goldie (2000, p. 228) shows how jealousy can be a
"cognitively impenetrable state", where education and rational belief
matter very little.
One possible explanation of the origin of jealousy in evolutionary psychology
is that the emotion evolved in order to maximize the success of our
genes: it is a biologically based emotion (Prinz after Buss and Larsen,
2004, p. 120) selected to foster the certainty about the paternity of
one’s own offspring. A jealous behavior, in men, is directed into
avoiding sexual betrayal and a consequent waste of resources and effort
in taking care of someone else’s offspring. There are, additionally,
cultural or social explanations of the origin of jealousy. According to
one, the narrative from which jealousy arises can be in great part made
by the imagination. Imagination is strongly affected by a person's
cultural milieu. The pattern of reasoning, the way one perceives
situations, depends strongly on cultural context. It has elsewhere been
suggested that jealousy is in fact a secondary emotion in reaction to
one's needs not being met, be those needs for attachment, attention,
reassurance or any other form of care that would be otherwise expected
to arise from that primary romantic relationship.
While mainstream psychology considers sexual arousal through jealousy a paraphilia, some authors on sexuality (Serge Kreutz, Instrumental Jealousy) have argued that jealousy in manageable dimensions can have a definite positive effect on sexual function
and sexual satisfaction. Studies have also shown that jealousy
sometimes heightens passion towards partners and increases the intensity
of passionate sex.
Jealousy in children and teenagers has been observed more often
in those with low self-esteem and can evoke aggressive reactions. One
such study suggested that developing intimate friends can be followed by
emotional insecurity and loneliness in some children when those
intimate friends interact with others. Jealousy is linked to aggression
and low self-esteem.
Research by Sybil Hart, Ph.D., at Texas Tech University indicates that
children are capable of feeling and displaying jealousy at as young as
six months.
Infants showed signs of distress when their mothers focused their
attention on a lifelike doll. This research could explain why children
and infants show distress when a sibling is born, creating the
foundation for sibling rivalry.
In sociology
Anthropologists have claimed that jealousy varies across cultures. Cultural learning
can influence the situations that trigger jealousy and the manner in
which jealousy is expressed. Attitudes toward jealousy can also change
within a culture over time. For example, attitudes toward jealousy
changed substantially during the 1960s and 1970s in the United States.
People in the United States adopted much more negative views about
jealousy. As men and women became more equal it became less appropriate
or acceptable to express jealousy.
Applications
In fiction, film, and art
Artistic depictions of jealousy
occur in fiction, films, and other art forms such as painting and
sculpture. Jealousy is a common theme in literature, art, theatre, and
film. Often, it is presented as a demonstration of particularly deep
feelings of love, rather than a destructive obsession.
A study done by Ferris, Smith, Greenberg, and Smith looked into the way people saw dating and romantic relationships based on how many reality dating shows they watched.
People who spent a large amount of time watching these reality dating
shows "endorsed" or supported the "dating attitudes" that would be shown
on the show. While the other people who do not spend time watching reality dating shows did not mirror the same ideas.This
means if someone watches a reality dating show that displays men and
women reacting violently or aggressively towards their partner due to
jealousy they can mirror that. This is reflected in romantic movies as well.Jessica
R. Frampton conducted a study looking into romantic jealousy in movies.
The study found that there were "230 instances of romantic jealousy
were identified in the 51 top-grossing romantic comedies from 2002–2014" Some of the films did not display romantic jealousy however, some featured many examples of romantic jealousy. This was due to the fact that some of the top grossing movies did not contain a rival or romantic competition. While others such as Forgetting Sarah Marshal was said to contain "19 instances of romantic jealousy." Out of the 230 instances 58% were reactive jealousy while 31% showed possessive jealousy. The last 11% displayed anxious jealousy it was seen the least in all 230 cases. Out of the 361 reactions to the jealousy found 53% were found to be "Destructive responses." Only 19% of responses were constructive while 10% showed avoidant responses. The
last 18% were considered "rival focused responses" which lead to the
finding that "there was a higher than expected number of rival-focused
responses to possessive jealousy."
In religion
Jealousy in religion examines how the scriptures and teachings of
various religions deal with the topic of jealousy. Religions may be
compared and contrasted on how they deal with two issues: concepts of
divine jealousy, and rules about the provocation and expression of human
jealousy.
The Christian New Testament records that the Jewish chief priests and elders had handed Jesus over to Pontius Pilate to be crucified because they were jealous of his popularity.
Cross Culture
A study was done in order to cross examine jealousy among four different cultures. The four cultures were chosen to try to reveal the difference in
expression when moving from culture to culture. These four cultures were
Ireland, Thailand, India and the United States.
The overall findings discovered that in male dominate cultures are more
likely to express and reveal jealousy than other cultures. This was proven when the survey found that Thais are less likely to express jealousy then the other three cultures.
This is because the men in these cultures are rewarded in a way for
showing jealousy due to the fact that some women interpret it as love.
This can also be seen when watching romantic comedies when males show
they are jealous of a rival or emotionally jealous women perceive it as
men caring more.