Attachment theory is a psychological model attempting to describe the dynamics of long-term and short-term interpersonal relationships between humans. "Attachment theory is not formulated as a general theory of relationships; it addresses only a specific facet": how human beings respond within relationships when hurt, separated from loved ones, or perceiving a threat.
Provided any caregiver, all infants become attached--however
individual differences in the quality of the relationships remain
significant.
In infants, attachment as a motivational and behavioral system
directs the child to seek proximity with a familiar caregiver when they
are alarmed, with expectation they will receive protection and
emotional support.
John Bowlby
believed that the tendency for primate infants to develop attachments
to familiar caregivers was the result of evolutionary pressures, since
attachment behavior would facilitate the infant's survival in the face
of dangers such as predation or exposure to the elements.
The most important tenet of attachment theory is an infant needs to develop a relationship with at least one primary caregiver
for the child's successful social and emotional development, and in
particular for learning how to regulate their feelings. Any caregiver is
likely to become the principal attachment figure if they provide most
of the child care and related social interaction.
In the presence of a sensitive and responsive caregiver, the infant
will use the caregiver as a "safe base" from which to explore.
This relationship can be dyadic,
as in the mother-child dyad often studied in Western culture, or it can
involve a community of caregivers (siblings/extended family/teachers)
as can be seen in areas of Africa and South America.
It should be recognized "even sensitive caregivers get it right
only about fifty per cent of the time. Their communications are either
out of sync, or mismatched. There are times when parents feel tired or
distracted. The telephone rings or there is breakfast to prepare. In
other words, attuned interactions rupture quite frequently. But the
hallmark of a sensitive caregiver is that the ruptures are managed and
repaired."
Attachments between infants and caregivers form even if this caregiver is not sensitive and responsive in social interactions with them.
This has important implications. Infants cannot exit unpredictable or
insensitive caregiving relationships. Instead they must manage
themselves as best they can within such relationships.
Based on her established Strange Situation Protocol, research by developmental psychologist Mary Ainsworth
in the 1960s and 1970s found children will have different patterns of
attachment depending on how they experienced their early caregiving
environment. Early patterns of attachment, in turn, shape — but do not
determine — the individual's expectations in later relationships.
Four different attachment classifications have been identified in children:
- Secure attachment occurs when children feel they can rely on their caregivers to attend to their needs of proximity, emotional support and protection. It is considered to be the best attachment style.
- Anxious-ambivalent attachment occurs when the infant feels separation anxiety when separated from the caregiver and does not feel reassured when the caregiver returns to the infant.
- Anxious-avoidant attachment occurs when the infant avoids their parents.
- Disorganized attachment occurs when there is a lack of attachment behavior.
In the 1980s, the theory was extended to attachment in adults.
Attachment applies to adults when adults feel close attachment to their
parents, their romantic and platonic partners and their friends.
Attachment theory has become the dominant theory used today in
the study of infant and toddler behavior and in the fields of infant
mental health, treatment of children, and related fields.
Infant attachment
Within attachment theory, attachment means "a biological
instinct in which proximity to an attachment figure is sought when the
child senses or perceives threat or discomfort. Attachment behaviour
anticipates a response by the attachment figure which will remove threat
or discomfort".
Such bonds may be reciprocal between two adults, but between a child
and a caregiver these bonds are based on the child's need for safety,
security and protection, paramount in infancy and childhood.
John Bowlby
begins by noting organisms at different levels of the phylogenetic
scale regulate instinctive behavior in distinct ways, ranging from
primitive reflex-like "fixed action patterns" to complex plan
hierarchies with subgoals and strong learning components. In the most
complex organisms, instinctive behaviors may be "goal-corrected" with
continual on-course adjustments (such as a bird of prey adjusting its
flight to the movements of the prey).
The concept of cybernetically controlled behavioral systems
organized as plan hierarchies (Miller, Galanter, and Pribram, 1960) thus
came to replace Freud's concept of drive and instinct. Such systems
regulate behaviors in ways that need not be rigidly innate,
but—depending on the organism—can adapt in greater or lesser degrees to
changes in environmental circumstances, provided these do not deviate
much from the organism's environment of evolutionary adaptedness. Such
flexible organisms pay a price, however, because adaptable behavioral
systems can more easily be subverted from their optimal path of
development.
For humans, Bowlby speculates, the environment of evolutionary
adaptedness probably resembles present-day hunter-gatherer societies for
the purpose of survival, and, ultimately, genetic replication.
Attachment theory is not an exhaustive description of human
relationships, nor is it synonymous with love and affection, although
these may indicate bonds exist.
Some infants direct attachment behaviour (proximity seeking) toward
more than one attachment figure almost as soon as they start to show
discrimination between caregivers; most come to do so during their
second year. These figures are arranged hierarchically, with the
principal attachment figure at the top.
The set-goal of the attachment behavioural system is to maintain the accessibility and availability of the attachment figure. Many cultures use multiple forms of attachment including the dyadic model most prominent in Western cultures and allomothering.
Alarm is the term used for activation of the attachment behavioural system caused by fear of danger.
Anxiety is the anticipation or fear of being cut off from
the attachment figure. If the figure is unavailable or unresponsive,
separation distress occurs.
In infants, physical separation can cause anxiety and anger,
followed by sadness and despair. By age three or four, physical
separation is no longer such a threat to the child's bond with the
attachment figure. Threats to security in older children and adults
arise from prolonged absence, breakdowns in communication, emotional
unavailability, or signs of rejection or abandonment.
Behaviors
The attachment behavioural system serves to achieve or maintain proximity to the attachment figure.
Pre-attachment behaviours occur in the first six months of life.
During the first phase (the first eight weeks), infants smile, babble,
and cry to attract the attention of potential caregivers. Although
infants of this age learn to discriminate between caregivers, these
behaviours are directed at anyone in the vicinity.
During the second phase (two to six months), the infant
discriminates between familiar and unfamiliar adults, becoming more
responsive toward the caregiver; following and clinging are added to the
range of behaviours. The infant's behaviour toward the caregiver
becomes organized on a goal-directed basis to achieve the conditions
that make it feel secure.
By the end of the first year, the infant is able to display a
range of attachment behaviours designed to maintain proximity. These
manifest as protesting the caregiver's departure, greeting the
caregiver's return, clinging when frightened, and following when able.
With the development of locomotion, the infant begins to use the caregiver or caregivers as a "safe base" from which to explore.
Infant exploration is greater when the caregiver is present because the
infant's attachment system is relaxed and it is free to explore. If the
caregiver is inaccessible or unresponsive, attachment behavior is more
strongly exhibited. Anxiety, fear, illness, and fatigue will cause a child to increase attachment behaviors.
After the second year, as the child begins to see the caregiver
as an independent person, a more complex and goal-corrected partnership
is formed.
Children begin to notice others' goals and feelings and plan their
actions accordingly. For example, whereas babies cry because of pain,
two-year-olds cry to summon their caregiver, and if that does not work,
cry louder, shout, or follow.
Tenets
Common attachment behaviours and emotions, displayed in most social primates including humans, are adaptive.
The long-term evolution of these species has involved selection for
social behaviors that make individual or group survival more likely. The
commonly observed attachment behaviour of toddlers staying near
familiar people would have had safety advantages in the environment of
early adaptation, and has similar advantages today. Bowlby saw the
environment of early adaptation as similar to current hunter-gatherer societies.
There is a survival advantage in the capacity to sense possibly
dangerous conditions such as unfamiliarity, being alone, or rapid
approach. According to Bowlby, proximity-seeking to the attachment
figure in the face of threat is the "set-goal" of the attachment
behavioral system.
Bowlby's original account of a sensitivity period
during which attachments can form of between six months and two to
three years has been modified by later researchers. These researchers
have shown there is indeed a sensitive period during which attachments
will form if possible, but the time frame is broader and the effect less
fixed and irreversible than first proposed.
With further research, authors discussing attachment theory have
come to appreciate social development is affected by later as well as
earlier relationships. Early steps in attachment take place most easily
if the infant has one caregiver, or the occasional care of a small
number of other people. According to Bowlby, almost from the beginning,
many children have more than one figure toward whom they direct
attachment behaviour. These figures are not treated alike; there is a
strong bias for a child to direct attachment behaviour mainly toward one
particular person. Bowlby used the term "monotropy" to describe this
bias.
Researchers and theorists have abandoned this concept insofar as it may
be taken to mean the relationship with the special figure differs qualitatively from that of other figures. Rather, current thinking postulates definite hierarchies of relationships.
Early experiences with caregivers gradually give rise to a system
of thoughts, memories, beliefs, expectations, emotions, and behaviours
about the self and others. This system, called the "internal working
model of social relationships", continues to develop with time and
experience.
Internal models regulate, interpret, and predict
attachment-related behaviour in the self and the attachment figure. As
they develop in line with environmental and developmental changes, they
incorporate the capacity to reflect and communicate about past and
future attachment relationships. They enable the child to handle new types of social interactions;
knowing, for example, an infant should be treated differently from an
older child, or that interactions with teachers and parents share
characteristics. This internal working model continues to develop
through adulthood, helping cope with friendships, marriage, and
parenthood, all of which involve different behaviours and feelings.
The development of attachment is a transactional process.
Specific attachment behaviours begin with predictable, apparently
innate, behaviours in infancy. They change with age in ways determined
partly by experiences and partly by situational factors.
As attachment behaviours change with age, they do so in ways shaped by
relationships. A child's behaviour when reunited with a caregiver is
determined not only by how the caregiver has treated the child before,
but on the history of effects the child has had on the caregiver.
Cultural differences
In
Western culture child-rearing, there is a focus on single attachment to
primarily the mother. This dyadic model is not the only strategy of
attachment producing a secure and emotionally adept child. Having a
single, dependably responsive and sensitive caregiver (namely the
mother) does not guarantee the ultimate success of the child. Results
from Israeli, Dutch and east African studies show children with multiple
caregivers grow up not only feeling secure, but developed "more
enhanced capacities to view the world from multiple perspectives." This evidence can be more readily found in hunter-gatherer communities rather than Western day-care contexts.
In hunter-gatherer communities, in the past and present, mothers
are the primary caregivers but share the maternal responsibility of
ensuring the child's survival with a variety of different allomothers.
So while the mother is important, she is not the only opportunity for
relational attachment a child can make. Several group members (with or
without blood relation) contribute to the task of bringing up a child,
sharing the parenting role and therefore can be sources of multiple
attachment. There is evidence of this communal parenting throughout
history that "would have significant implications for the evolution of
multiple attachment."
In "non-metropolis" India (where "dual income nuclear families"
are more the norm / dyadic mother relationship is), where a family
normally consists of 3 generations (and if lucky 4: grandparents,
parents & child / children, and maybe great-grandparents too), the
child / children by default have four / six caregivers from whom to
select for their "attachment figure". And a child's "uncles and aunts"
(father's siblings and their spouses) also contribute to the child's
psycho-social enrichment.
Although it has been debated for years, and there are tedious
differences among cultures, research shows that the three basic
aspects of Attachment Theory are in fact universal. The Hypotheses are: 1)that secure attachment is the most desirable
state, and the most prevalent. 2) maternal sensitivity influences infant
attachment patterns and 3) specific infant attachments predict later
social and cognitive competence.
Attachment classification in children: The Strange Situation Protocol
The
most common and empirically supported method for assessing attachment
in infants (11 months–17 months) is the Strange Situation Protocol,
developed by Mary Ainsworth as a result of her careful in-depth observations of infants with their mothers in Baltimore, USA.
The Strange Situation Protocol is a research tool that was not intended
for diagnostic purposes. While the procedure may be used to supplement
clinical impressions, the resulting classifications should not be
confused with the psychiatric diagnosis 'Reactive Attachment Disorder
(RAD)'. The clinical concept of RAD differs in a number of fundamental
ways from the theory and research driven attachment classifications
based on the Strange Situation Procedure. The idea that insecure
attachments are synonymous with RAD is, in fact, not accurate and leads
to ambiguity when formally discussing attachment theory as it has
evolved in the research literature. This is not to suggest that the
concept of RAD is without merit, but rather that the clinical and
research conceptualizations of insecure attachment and attachment disorder are not synonymous.
The 'Strange Situation' is a laboratory procedure used to assess
infants' pattern of attachment to their caregiver by introducing an
unexpected threat, two brief separations from the mother followed by
reunion. In the procedure, the mother and infant are placed in an
unfamiliar playroom equipped with toys while a researcher films the
procedure through a one-way mirror. The procedure consists of eight
sequential episodes in which the infant experiences both separation from
and reunion with the mother as well as the presence of an unfamiliar
person (the Stranger). The protocol is conducted in the following format unless modifications are otherwise noted by a particular researcher:
- Episode 1: Mother (or other familiar caregiver), Baby, Experimenter (30 seconds)
- Episode 2: Mother, Baby (3 mins)
- Episode 3: Mother, Baby, Stranger (3 mins)
- Episode 4: Stranger, Baby (3 mins or less)
- Episode 5: Mother, Baby (3 mins)
- Episode 6: Baby Alone (3 mins or less)
- Episode 7: Stranger, Baby (3 mins or less)
- Episode 8: Mother, Baby (3 mins)
Mainly on the basis of their reunion behaviour (although other
behaviors are taken into account) in the Strange Situation Paradigm
(Ainsworth et al., 1978; see below), infants can be categorized into
three 'organized' attachment categories: Group B (later called
'secure'), Group A (later called 'anxious avoidant'), and Group C (later
called 'anxious ambivalent'). There are subclassifications for each
group (see below).
Beginning in 1970, a series of expansions were added to Ainsworth's original patterns. They include the following: B4 (1970), A/C (1985) D/disorganized (1986), B5 (1988, 1992) A+, C+, & Depressed (1992, 2010).
At later ages, additional categories have been described. Each of these
patterns reflects a different kind of attachment relationship of the
infant with the mother/caregiver. An infant may have a different pattern
of attachment to each parent as well as to alternate caregivers.
Pattern of attachment is thus not a part of the infant, but is
characteristic of the protective and comforting quality of a specific
relationship. These attachment patterns are associated with behavioral
patterns and can help further predict a child's future personality.
Attachment patterns
"The
strength of a child's attachment behavior in a given circumstance does
not indicate the 'strength' of the attachment bond. Some insecure
children will routinely display very pronounced attachment behaviours,
while many secure children find that there is no great need to engage in
either intense or frequent shows of attachment behavior."
“Individuals with different attachment styles have different beliefs
about romantic love period, availability, trust capability of love
partners and love readiness.”
Secure attachment
A toddler who is securely attached to his or her parent (or other
familiar caregiver) will explore freely while the caregiver is present,
typically engages with strangers, is often visibly upset when the
caregiver departs, and is generally happy to see the caregiver return.
The extent of exploration and of distress are affected, however, by the
child's temperamental make-up and by situational factors as well as by
attachment status. A child's attachment is largely influenced by their
primary caregiver's sensitivity to their needs. Parents who consistently
(or almost always) respond to their child's needs will create securely
attached children. Such children are certain that their parents will be
responsive to their needs and communications.
In the traditional Ainsworth et al. (1978) coding of the Strange
Situation, secure infants are denoted as "Group B" infants and they are
further sub-classified as B1, B2, B3, and B4.
Although these sub-groupings refer to different stylistic responses to
the comings and goings of the caregiver, they were not given specific
labels by Ainsworth and colleagues, although their descriptive behaviors
led others (including students of Ainsworth) to devise a relatively
"loose" terminology for these subgroups. B1's have been referred to as
"secure-reserved", B2's as 'secure-inhibited', B3's as
"secure-balanced", and B4's as "secure-reactive". In academic
publications however, the classification of infants (if subgroups are
denoted) is typically simply "B1" or "B2" although more theoretical and
review-oriented papers surrounding attachment theory may use the above
terminology.
Securely attached children are best able to explore when they
have the knowledge of a secure base (their caregiver) to return to in
times of need. When assistance is given, this bolsters the sense of
security and also, assuming the parent's assistance is helpful, educates
the child in how to cope with the same problem in the future.
Therefore, secure attachment can be seen as the most adaptive attachment
style. According to some psychological researchers, a child becomes
securely attached when the parent is available and able to meet the
needs of the child in a responsive and appropriate manner. At infancy
and early childhood, if parents are caring and attentive towards their
children, those children will be more prone to secure attachment.
Anxious-ambivalent attachment
Anxious-ambivalent attachment is also misnamed as "resistant attachment".
In general, a child with an anxious-ambivalent pattern of attachment
will typically explore little (in the Strange Situation) and is often
wary of strangers, even when the parent is present. When the mother
departs, the child is often highly distressed. The child is generally
ambivalent when his mother returns.
The anxious-ambivalent strategy is a response to unpredictably
responsive caregiving, and the displays of anger (ambivalent resistant)
or helplessness (ambivalent passive) towards the caregiver on reunion
can be regarded as a conditional strategy for maintaining the
availability of the caregiver by preemptively taking control of the
interaction.
The C1 (ambivalent resistant) subtype is coded when "resistant
behavior is particularly conspicuous. The mixture of seeking and yet
resisting contact and interaction has an unmistakably angry quality and
indeed an angry tone may characterize behavior in the preseparation
episodes".
Regarding the C2 (ambivalent passive) subtype, Ainsworth et al. wrote:
Perhaps the most conspicuous characteristic of C2 infants is their passivity. Their exploratory behavior is limited throughout the SS and their interactive behaviors are relatively lacking in active initiation. Nevertheless, in the reunion episodes they obviously want proximity to and contact with their mothers, even though they tend to use signalling rather than active approach, and protest against being put down rather than actively resisting release ... In general the C2 baby is not as conspicuously angry as the C1 baby.
Research
done by McCarthy and Taylor (1999), found that children with abusive
childhood experiences were more likely to develop ambivalent
attachments. The study also found that children with ambivalent
attachments were more likely to experience difficulties in maintaining
intimate relationships as adults.
Anxious-avoidant attachment
An
infant with an anxious-avoidant pattern of attachment will avoid or
ignore the caregiver—showing little emotion when the caregiver departs
or returns. The infant will not explore very much regardless of who is
there. Infants classified as anxious-avoidant (A) represented a puzzle
in the early 1970s. They did not exhibit distress on separation, and
either ignored the caregiver on their return (A1 subtype) or showed some
tendency to approach together with some tendency to ignore or turn away
from the caregiver (A2 subtype). Ainsworth and Bell theorized that the
apparently unruffled behaviour of the avoidant infants was in fact a
mask for distress, a hypothesis later evidenced through studies of the
heart-rate of avoidant infants.
Infants are depicted as anxious-avoidant when there is:
... conspicuous avoidance of the mother in the reunion episodes which is likely to consist of ignoring her altogether, although there may be some pointed looking away, turning away, or moving away ... If there is a greeting when the mother enters, it tends to be a mere look or a smile ... Either the baby does not approach his mother upon reunion, or they approach in "abortive" fashions with the baby going past the mother, or it tends to only occur after much coaxing ... If picked up, the baby shows little or no contact-maintaining behavior; he tends not to cuddle in; he looks away and he may squirm to get down.
Ainsworth's
narrative records showed that infants avoided the caregiver in the
stressful Strange Situation Procedure when they had a history of
experiencing rebuff of attachment behaviour. The infant's needs were
frequently not met and the infant had come to believe that communication
of emotional needs had no influence on the caregiver.
Ainsworth's student Mary Main
theorized that avoidant behaviour in the Strange Situation Procedure
should be regarded as "a conditional strategy, which paradoxically
permits whatever proximity is possible under conditions of maternal
rejection" by de-emphasizing attachment needs.
Main proposed that avoidance has two functions for an infant
whose caregiver is consistently unresponsive to their needs. Firstly,
avoidant behavior allows the infant to maintain a conditional proximity
with the caregiver: close enough to maintain protection, but distant
enough to avoid rebuff. Secondly, the cognitive processes organizing
avoidant behavior could help direct attention away from the unfulfilled
desire for closeness with the caregiver—avoiding a situation in which
the child is overwhelmed with emotion ("disorganized distress"), and
therefore unable to maintain control of themselves and achieve even
conditional proximity.
Disorganized/disoriented attachment
Ainsworth
herself was the first to find difficulties in fitting all infant
behavior into the three classifications used in her Baltimore study.
Ainsworth and colleagues sometimes observed "tense movements such as
hunching the shoulders, putting the hands behind the neck and tensely
cocking the head, and so on. It was our clear impression that such
tension movements signified stress, both because they tended to occur
chiefly in the separation episodes and because they tended to be
prodromal to crying. Indeed, our hypothesis is that they occur when a
child is attempting to control crying, for they tend to vanish if and
when crying breaks through."
Such observations also appeared in the doctoral theses of Ainsworth's
students. Crittenden, for example, noted that one abused infant in her
doctoral sample was classed as secure (B) by her undergraduate coders
because her strange situation behavior was "without either avoidance or
ambivalence, she did show stress-related stereotypic headcocking
throughout the strange situation. This pervasive behavior, however, was
the only clue to the extent of her stress".
Beginning in 1983, Crittenden offered A/C and other new organized
classifications (see below). Drawing on records of behaviors discrepant
with the A, B and C classifications, a fourth classification was added
by Ainsworth's colleague Mary Main.
In the Strange Situation, the attachment system is expected to be
activated by the departure and return of the caregiver. If the behaviour
of the infant does not appear to the observer to be coordinated in a
smooth way across episodes to achieve either proximity or some relative
proximity with the caregiver, then it is considered 'disorganized' as it
indicates a disruption or flooding of the attachment system (e.g. by
fear). Infant behaviors in the Strange Situation Protocol coded as
disorganized/disoriented include overt displays of fear; contradictory
behaviours or affects occurring simultaneously or sequentially;
stereotypic, asymmetric, misdirected or jerky movements; or freezing and
apparent dissociation. Lyons-Ruth has urged, however, that it should be
more widely "recognized that 52% of disorganized infants continue to
approach the caregiver, seek comfort, and cease their distress without
clear ambivalent or avoidant behavior".
There is rapidly growing interest in disorganized attachment from clinicians and policy-makers as well as researchers.
However, the disorganized/disoriented attachment (D) classification has
been criticized by some for being too encompassing, including Ainsworth
herself.
In 1990, Ainsworth put in print her blessing for the new 'D'
classification, though she urged that the addition be regarded as
"open-ended, in the sense that subcategories may be distinguished", as
she worried that too many different forms of behavior might be treated
as if they were the same thing.
Indeed, the D classification puts together infants who use a somewhat
disrupted secure (B) strategy with those who seem hopeless and show
little attachment behaviour; it also puts together infants who run to
hide when they see their caregiver in the same classification as those
who show an avoidant (A) strategy on the first reunion and then an
ambivalent-resistant (C) strategy on the second reunion. Perhaps
responding to such concerns, George and Solomon have divided among
indices of disorganized/disoriented attachment (D) in the Strange
Situation, treating some of the behaviors as a 'strategy of
desperation' and others as evidence that the attachment system has been
flooded (e.g. by fear, or anger).
Moreover, Crittenden argues that some behaviour classified as
Disorganized/disoriented can be regarded as more 'emergency' versions of
the avoidant and/or ambivalent/resistant strategies, and function to
maintain the protective availability of the caregiver to some degree.
Sroufe et al. have agreed that "even disorganized attachment behaviour
(simultaneous approach-avoidance; freezing, etc.) enables a degree of
proximity in the face of a frightening or unfathomable parent".
However, "the presumption that many indices of 'disorganization' are
aspects of organized patterns does not preclude acceptance of the notion
of disorganization, especially in cases where the complexity and
danger of the threat are beyond children's capacity for
response."
For example, "Children placed in care, especially more than once, often
have intrusions. In videos of the Strange Situation Procedure, they
tend to occur when a rejected/neglected child approaches the stranger in
an intrusion of desire for comfort, then loses muscular control and
falls to the floor, overwhelmed by the intruding fear of the unknown,
potentially dangerous, strange person."
Main and Hesse
found most of the mothers of these children had suffered major losses
or other trauma shortly before or after the birth of the infant and had
reacted by becoming severely depressed.
In fact, fifty-six per cent of mothers who had lost a parent by death
before they completed high school had children with disorganized
attachments. Subsequent studies, whilst emphasising the potential importance of unresolved loss, have qualified these findings.
For example, Solomon and George found unresolved loss in the mother
tended to be associated with disorganized attachment in their infant
primarily when they had also experienced an unresolved trauma in their
life prior to the loss.
Categorization differences across cultures
Across
different cultures deviations from the Strange Situation Protocol have
been observed. A Japanese study in 1986 (Takahashi) studied 60 Japanese
mother-infant pairs and compared them with Ainsworth's distributional
pattern. Although the ranges for securely attached and insecurely
attached had no significant differences in proportions, the Japanese
insecure group consisted of only resistant children with no children
categorized as avoidant. This may be because the Japanese child rearing
philosophy stressed close mother infant bonds than in Western cultures.
In Northern Germany, Grossmann et al. (Grossmann, Huber, &
Wartner, 1981; Grossmann, Spangler, Suess, & Unzner, 1985)
replicated the Ainsworth Strange Situation with 46 mother infant pairs
and found a different distributions of attachment classifications with a
high number of avoidant infants: 52% avoidant, 34% secure, and 13%
resistant (Grossmann et al., 1985). Another study in Israel found there
was a high frequency of an ambivalent pattern which according to
Grossman et al. (1985) could be attributed a greater parental push
toward children's independence. Although it has been debated for years,
and there are tedious differences among cultures, research shows that
the three basic aspects of Attachment Theory are in fact universal. The Hypotheses are: 1)that secure attachment is the most desirable
state, and the most prevalent. 2) maternal sensitivity influences infant
attachment patterns and 3) specific infant attachments predict later
social and cognitive competence.
Later patterns and the dynamic-maturational model
Techniques
have been developed to allow verbal ascertainment of the child's state
of mind with respect to attachment. An example is the "stem story", in
which a child is given the beginning of a story that raises attachment
issues and asked to complete it. For older children, adolescents and
adults, semi-structured interviews are used in which the manner of
relaying content may be as significant as the content itself.
However, there are no substantially validated measures of attachment
for middle childhood or early adolescence (approximately 7 to 13 years
of age).
Some studies of older children have identified further attachment
classifications. Main and Cassidy observed that disorganized behavior in
infancy can develop into a child using caregiving-controlling or
punitive behaviour in order to manage a helpless or dangerously
unpredictable caregiver. In these cases, the child's behaviour is
organized, but the behavior is treated by researchers as a form of
'disorganization' (D) since the hierarchy in the family is no longer
organized according to parenting authority.
Patricia McKinsey Crittenden
has elaborated classifications of further forms of avoidant and
ambivalent attachment behavior. These include the caregiving and
punitive behaviors also identified by Main and Cassidy (termed A3 and
C3 respectively), but also other patterns such as compulsive compliance
with the wishes of a threatening parent (A4).
Crittenden's ideas developed from Bowlby's proposal that "given
certain adverse circumstances during childhood, the selective exclusion
of information of certain sorts may be adaptive. Yet, when during
adolescence and adulthood the situation changes, the persistent
exclusion of the same forms of information may become maladaptive".
Crittenden proposed that the basic components of human experience of danger are two kinds of information:
1. 'Affective information' – the emotions provoked by the
potential for danger, such as anger or fear. Crittenden terms this
"affective information". In childhood this information would include
emotions provoked by the unexplained absence of an attachment figure.
Where an infant is faced with insensitive or rejecting parenting, one
strategy for maintaining the availability of their attachment figure is
to try to exclude from consciousness or from expressed behaviour any
emotional information that might result in rejection.
2. Causal or other sequentially-ordered knowledge about the
potential for safety or danger. In childhood this would include
knowledge regarding the behaviors that indicate an attachment figure's
availability as a secure haven. If knowledge regarding the behaviors
that indicate an attachment figure's availability as a secure haven is
subject to segregation, then the infant can try to keep the attention of
their caregiver through clingy or aggressive behavior, or alternating
combinations of the two. Such behaviour may increase the availability of
an attachment figure who otherwise displays inconsistent or misleading
responses to the infant's attachment behaviors, suggesting the
unreliability of protection and safety.
Crittenden proposes that both kinds of information can be split
off from consciousness or behavioural expression as a 'strategy' to
maintain the availability of an attachment figure (See section above on
Disorganized/disoriented attachment for distinction of "Types"): "Type A
strategies were hypothesized to be based on reducing perception of
threat to reduce the disposition to respond. Type C was hypothesized to
be based on heightening perception of threat to increase the disposition
to respond."
Type A strategies split off emotional information about feeling
threatened and type C strategies split off temporally-sequenced
knowledge about how and why the attachment figure is available. By
contrast, type B strategies effectively utilize both kinds of
information without much distortion.
For example: a toddler may have come to depend upon a type C strategy
of tantrums in working to maintain the availability of an attachment
figure whose inconsistent availability has led the child to distrust or
distort causal information about their apparent behavior. This may lead
their attachment figure to get a clearer grasp on their needs and the
appropriate response to their attachment behaviors. Experiencing more
reliable and predictable information about the availability of their
attachment figure, the toddler then no longer needs to use coercive
behaviors with the goal of maintaining their caregiver's availability
and can develop a secure attachment to their caregiver since they trust
that their needs and communications will be heeded.
Significance of patterns
Research based on data from longitudinal studies, such as the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care
and the Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaption from Birth to Adulthood,
and from cross-sectional studies, consistently shows associations
between early attachment classifications and peer relationships as to
both quantity and quality. Lyons-Ruth, for example, found that "for each
additional withdrawing behavior displayed by mothers in relation to
their infant's attachment cues in the Strange Situation Procedure, the
likelihood of clinical referral by service providers was increased by
50%."
There is an extensive body of research demonstrating a
significant association between attachment organizations and children's
functioning across multiple domains.
Early insecure attachment does not necessarily predict difficulties,
but it is a liability for the child, particularly if similar parental
behaviours continue throughout childhood.
Compared to that of securely attached children, the adjustment of
insecure children in many spheres of life is not as soundly based,
putting their future relationships in jeopardy. Although the link is not
fully established by research and there are other influences besides
attachment, secure infants are more likely to become socially competent
than their insecure peers. Relationships formed with peers influence the
acquisition of social skills, intellectual development and the
formation of social identity. Classification of children's peer status
(popular, neglected or rejected) has been found to predict subsequent
adjustment.
Insecure children, particularly avoidant children, are especially
vulnerable to family risk. Their social and behavioral problems
increase or decline with deterioration or improvement in parenting.
However, an early secure attachment appears to have a lasting protective
function. As with attachment to parental figures, subsequent experiences may alter the course of development.
Studies have suggested that infants with a high-risk for Autism
Spectrum Disorders (ASD) may express attachment security differently
from infants with a low-risk for ASD.
Behavioral problems and social competence in insecure children increase
or decline with deterioration or improvement in quality of parenting
and the degree of risk in the family environment.
Some authors have questioned the idea that a taxonomy
of categories representing a qualitative difference in attachment
relationships can be developed. Examination of data from 1,139
15-month-olds showed that variation in attachment patterns was
continuous rather than grouped.
This criticism introduces important questions for attachment typologies
and the mechanisms behind apparent types. However, it has relatively
little relevance for attachment theory itself, which "neither requires
nor predicts discrete patterns of attachment."
There is some evidence that gender differences in attachment patterns of adaptive
significance begin to emerge in middle childhood. Insecure attachment
and early psychosocial stress indicate the presence of environmental
risk (for example poverty, mental illness, instability, minority status,
violence). Environmental risk can cause insecure attachment, while also
favoring the development of strategies for earlier reproduction.
Different reproductive strategies have different adaptive values for
males and females: Insecure males tend to adopt avoidant strategies,
whereas insecure females tend to adopt anxious/ambivalent strategies,
unless they are in a very high risk environment. Adrenarche is proposed as the endocrine mechanism underlying the reorganization of insecure attachment in middle childhood.
Changes in attachment during childhood and adolescence
Childhood
and adolescence allows the development of an internal working model
useful for forming attachments. This internal working model is related
to the individual's state of mind which develops with respect to
attachment generally and explores how attachment functions in
relationship dynamics based on childhood and adolescent experience. The
organization of an internal working model is generally seen as leading
to more stable attachments in those who develop such a model, rather
than those who rely more on the individual's state of mind alone in
forming new attachments.
Age, cognitive growth, and continued social experience advance the
development and complexity of the internal working model.
Attachment-related behaviors lose some characteristics typical of the
infant-toddler period and take on age-related tendencies. The preschool
period involves the use of negotiation and bargaining.
For example, four-year-olds are not distressed by separation if they
and their caregiver have already negotiated a shared plan for the
separation and reunion.
Ideally, these social skills become incorporated into the internal
working model to be used with other children and later with adult peers.
As children move into the school years at about six years old, most
develop a goal-corrected partnership with parents, in which each partner
is willing to compromise in order to maintain a gratifying
relationship.
By middle childhood, the goal of the attachment behavioral system has
changed from proximity to the attachment figure to availability.
Generally, a child is content with longer separations, provided
contact—or the possibility of physically reuniting, if needed—is
available. Attachment behaviours such as clinging and following decline
and self-reliance increases. By middle childhood (ages 7–11), there may
be a shift toward mutual coregulation
of secure-base contact in which caregiver and child negotiate methods
of maintaining communication and supervision as the child moves toward a
greater degree of independence.
The attachment system used by adolescents is seen as a "safety
regulating system" whose main function is to promote physical and
psychological safety. There are 2 different events that can trigger the
attachment system. Those triggers include, the presence of a potential
danger or stress, internal and external, and a threat of accessibility
and or availability of an attachment figure. The ultimate goal of the
attachment system is security, so during a time of danger or
inaccessibility the behavioral system accepts felt security in the
context of the availability of protection. By adolescence we are able to
find security through a variety of things, such as food, exercise, and
social media.
Felt security can be achieved through a number of ways, and often
without the physical presence of the attachment figure. Higher levels of
maturity allows adolescent teens to more capably interact with their
environment on their own because the environment is perceived as less
threatening. Adolescents teens will also see an increase in cognitive,
emotional and behavioral maturity that dictates whether or not teens are
less likely to experience conditions that activate their need for an
attachment figure. For example, when teenagers get sick and stay home
from school, surely they want their parents to be home so they can take
care of them, but they are also able to stay home by themselves without
experiencing serious amounts of distress.
Here are the attachment style differences during adolescence.
Secure adolescents are expected to hold their mothers at a higher rate
than all other support figures, including father, boy and girlfriends,
and best friends.
Insecure adolescents identify more strongly with their peers than their
parents as their primary attachment figures. Their friends are seen as a
significantly strong source of attachment support.
Dismissing adolescents rate their parents as a less significant source
of attachment support and would consider themselves as their primary
attachment figure.
Preoccupied adolescents would rate their parents a their primary source
of attachment support and would consider themselves as a much less
significant source of attachment support.
Attachment in adults
Attachment theory was extended to adult romantic relationships
in the late 1980s by Cindy Hazan and Phillip Shaver. Four styles of
attachment have been identified in adults: secure, anxious-preoccupied,
dismissive-avoidant and fearful-avoidant. These roughly correspond to
infant classifications: secure, insecure-ambivalent, insecure-avoidant
and disorganized/disoriented.
Securely attached adults tend to have positive views of
themselves, their partners and their relationships. They feel
comfortable with intimacy and independence, balancing the two. Feeling
secure has to do with a person's perception of control, feelings of
competence, and a belief that they possess the ability to meet the
challenges that they will face in the future. With adults, attachments
in general, are more cumulative and much more multifaceted than earlier
attachments. There are more exchanges of support within someones inner
circle relationships or secure relationships, such as respect,
confiding, reassurance, sick care, talking about one's health, and
talking about things that could worry or upset them. these types of
exchanges provide adults with a secure base that they are able to depend
on on a daily basis.
Anxious-preoccupied adults seek high levels of intimacy,
approval and responsiveness from partners, becoming overly dependent.
They tend to be less trusting, have less positive views about themselves
and their partners, and may exhibit high levels of emotional
expressiveness, worry and impulsiveness in their relationships. the
anxiety that adults feel prevent the establishment of satisfactory
defense exclusion. Thus, it is possible that individuals that have been
anxiously attached to their attachment figure or figures have not been
able to develop sufficient defenses against separation anxiety. Because
their lack of preparation these individuals will then overreact to the
anticipation of separation or the actual separation from their
attachment figure. The anxiety comes from an individuals intense and/or
unstable relationship that live the anxious or preoccupied individual
in relatively defenseless. Adults with this attachment style tend to look way too far into things,
whether that's a text message or a face-to-face conversation. Their
thought and actions can lead to a painful cycle of self-fulfilling
prophecies and even self-sabotage. They often seek a dismissive-avoidant
partner.
Dismissive-avoidant adults desire a high level of
independence, often appearing to avoid attachment altogether. They view
themselves as self-sufficient, invulnerable to attachment feelings and
not needing close relationships. They tend to suppress their feelings,
dealing with conflict by distancing themselves from partners of whom
they often have a poor opinion. Adults lack the interest of forming
close relationships and maintaining emotional closeness with the people
around them. They have a great amount of distrust in others but at the
same time possess a positive model of self, they would prefer to invest
in their own ego skills. Because of their distrust they cannot be
convinced that other people have the ability to deliver emotional
support. They try to create high levels of self-esteem by investing
disproportionately in their abilities or accomplishments. These adults
maintain their positive views of self, based on their personal
achievements and competence rather than searching for and feeling
acceptance from others. These adults will explicitly reject or minimize
the importance of emotional attachment and passively avoid relationships
when they feel as though they are becoming too close. They strive for
self-reliance and independence. When it comes to the opinions of others
about themselves, they are very indifferent and are relatively hesitant
to positive feedback from their peers. Dismissive avoidance can also be
explained as the result of defensive deactivation of the attachment
system to avoid potential rejection, or genuine disregard for
interpersonal closeness.
Fearful-avoidant adults have mixed feelings about close
relationships, both desiring and feeling uncomfortable with emotional
closeness. They tend to mistrust their partners and view themselves as
unworthy. Like dismissive-avoidant adults, fearful-avoidant adults tend
to seek less intimacy, suppressing their feelings.
Sexually, securely attached individuals are less likely to be
involved in one-night stands or sexual activity outside of the primary
relationship, and more likely to report mutual initiation and enjoyment
of sex.
Dismissive-avoidant individuals tend to report activities
reflecting low psychological intimacy (one-night sex, extra-dyadic sex,
sex without love), as well as less enjoyment of physical contact.
Research has demonstrated that for both sexes, insecure-ambivalent
attachment was related to enjoyment of holding and caressing, but not of
more clearly sexual behaviors.
Relationally, insecure individuals tend to be partnered with
insecure individuals, and secure individuals with secure individuals.
Insecure relationships tend to be enduring but less emotionally
satisfying compared to the relationship(s) of two securely attached
individuals.
Attachment styles are activated from the first date onwards and
impact relationship dynamics and how a relationship ends. Secure
attachment has been shown to allow for better conflict resolution in a
relationship and for one's ability to exit an unsatisfying relationship
compared to other attachment types. Secure individuals authentic high
self-esteem and positive view of others allows for this as they are
confident that they will find another relationship. Secure attachment
has also shown to allow for the successful processing of relational
losses (e.g. death, rejection, infidelity, abandonment etc.) Attachment
has also been show to impact caregiving behavior in relationships, too
(Shaver & Cassidy, 2018)
Two main aspects of adult attachment have been studied. The
organization and stability of the mental working models that underlie
the attachment styles is explored by social psychologists interested in
romantic attachment.
Developmental psychologists interested in the individual's state of
mind with respect to attachment generally explore how attachment
functions in relationship dynamics and impacts relationship outcomes.
The organization of mental working models is more stable while the
individual's state of mind with respect to attachment fluctuates more.
Some authors have suggested that adults do not hold a single set of
working models. Instead, on one level they have a set of rules and
assumptions about attachment relationships in general. On another level
they hold information about specific relationships or relationship
events. Information at different levels need not be consistent.
Individuals can therefore hold different internal working models for
different relationships.
There are a number of different measures of adult attachment, the
most common being self-report questionnaires and coded interviews based
on the Adult Attachment Interview.
The various measures were developed primarily as research tools, for
different purposes and addressing different domains, for example
romantic relationships, platonic relationships, parental relationships
or peer relationships. Some classify an adult's state of mind with
respect to attachment and attachment patterns by reference to childhood
experiences, while others assess relationship behaviours and security
regarding parents and peers.
History
Maternal deprivation
The early thinking of the object relations school of psychoanalysis, particularly Melanie Klein,
influenced Bowlby. However, he profoundly disagreed with the prevalent
psychoanalytic belief that infants' responses relate to their internal
fantasy life rather than real-life events. As Bowlby formulated his
concepts, he was influenced by case studies on disturbed and delinquent
children, such as those of William Goldfarb published in 1943 and 1945.
Bowlby's contemporary René Spitz
observed separated children's grief, proposing that "psychotoxic"
results were brought about by inappropriate experiences of early care. A strong influence was the work of social worker and psychoanalyst James Robertson who filmed the effects of separation on children in hospital. He and Bowlby collaborated in making the 1952 documentary film A Two-Year Old Goes to the Hospital which was instrumental in a campaign to alter hospital restrictions on visits by parents.
In his 1951 monograph for the World Health Organization, Maternal Care and Mental Health,
Bowlby put forward the hypothesis that "the infant and young child
should experience a warm, intimate, and continuous relationship with his
mother in which both find satisfaction and enjoyment", the lack of
which may have significant and irreversible mental health consequences.
This was also published as Child Care and the Growth of Love for public consumption. The central proposition was influential but highly controversial. At the time there was limited empirical data and no comprehensive theory to account for such a conclusion.
Nevertheless, Bowlby's theory sparked considerable interest in the
nature of early relationships, giving a strong impetus to, (in the words
of Mary Ainsworth), a "great body of research" in an extremely
difficult, complex area.
Bowlby's work (and Robertson's films) caused a virtual revolution in
hospital visiting by parents, hospital provision for children's play,
educational and social needs and the use of residential nurseries. Over
time, orphanages were abandoned in favor of foster care or family-style
homes in most developed countries.
Following the publication of Maternal Care and Mental Health, Bowlby sought new understanding from the fields of evolutionary biology, ethology, developmental psychology, cognitive science
and control systems theory. He formulated the innovative proposition
that mechanisms underlying an infant's emotional tie to the caregiver(s)
emerged as a result of evolutionary pressure.
He set out to develop a theory of motivation and behaviour control
built on science rather than Freud's psychic energy model. Bowlby argued
that with attachment theory he had made good the "deficiencies of the
data and the lack of theory to link alleged cause and effect" of Maternal Care and Mental Health.
Ethology
Bowlby's attention was first drawn to ethology when he read Konrad Lorenz's 1952 publication in draft form (although Lorenz had published earlier work). Other important influences were ethologists Nikolaas Tinbergen and Robert Hinde. Bowlby subsequently collaborated with Hinde.
In 1953 Bowlby stated "the time is ripe for a unification of
psychoanalytic concepts with those of ethology, and to pursue the rich
vein of research which this union suggests." Konrad Lorenz had examined the phenomenon of "imprinting", a behavior characteristic of some birds and mammals which involves rapid learning of recognition by the young, of a conspecific or comparable object. After recognition comes a tendency to follow.
Certain types of learning are possible, respective to each applicable
type of learning, only within a limited age range known as a critical period.
Bowlby's concepts included the idea that attachment involved learning
from experience during a limited age period, influenced by adult
behavior. He did not apply the imprinting concept in its entirety to
human attachment. However, he considered that attachment behaviour was
best explained as instinctive, combined with the effect of experience,
stressing the readiness the child brings to social interactions.
Over time it became apparent there were more differences than
similarities between attachment theory and imprinting so the analogy was
dropped.
Ethologists expressed concern about the adequacy of some research on
which attachment theory was based, particularly the generalization to
humans from animal studies.
Schur, discussing Bowlby's use of ethological concepts (pre-1960)
commented that concepts used in attachment theory had not kept up with
changes in ethology itself.
Ethologists and others writing in the 1960s and 1970s questioned and
expanded the types of behavior used as indications of attachment.
Observational studies of young children in natural settings provided
other behaviors that might indicate attachment; for example, staying
within a predictable distance of the mother without effort on her part
and picking up small objects, bringing them to the mother but not to
others.
Although ethologists tended to be in agreement with Bowlby, they
pressed for more data, objecting to psychologists writing as if there
were an "entity which is 'attachment', existing over and above the
observable measures." Robert Hinde
considered "attachment behavior system" to be an appropriate term
which did not offer the same problems "because it refers to postulated
control systems that determine the relations between different kinds of
behavior."
Psychoanalysis
Psychoanalytic concepts influenced Bowlby's view of attachment, in particular, the observations by Anna Freud and Dorothy Burlingham of young children separated from familiar caregivers during World War II. However, Bowlby rejected psychoanalytical explanations for early infant bonds including "drive theory" in which the motivation for attachment derives from gratification of hunger and libidinal drives. He called this the "cupboard-love"
theory of relationships. In his view it failed to see attachment as a
psychological bond in its own right rather than an instinct derived from
feeding or sexuality. Based on ideas of primary attachment and Neo-Darwinism,
Bowlby identified what he saw as fundamental flaws in psychoanalysis:
the overemphasis of internal dangers rather than external threat, and
the view of the development of personality via linear phases with regression
to fixed points accounting for psychological distress. Bowlby instead
posited that several lines of development were possible, the outcome of
which depended on the interaction between the organism and the
environment. In attachment this would mean that although a developing
child has a propensity to form attachments, the nature of those
attachments depends on the environment to which the child is exposed.
From early in the development of attachment theory there was
criticism of the theory's lack of congruence with various branches of
psychoanalysis. Bowlby's decisions left him open to criticism from
well-established thinkers working on similar problems.
Internal working model
The philosopher Kenneth Craik
had noted the ability of thought to predict events. He stressed the
survival value of natural selection for this ability. A key component of
attachment theory is the attachment behavior system. where certain
behaviors have a predictable outcome (i.e. proximity) and serve as
self-preservation method (i.e. protection). All taking place outside of an individuals awareness, This internal working model
allows a person to try out alternatives mentally, using knowledge of
the past while responding to the present and future. Bowlby applied
Craik's ideas to attachment, when other psychologists were applying
these concepts to adult perception and cognition.
Infants absorb all sorts of complex social-emotional information
from the social interactions that they observe. They notice the helpful
and hindering behaviors of one person to another. From these
observations they develop expectations of how two characters should
behave, known as a "secure base script." These scripts provide as a
template of how attachment related events should unfold and they are the
building blocks of ones internal working models. The infant's
internal working model is developed in response to the infant's
experience based internal working models of self, and environment, with
emphasis on the caregiving environment and the outcomes of his or her
proximity-seeking behaviors. Theoretically, secure child and adult
script, would allow for an attachment situation where one person
successfully utilizes another as a secure base from which to explore and
as a safe haven in times of distress. In contrast, insecure individuals
would create attachment situations with more complications. For example, If the caregiver is accepting of these proximity-seeking
behaviors and grants access, the infant develops a secure organization;
if the caregiver consistently denies the infant access, an avoidant
organization develops; and if the caregiver inconsistently grants
access, an ambivalent organization develops.
In retrospect, internal working models are constant with and reflect
the primary relationship with our caregivers. Childhood attachment has a
direct impact on our adult relationships.
A parent's
internal working model that is operative in the attachment relationship
with her infant can be accessed by examining the parent's mental
representations.
Recent research has demonstrated that the quality of maternal
attributions as markers of maternal mental representations can be
associated with particular forms of maternal psychopathology and can be
altered in a relative short time-period by targeted psychotherapeutic
intervention.
Developments
In the 1970s, problems with viewing attachment as a trait (stable
characteristic of an individual) rather than as a type of behavior with
organizing functions and outcomes, led some authors to the conclusion
that attachment behaviors were best understood in terms of their
functions in the child's life.
This way of thinking saw the secure base concept as central to
attachment theory's logic, coherence, and status as an organizational
construct. Following this argument, the assumption that attachment is expressed identically in all humans cross-culturally was examined.
The research showed that though there were cultural differences, the
three basic patterns, secure, avoidant and ambivalent, can be found in
every culture in which studies have been undertaken, even where communal
sleeping arrangements are the norm.
Selection of the secure pattern is found in
the majority of children across cultures studied. This follows logically
from the fact that attachment theory provides for infants to adapt to
changes in the environment, selecting optimal behavioural strategies.
How attachment is expressed shows cultural variations which need to be
ascertained before studies can be undertaken; for example Gusii
infants are greeted with a handshake rather than a hug. Securely
attached Gusii infants anticipate and seek this contact. There are also
differences in the distribution of insecure patterns based on cultural
differences in child-rearing practices. The scholar Michael Rutter
in 1974 studied the importance of distinguishing between the
consequences of attachment deprivation upon intellectual retardation in
children and lack of development in the emotional growth in children.
Rutter's conclusion was that a careful delineation of maternal
attributes needed to be identified and differentiated for progress in
the field to continue.
The biggest challenge to the notion of the universality of
attachment theory came from studies conducted in Japan where the concept
of amae
plays a prominent role in describing family relationships. Arguments
revolved around the appropriateness of the use of the Strange Situation
procedure where amae is practiced. Ultimately research tended to confirm the universality hypothesis of attachment theory. Most recently a 2007 study conducted in Sapporo
in Japan found attachment distributions consistent with global norms
using the six-year Main and Cassidy scoring system for attachment
classification.
Critics in the 1990s such as J. R. Harris, Steven Pinker and Jerome Kagan were generally concerned with the concept of infant determinism (nature versus nurture), stressing the effects of later experience on personality. Building on the work on temperament of Stella Chess,
Kagan rejected almost every assumption on which attachment theory's
cause was based. Kagan argued that heredity was far more important than
the transient developmental effects of early environment. For example, a
child with an inherently difficult temperament would not elicit
sensitive behavioural responses from a caregiver. The debate spawned
considerable research and analysis of data from the growing number of
longitudinal studies. Subsequent research has not borne out Kagan's
argument, possibly suggesting that it is the caregiver's behaviours that
form the child's attachment style, although how this style is expressed
may differ with the child's temperament.
Harris and Pinker put forward the notion that the influence of parents
had been much exaggerated, arguing that socialization took place
primarily in peer groups. H. Rudolph Schaffer concluded that parents and
peers had different functions, fulfilling distinctive roles in
children's development. Psychoanalyst/psychologists Peter Fonagy
and Mary Target have attempted to bring attachment theory and
psychoanalysis into a closer relationship through cognitive science as mentalization.
Mentalization, or theory of mind, is the capacity of human beings to
guess with some accuracy what thoughts, emotions and intentions lie
behind behaviours as subtle as facial expression.
It has been speculated that this connection between theory of mind and
the internal working model may open new areas of study, leading to
alterations in attachment theory.
Since the late 1980s, there has been a developing rapprochement between
attachment theory and psychoanalysis, based on common ground as
elaborated by attachment theorists and researchers, and a change in what
psychoanalysts consider to be central to psychoanalysis. Object relations
models which emphasize the autonomous need for a relationship have
become dominant and are linked to a growing recognition within
psychoanalysis of the importance of infant development in the context of
relationships and internalized representations. Psychoanalysis has
recognized the formative nature of a child's early environment including
the issue of childhood trauma. A psychoanalytically based exploration
of the attachment system and an accompanying clinical approach has
emerged together with a recognition of the need for measurement of
outcomes of interventions.
One focus of attachment research has been the difficulties of
children whose attachment history was poor, including those with
extensive non-parental child care experiences. Concern with the effects
of child care was intense during the so-called "day care wars" of the
late-20th century, during which some authors stressed the deleterious
effects of day care.
As a result of this controversy, training of child care professionals
has come to stress attachment issues, including the need for
relationship-building by the assignment of a child to a specific
care-giver. Although only high-quality child care settings are likely to
provide this, more infants in child care receive attachment-friendly
care than in the past. A natural experiment
permitted extensive study of attachment issues as researchers followed
thousands of Romanian orphans adopted into Western families after the
end of the Nicolae Ceauşescu regime. The English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team, led by Michael Rutter,
followed some of the children into their teens, attempting to unravel
the effects of poor attachment, adoption, new relationships, physical
problems and medical issues associated with their early lives. Studies
of these adoptees, whose initial conditions were shocking, yielded
reason for optimism as many of the children developed quite well.
Researchers noted that separation from familiar people is only one of
many factors that help to determine the quality of development.
Although higher rates of atypical insecure attachment patterns were
found compared to native-born or early-adopted samples, 70% of
later-adopted children exhibited no marked or severe attachment disorder
behaviors.
Authors considering attachment in non-Western cultures have noted
the connection of attachment theory with Western family and child care
patterns characteristic of Bowlby's time.
As children's experience of care changes, so may attachment-related
experiences. For example, changes in attitudes toward female sexuality
have greatly increased the numbers of children living with their
never-married mothers or being cared for outside the home while the
mothers work. This social change has made it more difficult for
childless people to adopt infants in their own countries. There has been
an increase in the number of older-child adoptions and adoptions from
third-world sources in first-world countries. Adoptions and births to
same-sex couples have increased in number and gained legal protection,
compared to their status in Bowlby's time. Issues have been raised to the effect that the dyadic
model characteristic of attachment theory cannot address the complexity
of real-life social experiences, as infants often have multiple
relationships within the family and in child care settings. It is suggested these multiple relationships influence one another reciprocally, at least within a family.
Principles of attachment theory have been used to explain adult
social behaviors, including mating, social dominance and hierarchical
power structures, in-group identification, group coalitions, and negotiation of reciprocity and justice.
Those explanations have been used to design parental care training, and
have been particularly successful in the design of child abuse
prevention programs.
While a wide variety of studies have upheld the basic tenets of
attachment theory, research has been inconclusive as to whether
self-reported early attachment and later depression are demonstrably
related.
Biology of attachment
In addition to longitudinal studies, there has been psychophysiological research on the biology of attachment. Research has begun to include neural development, behavior genetics and temperament concepts.
Generally, temperament and attachment constitute separate developmental
domains, but aspects of both contribute to a range of interpersonal and
intrapersonal developmental outcomes.
Some types of temperament may make some individuals susceptible to the
stress of unpredictable or hostile relationships with caregivers in the
early years.
In the absence of available and responsive caregivers it appears that
some children are particularly vulnerable to developing attachment
disorders.
The quality of caregiving received at infancy and childhood directly affects an individuals neurological systems systems which controls stress regulation. In psychophysiological research on attachment, the two main areas studied have been autonomic responses, such as heart rate or respiration, and the activity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. a system that is responsible for the body's reaction to stress. Infants' physiological responses have been measured during the Strange Situation procedure looking at individual differences in infant temperament and the extent to which attachment acts as a moderator. Recent studies convey that early attachment relationships become molecularly instilled into the being, thus affecting later immune system functioning. Empirical Evidence communicates that early negative experiences produces pro inflammatory phenotype cells in the immune system, which is directly related to cardiovascular disease, autoimmune diseases, and certain types of cancer.
Recent improvements involving methods of research, have enabled
researchers to further investigate the neural correlates of attachment
in humans. These advances include identifying key brain structures,
neural circuits, neurotransmitter systems, and neuropeptides are all
involved in attachment system functioning and can tell us more about a
certain individual, even predict their behavior. There is initial evidence that caregiving and attachment involve both unique and overlapping brain regions Another issue is the role of inherited genetic factors in shaping attachments: for example one type of polymorphism of the gene coding for the D2 dopamine receptor has been linked to anxious attachment and another in the gene for the 5-HT2A serotonin receptor with avoidant attachment. studies show that attachment in adulthood is simultaneously related to
biomarkers of immunity. For example, individuals with an avoidance
attachment attachment style produce higher levels of the pro
inflammatory cytokine interleukin-6 (IL-6) when reacting to an
interpersonal stressor
While individuals representing an anxious attachment style you tend to
see elevated cortisol production and lower numbers of T cells. .Although
children vary genetically and each individual requires different
attachment relationships, there is consistent evidence that maternal
warmth during infancy and childhood creates a safe haven for individuals
resulting in superior immune system functioning.
One theoretical basis for this is that it makes biological sense for
children to vary in their susceptibility to rearing influence.
Practical applications
As a theory of socioemotional development,
attachment theory has implications and practical applications in social
policy, decisions about the care and welfare of children and mental
health.
Child care policies
Social
policies concerning the care of children were the driving force in
Bowlby's development of attachment theory. The difficulty lies in
applying attachment concepts to policy and practice. In 2008 C.H. Zeanah
and colleagues stated, "Supporting early child-parent relationships is
an increasingly prominent goal of mental health practitioners,
community-based service providers and policy makers ... Attachment
theory and research have generated important findings concerning early
child development and spurred the creation of programs to support early
child-parent relationships."
Seeing that early child parent relationships is crucial for child
development. You would assume that individuals would be educated on
child development and the importance of the early parent-child
relationship. However, our current education system is more concerned
with sex education rather than healthy relationship tactics and skills
needed to parent in a sensitive, responsive manner. If Researchers and
educators could work together and develop future-parent curriculum for
the high school and university levels, It would benefit society as a
whole. With the dynamic nature of social and economic environments,
many families struggle balancing work and family responsibilities.
Finding quality childcare while at work or school is another issue for
many families.NIHD recent study convey that top notch day care
contributes to secure attachment relationships in children.
people have commented on this matter stating that “legislative
initiatives reflecting higher standards for credentialing and licensing
childcare workers, requiring education in child development and
attachment theory, and at least a two-year associate's degree course as
well as salary increases and increased stature for childcare positions” Corporations should implement more flexible work arrangements that
recognize child care as essential for all its employees. This includes
re-examination of parental leave policies. Too many parents are forced
to return to work too soon post childbirth because of company policy or
financial necessity. No matter the reason this inhibits early parent
child bonding.
In addition to this, there should be increased attention to the
training and screening of childcare workers. In his article reviewing
Attachment Theory, Sweeney suggested, among several policy implications,
“legislative initiatives reflecting higher standards for credentialing
and licensing childcare workers, requiring education in child
development and attachment theory, and at least a two-year associate's
degree course as well as salary increases and increased stature for
childcare positions”
Historically, attachment theory had significant policy
implications for hospitalized or institutionalized children, and those
in poor quality daycare.
Controversy remains over whether non-maternal care, particularly in
group settings, has deleterious effects on social development. It is
plain from research that poor quality care carries risks but that those
who experience good quality alternative care cope well although it is
difficult to provide good quality, individualized care in group
settings.
Attachment theory has implications in residence and contact disputes,
and applications by foster parents to adopt foster children. In the
past, particularly in North America, the main theoretical framework was
psychoanalysis. Increasingly attachment theory has replaced it, thus
focusing on the quality and continuity of caregiver relationships rather
than economic well-being or automatic precedence of any one party, such
as the biological mother. Rutter
noted that in the UK, since 1980, family courts have shifted
considerably to recognize the complications of attachment relationships.
Children tend to have attachment relationships with both parents and
often grandparents or other relatives. Judgements need to take this into
account along with the impact of step-families. Attachment theory has
been crucial in highlighting the importance of social relationships in
dynamic rather than fixed terms.
Attachment theory can also inform decisions made in social work, especially in humanistic social work (Petru Stefaroi), and court processes about foster care or other placements. Considering the child's attachment needs can help determine the level of risk posed by placement options.
Within adoption, the shift from "closed" to "open" adoptions and the
importance of the search for biological parents would be expected on the
basis of attachment theory. Many researchers in the field were strongly
influenced by it.
Clinical practice in children
Although attachment theory has become a major scientific theory of socioemotional development with one of the widest research lines in modern psychology, it has, until recently, been less used in clinical practice.
The
attachment theory focused on the attention of the child when the mother
is there and the responses that the child shows when the mother leaves,
which indicated the attachment and bonding of the mother and the child.
The attention therapy is the done while the child is being restrained
by the therapists and the responses displayed were noted. The tests were
done to show the responses of the child.
This may be partly due to lack of attention paid to clinical
application by Bowlby himself and partly due to broader meanings of the
word 'attachment' used among practitioners. It may also be partly due
to the mistaken association of attachment theory with the pseudoscientific interventions misleadingly known as "attachment therapy".
Prevention and treatment
In 1988, Bowlby published a series of lectures indicating how
attachment theory and research could be used in understanding and
treating child and family disorders. His focus for bringing about change
was the parents' internal working models, parenting behaviors and the
parents' relationship with the therapeutic intervenor. Ongoing research has led to a number of individual treatments and prevention and intervention programs.
In regards to personal development, children from all the age groups
were tested to show the effectiveness of the theory that is being
theorized by Bowlby. They range from individual therapy to public health
programs to interventions designed for foster caregivers. For infants
and younger children, the focus is on increasing the responsiveness and
sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, placing the
child with a different caregiver.
An assessment of the attachment status or caregiving responses of the
caregiver is invariably included, as attachment is a two-way process
involving attachment behavior and caregiver response. Some programs
are aimed at foster carers because the attachment behaviors of infants
or children with attachment difficulties often do not elicit appropriate
caregiver responses. Modern prevention and intervention programs have
proven successful.
Reactive attachment disorder and attachment disorder
One atypical attachment pattern is considered to be an actual disorder, known as reactive attachment disorder or RAD, which is a recognized psychiatric diagnosis (ICD-10 F94.1/2 and DSM-IV-TR
313.89). Against common misconception, this is not the same as
'disorganized attachment'. The essential feature of reactive attachment
disorder is markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social
relatedness in most contexts that begins before age five years,
associated with gross pathological care. There are two subtypes, one
reflecting a disinhibited attachment pattern, the other an inhibited
pattern. RAD is not a description of insecure attachment styles, however
problematic those styles may be; instead, it denotes a lack of
age-appropriate attachment behaviors that may appear to resemble a
clinical disorder.
Although the term "reactive attachment disorder" is now popularly
applied to perceived behavioural difficulties that fall outside the DSM
or ICD criteria, particularly on the Web and in connection with the
pseudo-scientific attachment therapy, "true" RAD is thought to be rare.
"Attachment disorder" is an ambiguous term, which may be used to
refer to reactive attachment disorder or to the more problematical
insecure attachment styles (although none of these are clinical
disorders). It may also be used to refer to proposed new classification
systems put forward by theorists in the field, and is used within attachment therapy as a form of unvalidated diagnosis.
One of the proposed new classifications, "secure base distortion" has
been found to be associated with caregiver traumatization.
Clinical practice in adults and families
As
attachment theory offers a broad, far-reaching view of human
functioning, it can enrich a therapist's understanding of patients and
the therapeutic relationship rather than dictate a particular form of
treatment. Some forms of psychoanalysis-based therapy for adults—within relational psychoanalysis and other approaches—also incorporate attachment theory and patterns.