Disgust is an emotional response of rejection or revulsion to something potentially contagious or something considered offensive, distasteful, or unpleasant. In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Charles Darwin wrote that disgust is a sensation that refers to something revolting. Disgust is experienced primarily in relation to the sense of taste (either perceived or imagined), and secondarily to anything which causes a similar feeling by sense of smell, touch, or vision. Musically sensitive people may even be disgusted by the cacophony of inharmonious sounds. Research continually has proven a relationship between disgust and anxiety disorders such as arachnophobia, blood-injection-injury type phobias, and contamination fear related obsessive–compulsive disorder (also known as OCD).
Disgust is one of the basic emotions of Robert Plutchik's theory of emotions and has been studied extensively by Paul Rozin. It invokes a characteristic facial expression, one of Paul Ekman's six universal facial expressions of emotion. Unlike the emotions of fear, anger, and sadness, disgust is associated with a decrease in heart rate.
Evolutionary significance
It is believed that the emotion of disgust has evolved as a response to offensive foods that may cause harm to the organism. A common example of this is found in human beings who show disgust reactions to mouldy milk or contaminated meat. Disgust appears to be triggered by objects or people who possess attributes that signify disease.
Self-report and behavioural studies found that disgust elicitors include:
- body products (feces, urine, vomit, sexual fluids, saliva, and mucus);
- foods (spoiled foods);
- animals (fleas, ticks, lice, cockroaches, worms, flies, rats, and mice);
- hygiene (visible dirt and "inappropriate" acts [e.g., using an unsterilized surgical instrument]);
- body envelope violations (blood, gore, and mutilation);
- death (dead bodies and organic decay);
- visible signs of infection
The above-mentioned main disgust stimuli are similar to one another
in the sense that they can all potentially transmit infections, and are
the most common referenced elicitors of disgust cross-culturally.
Because of this, disgust is believed to have evolved as a component of a
behavioral immune system in which the body attempts to avoid
disease-carrying pathogens in preference to fighting them after they have entered the body. This behavioral immune system
has been found to make sweeping generalizations because "it is more
costly to perceive a sick person as healthy than to perceive a healthy
person as sickly".
Researchers have found that sensitivity to disgust is negatively
correlated to aggression because feelings of disgust typically bring
about a need to withdraw while aggression results in a need to approach.
This can be explained in terms of each of the types of disgust. For
those especially sensitive to moral disgust, they would want to be less
aggressive because they want to avoid hurting others. Those especially
sensitive to pathogen disgust might be motivated by a desire to avoid
the possibility of an open wound on the victim of the aggression;
however, for those sensitive to sexual disgust, some sexual object must
be present for them to be especially avoidant of aggression.
Based on these findings, disgust may be used as an emotional tool to
decrease aggression in individuals. Disgust may produce specific autonomic
responses, such as reduced blood pressure, lowered heart-rate and
decreased skin conductance along with changes in respiratory behavior.
Research has also found that people who are more sensitive to
disgust tend to find their own in-group more attractive and tend to have
more negative attitudes toward other groups.
This may be explained by assuming that people begin to associate
outsiders and foreigners with disease and danger while simultaneously
associating health, freedom from disease, and safety with people similar
to themselves.
Taking a further look into hygiene, disgust was the strongest
predictor of negative attitudes toward obese individuals. A disgust
reaction to obese individuals was also connected with views of moral
values.
Domains of disgust
Tybur, et al., outlines three domains of disgust: pathogen disgust, which "motivates the avoidance of infectious microorganisms"; sexual disgust, "which motivates the avoidance of [dangerous] sexual partners and behaviors"; and moral disgust, which motivates people to avoid breaking social norms. Disgust may have an important role in certain forms of morality.
Pathogen disgust
arises from a desire to survive and, ultimately, a fear of death. He
compares it to a "behavioral immune system" that is the 'first line of
defense' against potentially deadly agents such as dead bodies, rotting
food, and vomit.
Sexual disgust arises from a desire to avoid "biologically costly
mates" and a consideration of the consequences of certain reproductive
choices. The two primary considerations are intrinsic quality (e.g.,
body symmetry, facial attractiveness, etc.) and genetic compatibility
(e.g., avoidance of inbreeding such as the incest taboo).
Moral disgust "pertains to social transgressions" and may include
behaviors such as lying, theft, murder, and rape. Unlike the other two
domains, moral disgust "motivates avoidance of social relationships with
norm-violating individuals" because those relationships threaten group
cohesion.
Gender differences
Women
generally report greater disgust than men, especially regarding sexual
disgust or general repulsiveness which have been argued to be consistent
with women being more selective regarding sex for evolutionary reasons.
Sensitivity to disgust rises during pregnancy, along with levels of the hormone progesterone.
Scientists have conjectured that pregnancy requires the mother to "dial
down" her immune system so that the developing embryo won't be
attacked. To protect the mother, this lowered immune system is then
compensated by a heightened sense of disgust.
Because disgust is an emotion with physical responses to
undesirable or dirty situations, studies have proven there are
cardiovascular and respiratory changes while experiencing the emotion of
disgust.
As mentioned earlier, women experience disgust more prominently
than men. This is reflected in a study about dental phobia. A dental
phobia comes from experiencing disgust when thinking about the dentist
and all that entails. 4.6 percent of women compared to 2.7 percent of
men find the dentist disgusting.
Body language
In a series of significant studies by Paul Ekman
in the 1970s, it was discovered that facial expressions of emotion are
not culturally determined, but universal across human cultures and thus
likely to be biological in origin.
The facial expression of disgust was found to be one of these facial
expressions. This characteristic facial expression includes slightly
narrowed brows, a curled upper lip, wrinkling of the nose and visible
protrusions of the tongue, although different elicitors may produce
different forms of this expression. It was found that the facial expression of disgust is readily recognizable across cultures. This facial expression is also produced in blind individuals and is correctly interpreted by individuals born deaf.
This evidence indicates an innate biological basis for the expression
and recognition of disgust. The recognition of disgust is also important
among species as it has been found that when an individual sees a
conspecific looking disgusted after tasting a particular food, he or she
automatically infers that the food is bad and should not be eaten.
This evidence suggests that disgust is experienced and recognized
almost universally and strongly implicates its evolutionary
significance.
Facial feedback
has also been implicated in the expression of disgust. That is, the
making of the facial expression of disgust leads to an increased feeling
of disgust. This can occur if the person just wrinkles one's nose
without awareness that they are making a disgust expression.
The mirror-neuron matching system found in monkeys and humans is a
proposed explanation for such recognition, and shows that our internal
representation of actions is triggered during the observation of
another’s actions.
It has been demonstrated that a similar mechanism may apply to
emotions. Seeing someone else's facial emotional expressions triggers
the neural activity that would relate to our own experience of the same
emotion. This points to the universality, as well as survival value of the emotion of disgust.
Children's reactions to a face showing disgust
At
a very young age, children are able to identify different, basic facial
emotions. If a parent makes a negative face and a positive emotional
face toward two different toys, a child as young as five months would
avoid the toy associated with a negative face. Young children tend to
associate a face showing disgust with anger instead of being able to
identify the difference. Adults, however, are able to make the
distinction. The age of understanding seems to be around ten years old.
Cultural differences
Because
disgust is partially a result of social conditioning, there are
differences among different cultures in the objects of disgust.
Americans "are more likely to link feelings of disgust to actions that
limit a person's rights or degrade a person's dignity" while Japanese
people "are more likely to link feelings of disgust to actions that
frustrate their integration into the social world".
Practices construed as socially acceptable, may also be met with
reactions of aversion by other cultures. For example, instead of
kissing, mothers from the Manchu minority ethnic group, as only researched in the 1900s in Aigun of Northern Manchuria where the researcher S. M. Shirokogoroff personally believed the Manchu element were "purer" than those of Southern Manchuria and Peking, used to show affection for their children by performing fellatio
on their male babies, placing the penis in their mouths and stimulating
it, while the Manchu regarded public kissing with revulsion.
Also, Chinese and Vietnamese culture directly advocate consuming human
placenta. Chinese nursing mothers were suggested to boil the placenta
and drink the broth to improve the quality of their milk. Similiarly,
Chinese also consume the bull penis soup for health purpose.
Disgust is one of the basic emotions recognizable across multiple
cultures and is a response to something revolting typically involving
taste or sight. Though different cultures find different things
disgusting, the reaction to the grotesque things remains the same
throughout each culture; people and their emotional reactions in the
realm of disgust remain the same.
Neural basis
The
scientific attempts to map specific emotions onto underlying neural
substrates dates back to the first half of the 20th century.
Functional MRI experiments have revealed that the anterior insula
in the brain is particularly active when experiencing disgust, when
being exposed to offensive tastes, and when viewing facial expressions
of disgust. The research has supported that there are independent neural systems in the brain, each handling a specific basic emotion.
Specifically, f-MRI studies have provided evidence for the activation
of the insula in disgust recognition, as well as visceral changes in
disgust reactions such as the feeling of nausea.
The importance of disgust recognition and the visceral reaction of
"feeling disgusted" is evident when considering the survival of
organisms, and the evolutionary benefit of avoiding contamination.
Insula
The insula (or insular cortex), is the main neural structure involved in the emotion of disgust.
The insula has been shown by several studies to be the main neural
correlate of the feeling of disgust both in humans and in macaque
monkeys. The insula is activated by unpleasant tastes, smells, and the
visual recognition of disgust in conspecific organisms.
The anterior insula is an olfactory and gustatory center that controls visceral sensations and the related autonomic responses.
It also receives visual information from the anterior portion of the
ventral superior temporal cortex, where cells have been found to respond
to the sight of faces.
The posterior insula is characterized by connections with auditory, somatosensory, and premotor areas, and is not related to the olfactory or gustatory modalities.
The fact that the insula is necessary for our ability to feel and
recognize the emotion of disgust is further supported by
neuropsychological studies. Both Calder (2000) and Adolphs (2003)
showed that lesions on the anterior insula lead to deficits in the
experience of disgust and recognizing facial expressions of disgust in
others.
The patients also reported having reduced sensations of disgust
themselves. Furthermore, electrical stimulation of the anterior insula
conducted during neurosurgery triggered nausea, the feeling of wanting
to throw up and uneasiness in the stomach. Finally, electrically
stimulating the anterior insula through implanted electrodes produced
sensations in the throat and mouth that were "difficult to stand".
These findings demonstrate the role of the insula in transforming
unpleasant sensory input into physiological reactions, and the
associated feeling of disgust.
Studies have demonstrated that the insula is activated by
disgusting stimuli, and that observing someone else's facial expression
of disgust seems to automatically retrieve a neural representation of
disgust. Furthermore, these findings emphasize the role of the insula in feelings of disgust.
One particular neuropsychological study focused on patient NK who
was diagnosed with a left hemisphere infarction involving the insula,
internal
capsule, putamen and globus pallidus. NK’s neural damage included the
insula and putamen and it was found that NK’s overall response to disgust-inducing stimuli was significantly lower than that of controls.
The patient showed a reduction in disgust-response on eight categories
including food, animals, body products, envelope violation and death.
Moreover, NK incorrectly categorized disgust facial expressions as
anger. The results of this study support the idea that NK suffered
damage to a system involved in recognizing social signals of disgust,
due to a damaged insula caused by neurodegeneration.
Disorders
Huntington's disease
Many patients suffering from Huntington's disease,
a genetically transmitted progressive neurodegenerative disease, are
unable to recognize expressions of disgust in others and also don't show
reactions of disgust to foul odors or tastes. The inability to recognize expressions of disgust appears in carriers of the Huntington gene before other symptoms appear.
People with Huntington's disease are impaired at recognition of anger
and fear, and experience a notably severe problem with disgust
recognition.
Major depressive disorder
Patients suffering from major depression have been found to display greater brain activation to facial expressions of disgust.
Obsessive-compulsive disorder
The emotion of disgust may have an important role in understanding the neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), particularly in those with contamination preoccupations.
In a study by Shapira & colleagues (2003), eight OCD subjects with
contamination preoccupations and eight healthy volunteers viewed
pictures from the International Affective Picture System during f-MRI
scans. OCD subjects showed significantly greater neural responses to
disgust-invoking images, specifically in the right insula.
Furthermore, Sprengelmeyer (1997) found that the brain activation
associated with disgust included the insula and part of the gustatory
cortex that processes unpleasant tastes and smells. OCD subjects and
healthy volunteers showed activation patterns in response to disgust
pictures that differed significantly at the right insula. In contrast,
the two groups were similar in their response to threat-inducing
pictures, with no significant group differences at any site.
Animal research
With respect to studies using rats,
prior research of signs of a conditioned disgust response have been
experimentally verified by Grill and Norgren (1978) who developed a
systematic test to assess palatability. The Taste Reactivity (TR) test has thus become a standard tool in measuring disgust response. When given a stimulus intraorally which had been previously paired with a nausea-inducing
substance, rats will show conditioned disgust reactions. "Gaping" in
rats is the most dominant conditioned disgust reaction and the muscles
used in this response mimic those used in species capable of vomiting. Recent studies have shown that treatments that reduced serotonin availability or that activate the endocannabinoid system
can interfere with the expression of a conditioned disgust reaction in
rats. These researchers showed that as nausea produced conditioned
disgust reactions, by administering the rats with an antinausea
treatment they could prevent toxin-induced conditioned disgust
reactions. Furthermore, in looking at the different disgust and vomiting
reactions between rats and shrews the authors showed that these
reactions (particularly vomiting) play a crucial role in the associative
processes that govern food selection across species.
In discussing specific neural locations of disgust, research has
shown that forebrain mechanisms are necessary for rats to acquire
conditioned disgust for a specific emetic (vomit-inducing) substance
(such as lithium chloride). Other studies have shown that lesions to the area postrema and the parabrachial nucleus of the pons but not the nucleus of the solitary tract prevented conditioned disgust. Moreover, lesions of the dorsal and medial raphe nuclei (depleting forebrain serotonin) prevented the establishment of lithium chloride-induced conditioned disgust.
Morality
Although
disgust was first thought to be a motivation for humans to only
physical contaminants, it has since been applied to moral and social
moral contaminants as well. The similarities between these types of
disgust can especially be seen in the way people react to the
contaminants. For example, if someone stumbles upon a pool of vomit,
he/she will do whatever possible to place as much distance between
himself/herself and the vomit as possible, which can include pinching
the nose, closing the eyes, or running away. Likewise, when a group
experiences someone who cheats, rapes, or murders another member of the
group, its reaction is to shun or expel that person from the group.
Jones & Fitness (2008) coined the term "moral hypervigilance"
to describe the phenomenon that individuals who are prone to physical
disgust are also prone to moral disgust. The link between physical
disgust and moral disgust can be seen in the United States where
criminals are often referred to as "slime" or "scum" and criminal
activity as "stinking" or being "fishy". Furthermore, people often try
to block out the stimuli of morally repulsive images in much the same
way that they would block out the stimuli of a physically repulsive
image. When people see an image of abuse, rape, or murder, they often
avert their gazes to inhibit the incoming visual stimuli from the
photograph just like they would if they saw a decomposing body.
Moral judgments can be traditionally defined or thought of as
directed by standards such as impartiality and respect towards others
for their well-being. From more recent theoretical and empirical
information, it can be suggested that morality may be guided by basic affective processes. Jonathan Haidt
proposed that one’s instant judgments about morality are experienced as
a "flash of intuition" and that these affective perceptions operate
rapidly, associatively, and outside of consciousness.
From this, moral intuitions are believed to be stimulated prior to
conscious moral cognitions which correlates with having a greater
influence on moral judgments.
Research suggests that the experience of disgust can alter moral
judgments. Many studies have focused on the average change in behavior
across participants, with some studies indicating disgust stimuli
intensifies the severity of moral judgments. However, additional studies have found the reverse effect, and recent studies have suggested that the average effect of disgust on moral judgments is small or absent.
Potentially reconciling these effects, a study recently indicated that
the direction and size of the effect of disgust stimuli on moral
judgment depends upon an individual's sensitivity to disgust.
The effect also seems to be limited to a certain aspect of
morality. Horberg et al. found that disgust plays a role in the
development and intensification of moral judgments of purity in
particular.
In other words, the feeling of disgust is often associated with a
feeling that some image of what is pure has been violated. For example, a
vegetarian might feel disgust after seeing another person eating meat
because he/she has a view of vegetarianism as the pure state-of-being.
When this state-of-being is violated, the vegetarian feels disgust.
Furthermore, disgust appears to be uniquely associated with purity
judgments, not with what is just/unjust or what is harmful/caregiving,
while other emotions such as fear, anger, and sadness are "unrelated to
moral judgments of purity".
Some other research suggests that an individual’s level of disgust sensitivity is due to their particular experience of disgust.
One’s disgust sensitivity can be either high or low. The higher one’s
disgust sensitivity is, the greater the tendency to make stricter moral
judgments.
Disgust sensitivity can also relate to various aspects of moral values,
which can have a negative or positive impact. For example, Disgust
sensitivity is associated with moral hypervigilance, which means people
who have higher disgust sensitivity are more likely to think that other
people who are suspects of a crime are more guilty. They also associate
them as being morally evil and criminal, thus endorsing them to harsher
punishment in the setting of a court.
Disgust is also theorized as an evaluative emotion that can control moral behavior.
When one experiences disgust, this emotion might signal that certain
behaviors, objects, or people are to be avoided in order to preserve
their purity.
Research has established that when the idea or concept of cleanliness
is made salient then people make less severe moral judgments of others.
From this particular finding, it can be suggested that this reduces the
experience of disgust and the ensuing threat of psychological impurity
diminishes the apparent severity of moral transgressions.
Political orientation
In one study, people of differing political persuasions were shown disgusting images in a brain scanner. In conservatives, the basal ganglia and amygdala and several other regions showed increased activity, while in liberals
other regions of the brain increased in activity. Both groups reported
similar conscious reactions to the images. The difference in activity
patterns was large: the reaction to a single image could predict a
person's political leanings with 95% accuracy.
Self-disgust
Although
limited research has been done on self-disgust, one study found that
self-disgust and severity of moral judgments were negatively correlated.
This is in contrast to findings related to disgust, which typically
results in harsher judgments of transgressions. This implies that
disgust directed towards the self functions very differently from
disgust directed towards other people or objects.
Self-disgust "may reflect a pervasive condition of self-loathing that
makes it difficult to assign deserving punishment to others".
In other words, those who feel self-disgust cannot easily condemn
others to punishment because they feel that they may also be deserving
of punishment.
Functions
The emotion of disgust can be described to serve as an affective
mechanism following occurrences of negative social value, provoking
repulsion, and desire for social distance. The origin of disgust can be defined by motivating the avoidance of offensive things, and in the context of a social environment, it can become an instrument of social avoidance. An example of disgust in action can be found from the Bible in the book of Leviticus. Leviticus includes direct commandments from God to avoid disgust causing individuals, which included people who were sexually immoral and those who had leprosy. Disgust is also known to have originally evolved as a response to unpleasant food that may have been carriers of disease.
As an effective instrument for reducing motivations for social interaction, disgust can be anticipated to interfere with dehumanization or the maltreatment of persons as less than human.
Research was performed which conducted several functional magnetic
resonance images (fMRI) in which participants viewed images of
individuals from stigmatized groups that were associated with disgust,
which were drug addicts and homeless people.
What the study found was that people were not inclined in making
inferences about the mental conditions of these particular disgust
inducing groups.
Therefore, examining images of homeless people and drug addicts caused
disgust in the response of the people who participated with this study. This study coincides with disgust following the law of contagion, which explains that contact with disgusting material renders one disgusting.
Disgust can be applied towards people and can function as maltreatment
towards another human being. Disgust can exclude people from being a
part of a clique
by leading to the view that they are merely less than human. An example
of this is if groups were to avoid people from outside of their own
particular group. Some researchers have distinguished between two
different forms of dehumanization. The first form is the denial of
uniquely human traits, examples include: products of culture and
modification. The second form is the denial of human nature, examples include: emotionality and personality.
Failure to attribute distinctively human traits to a group leads to animalistic dehumanization, which defines the object group or individual as savage, crude, and similar to animals. These forms of dehumanization have clear connections to disgust.
Researchers have proposed that many disgust elicitors are disgusting
because they are reminders that humans are not diverse from other
creatures.
With the aid of disgust, animalistic dehumanization directly reduces
one’s moral concerns towards excluding members from the outer group. Disgust can be a cause and consequence of dehumanization. Animalistic dehumanization may generate feelings of disgust and revulsion. Feelings of disgust, through rousing social distance,
may lead to dehumanization. Therefore, a person or group that is
generally connected with disgusting effects and seen as physically
unclean may induce moral avoidance. Being deemed disgusting produces a variety of cognitive effects that result in exclusion from the perceived inner group.
Political and legal aspects of disgust
The
emotion disgust has been noted to feature strongly in the public sphere
in relation to issues and debates, among other things, regarding anatomy, sex and bioethics. There is a range of views by different commentators on the role, purpose and effects of disgust on public discourse.
Leon Kass, a bioethicist,
has advocated that "in crucial cases...repugnance is the emotional
expression of deep wisdom, beyond reason's power fully to articulate
it." in relation to bio-ethical issues (See: Wisdom of repugnance).
Martha Nussbaum, a jurist and ethicist,
explicitly rejects disgust as an appropriate guide for legislating,
arguing the "politics of disgust" is an unreliable emotional reaction
with no inherent wisdom. Furthermore, she argues this "politics of
disgust" has in the past and present had the effects of supporting
bigotry in the forms of sexism, racism and antisemitism and links the
emotion of disgust to support for laws against Miscegenation and the oppressive caste system in India. In place of this "politics of disgust", Nussbaum argues for the Harm principle from John Stuart Mill as the proper basis for legislating. Nussbaum argues the harm principle supports the legal ideas of consent, the Age of majority and privacy
and protects citizens. She contrasts this with the "politics of
disgust" which she argues denies citizens humanity and equality before
the law on no rational grounds and cause palpable social harm. (See
Martha Nussbaum, From Disgust to Humanity: Sexual Orientation and Constitutional Law). Nussbaum published Hiding From Humanity: Disgust, Shame, and the Law in 2004; the book examines the relationship of disgust and shame
to a society's laws. Nussbaum identifies disgust as a marker that
bigoted, and often merely majoritarian, discourse employs to "place", by
diminishment and denigration, a despised minority. Removing "disgust"
from public discourse constitutes an important step in achieving humane
and tolerant democracies.
Leigh Turner
(2004) has argued that "reactions of disgust are often built upon
prejudices that should be challenged and rebutted." On the other hand,
writers, such as Kass, find wisdom in adhering to one's initial feelings
of disgust. A number of writers on the theory of disgust find it to be
the proto-legal foundation of human law.
Disgust has also figured prominently in the work of several other philosophers. Nietzsche became disgusted with the music and orientation of Richard Wagner, as well as other aspects of 19th century culture and morality. Jean-Paul Sartre wrote widely about experiences involving various negative emotions related to disgust.
The Hydra's Tale: Imagining Disgust
According to the book The Hydra's Tale: Imagining Disgust by Robert Rawdon Wilson,
disgust may be further subdivided into physical disgust, associated
with physical or metaphorical uncleanliness, and moral disgust, a
similar feeling related to courses of action. For example; "I am
disgusted by the hurtful things that you are saying." Moral disgust
should be understood as culturally determined;
physical disgust as more universally grounded. The book also discusses
moral disgust as an aspect of the representation of disgust. Wilson
does this in two ways. First, he discusses representations of disgust in
literature, film and fine art. Since there are characteristic facial
expressions (the clenched nostrils, the pursed lips)—as Charles Darwin, Paul Ekman,
and others have shown—they may be represented with more or less skill
in any set of circumstances imaginable. There may even be "disgust
worlds" in which disgust motifs so dominate that it may seem that entire
represented world is, in itself, disgusting. Second, since people know
what disgust is as a primary, or visceral, emotion (with characteristic
gestures and expressions), they may imitate it. Thus, Wilson argues
that, for example, contempt is acted out on the basis of the visceral
emotion, disgust, but is not identical with disgust. It is a "compound
affect" that entails intellectual preparation, or formatting, and
theatrical techniques. Wilson argues that there are many such
"intellectual" compound affects—such as nostalgia and outrage—but that
disgust is a fundamental and unmistakable example. Moral disgust, then,
is different from visceral disgust; it is more conscious and more
layered in performance.
Wilson links shame and guilt to disgust (now transformed, wholly or partially, into self-disgust) primarily as a consequence rooted in self-consciousness. Referring to a passage in Doris Lessing's The Golden Notebook, Wilson writes that "the dance between disgust and shame takes place. A slow choreography unfolds before the mind's-eye."
Wilson examines the claims of several jurists and legal
scholars—such as William Ian Miller—that disgust must underlie positive
law. "In the absence of disgust", he observes, stating their claim, ". .
. there would be either total barbarism or a society ruled solely by
force, violence and terror." The moral-legal argument, he remarks,
"leaves much out of account."
His own argument turns largely upon the human capacity to learn how to
control, even to suppress, strong and problematic affects and, over
time, for entire populations to abandon specific disgust responses.