In psychology, attitude is a psychological construct, a mental and emotional entity that inheres in, or characterizes a person.
They are complex and an acquired state through experiences. It is an
individual's predisposed state of mind regarding a value and it is
precipitated through a responsive expression toward a person, place,
thing, or event (the attitude object) which in turn influences the individual's thought and action. Prominent psychologist Gordon Allport described this latent psychological construct as "the most distinctive and indispensable concept in contemporary social psychology." Attitude can be formed from a person's past and present. Key topics in the study of attitudes include attitude strength, attitude change, consumer behavior, and attitude-behavior relationships.
Definitions
Social psychology
An
attitude is an evaluation of an attitude object, ranging from extremely
negative to extremely positive. Most contemporary perspectives on
attitudes also permit that people can also be conflicted or ambivalent
toward an object by simultaneously holding both positive and negative
attitudes toward the same object. This has led to some discussion of
whether individual can hold multiple attitudes toward the same object.
An attitude can be as a positive or negative evaluation of
people, objects, events, activities, and ideas. It could be concrete,
abstract or just about anything in your environment, but there is a
debate about precise definitions. Eagly and Chaiken, for example, define
an attitude as "a psychological tendency that is expressed by
evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favor or disfavor." Though it is sometimes common to define an attitude as affect
toward an object, affect (i.e., discrete emotions or overall arousal)
is generally understood as an evaluative structure used to form attitude
object.
Attitude may influence the attention to attitude objects, the use of
categories for encoding information and the interpretation, judgement
and recall of attitude-relevant information.
These influences tend to be more powerful for strong attitudes which
are accessible and based on elaborate supportive knowledge structure.
The durability and impact of influence depend upon the strength
formed from consistency of heuristics. Attitudes can guide encoding information, attention and behaviors, even if the individual is pursuing unrelated goals.
Jung's definition
Attitude is one of Jung's 57 definitions in Chapter XI of Psychological Types. Jung's definition of attitude is a "readiness of the psyche to act or react in a certain way".
Attitudes very often come in pairs, one conscious and the other
unconscious. Within this broad definition Jung defines several
attitudes.
The main (but not only) attitude dualities that Jung defines are the following.
- Consciousness and the unconscious. The "presence of two attitudes is extremely frequent, one conscious and the other unconscious. This means that consciousness has a constellation of contents different from that of the unconscious, a duality particularly evident in neurosis".
- Extraversion and introversion. This pair is so elementary to Jung's theory of types that he labeled them the "attitude-types".
- Rational and irrational attitudes. "I conceive reason as an attitude".
- The rational attitude subdivides into the thinking and feeling psychological functions, each with its attitude.
- The irrational attitude subdivides into the sensing and intuition psychological functions, each with its attitude. "There is thus a typical thinking, feeling, sensation, and intuitive attitude".
- Individual and social attitudes. Many of the latter are "isms".
In addition, Jung discusses the abstract attitude. “When I take an abstract attitude...”. Abstraction
is contrasted with creationism “CREATIONISM. By this I mean a
peculiarity of thinking and feeling which is the antithesis of
abstraction”.
Factors
Psychological
The
attitude of a person is determined by psychological factors like ideas,
values, beliefs, perception, etc. All these have a complex role in
determining a person's attitude. Values are ideals, guiding principles
in one’s life, or overarching goals that people strive to obtain (Maio
& Olson, 1998). Beliefs are cognitions about the world—subjective
probabilities that an object has a particular attribute or that an
action will lead to a particular outcome (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975).
Beliefs can be patently and unequivocally false. For example, surveys
show that a third of U.S. adults think that vaccines cause autism,
despite the preponderance of scientific research to the contrary (Dixon
et al., 2015).
It was found that beliefs like these are tenaciously held and highly
resistant to change. Another important factor that affects attitude is symbolic interactionism, these are rife with powerful symbols and charged with affect which can lead to a selective perception.
Persuasion theories says that in politics, successful persuaders
convince its message recipients into a selective perception or attitude
polarization for turning against the opposite candidate through a repetitive process
that they are in a noncommittal state and it is unacceptable and
doesn't have any moral basis for it and for this they only require to
chain the persuading message into a realm of plausibility (Gopnik, 2015
& O’Keefe, 2016).
Family
Family
plays a significant role in the primary stage of attitudes held by
individuals. Initially, a person develops certain attitudes from his
parents, brothers, sister, and elders in the family. There is a high
degree of relationship between parent and children in attitudes found in
them.
Society
Societies
play an important role in formatting the attitudes of an individual.
The culture, the tradition, the language, etc., influence a person's
attitudes. Society, tradition, and the culture teach individuals what is
and what is not acceptable.
Economic
A person's attitude also depends on issues such as his salary, status, work environment, work as such, etc.
Structure
The classic, tripartite view offered by Rosenberg and Hovland is that an attitude contains cognitive, affective, and behavioral
components. Empirical research, however, fails to support clear
distinctions between thoughts, emotions, and behavioral intentions
associated with a particular attitude.
A criticism of the tripartite view of attitudes is that it requires
cognitive, affective, and behavioral associations of an attitude to be
consistent, but this may be implausible. Thus some views of attitude
structure see the cognitive and behavioral components as derivative of
affect or affect and behavior as derivative of underlying beliefs.
Despite debate about the particular structure of attitudes, there
is considerable evidence that attitudes reflect more than evaluations
of a particular object that vary from positive to negative.
Among numerous attitudes, one example is people's money attitudes which
may help people understand their affective love of money motive,
stewardship behavior, and money cognition. These ABC components of
attitudes formulate, define, and contribute to an overall construct of Monetary Intelligence which, in turn, may be related to many theoretical work-related constructs.
There is also a considerable interest in intra-attitudinal and
inter-attitudinal structure, which is how an attitude is made
(expectancy and value) and how different attitudes relate to one
another. Which connects different attitudes to one another and to more
underlying psychological structures, such as values or ideology.
Attitude component models
An
influential model of attitude is the multicomponent model, where
attitudes are evaluations of an object that have affective, behavioral,
and cognitive components (the ABC model):
- Affective component The affective component of attitudes refer to your feelings or emotions linked to an attitude object. Affective responses influence attitudes in a number of ways. For example, many people are afraid/scared of spiders. So this negative affective response is likely to cause you to have a negative attitude towards spiders.
- Behavioural component The behavioral component of attitudes refer to past behaviours or experiences regarding an attitude object. The idea that people might infer their attitudes from their previous actions.
- Cognitive component The cognitive component of attitudes refer to the beliefs, thoughts, and attributes that we would associate with an object. Many times a person's attitude might be based on the negative and positive attributes they associate with an object.
MODE model
This is the theory of attitude evaluation (motivation and opportunity as determinants
of the attitude - behavior relation). When both are present, behavior
will be deliberate. When one is absent, impact on behavior will be
spontaneous. The MODE model was developed by Fazio. A person's attitude can be measured in two different ways:
- Explicit measure
- Implicit measure
Explicit measure are attitudes at the conscious level, that are
deliberately formed and easy to self-report. Implicit measures are
attitudes that are at an unconscious level, that are involuntarily
formed and are typically unknown to us.
Both explicit and implicit attitudes can shape an individual's
behavior. Implicit attitudes, however, are most likely to affect
behavior when the demands are steep and an individual feels
stressed or distracted.
Function
Another
classic view of attitudes is that attitudes serve particular functions
for individuals. That is, researchers have tried to understand why
individuals hold particular attitudes or why they hold attitudes in
general by considering how attitudes affect the individuals who hold
them. Daniel Katz,
for example, writes that attitudes can serve "instrumental, adjustive
or utilitarian," "ego-defensive," "value-expressive," or "knowledge"
functions. This functional attitude theory suggests that in order for attitudes to change (e.g., via persuasion),
appeals must be made to the function(s) that a particular attitude
serves for the individual. As an example, the "ego-defensive" function
might be used to influence the racially prejudicial attitudes of an
individual who sees themselves as open-minded and tolerant. By appealing
to that individual's image of themselves as tolerant and open-minded,
it may be possible to change their prejudicial attitudes to be more
consistent with their self-concept. Similarly, a persuasive message that threatens self-image is much more likely to be rejected.
Daniel Katz classified attitudes into four different groups based on their functions:
- Utilitarian: provides us with general approach or avoidance tendencies:
- Knowledge: help people organize and interpret new information:
- Ego-defensive: attitudes can help people protect their self-esteem;
- Value-expressive: used to express central values or beliefs.
Utilitarian
People adopt attitudes that are rewarding and that help them avoid
punishment. In other words, any attitude that is adopted in a person's
own self-interest is considered to serve a utilitarian function.
Consider you have a condo, people with condos pay property taxes, and as
a result you don't want to pay more taxes. If those factors lead to
your attitude that "increases in property taxes are bad" your attitude
is serving a utilitarian function.
Knowledge
People need to maintain an organized, meaningful, and stable view of the
world. That being said important values and general principles can
provide a framework for our knowledge. Attitudes achieve this goal by
making things fit together and make sense.
Example:
- I believe that I am a good person.
- I believe that good things happen to good people.
- Something bad happens to Bob.
- So I believe Bob must not be a good person.
Ego-Defensive
This function involves psychoanalytic principles where people use
defense mechanisms to protect themselves from psychological harm.
Mechanisms include:
The ego-defensive notion correlates nicely with Downward Comparison
Theory which holds the view that derogating a less fortunate other
increases our own subjective well-being. We are more likely to use the
ego-defensive function when we suffer a frustration or misfortune.
Value-Expressive
- Serves to express one's central values and self-concept.
- Central values tend to establish our identity and gain us social approval thereby showing us who we are, and what we stand for.
An example would concern attitudes toward a controversial political issue.
Formation
According
to Doob (1947), learning can account for most of the attitudes we hold.
The study of attitude formation is the study of how people form
evaluations of persons, places or things. Theories of classical
conditioning, instrumental conditioning and social learning are mainly
responsible for formation of attitude. Unlike personality, attitudes are expected to change as a function of experience.
In addition, exposure to the 'attitude' objects may have an effect on
how a person forms his or her attitude. This concept was seen as the
"Mere-Exposure Effect". Robert Zajonc showed that people were more
likely to have a positive attitude on 'attitude objects' when they were
exposed to it frequently than if they were not. Mere repeated exposure
of the individual to a stimulus is a sufficient condition for the
enhancement of his attitude toward it.
Tesser (1993) has argued that hereditary variables may affect
attitudes - but believes that they may do so indirectly. For example,
consistency theories, which imply that we must be consistent in our
beliefs and values. As with any type of heritability, to determine if a
particular trait has a basis in our genes, twin studies are used. The most famous example of such a theory is Dissonance-reduction theory, associated with Leon Festinger,
which explains that when the components of an attitude (including
belief and behavior) are at odds an individual may adjust one to match
the other (for example, adjusting a belief to match a behavior). Other theories include balance theory, originally proposed by Heider (1958), and the self-perception theory, originally proposed by Daryl Bem.
Change
Attitudes can be changed through persuasion
and an important domain of research on attitude change focuses on
responses to communication. Experimental research into the factors that
can affect the persuasiveness of a message include:
- Target characteristics: These are characteristics that refer to the person who receives and processes a message. One such trait is intelligence - it seems that more intelligent people are less easily persuaded by one-sided messages. Another variable that has been studied in this category is self-esteem. Although it is sometimes thought that those higher in self-esteem are less easily persuaded, there is some evidence that the relationship between self-esteem and persuasibility is actually curvilinear, with people of moderate self-esteem being more easily persuaded than both those of high and low self-esteem levels (Rhodes & Woods, 1992). The mind frame and mood of the target also plays a role in this process.
- Source characteristics: The major source characteristics are expertise, trustworthiness and interpersonal attraction or attractiveness. The credibility of a perceived message has been found to be a key variable here; if one reads a report about health and believes it came from a professional medical journal, one may be more easily persuaded than if one believes it is from a popular newspaper. Some psychologists have debated whether this is a long-lasting effect and Hovland and Weiss (1951) found the effect of telling people that a message came from a credible source disappeared after several weeks (the so-called "sleeper effect"). Whether there is a sleeper effect is controversial. Perceived wisdom is that if people are informed of the source of a message before hearing it, there is less likelihood of a sleeper effect than if they are told a message and then told its source.
- Message Characteristics: The nature of the message plays a role in persuasion. Sometimes presenting both sides of a story is useful to help change attitudes. When people are not motivated to process the message, simply the number of arguments presented in a persuasive message will influence attitude change, such that a greater number of arguments will produce greater attitude change.
- Cognitive routes: A message can appeal to an individual's cognitive evaluation to help change an attitude. In the central route to persuasion the individual is presented with the data and motivated to evaluate the data and arrive at an attitude changing conclusion. In the peripheral route to attitude change, the individual is encouraged to not look at the content but at the source. This is commonly seen in modern advertisements that feature celebrities. In some cases, physician, doctors or experts are used. In other cases film stars are used for their attractiveness.
Emotion and attitude change
Emotion is a common component in persuasion, social influence, and attitude change.
Much of attitude research emphasized the importance of affective or
emotion components. Emotion works hand-in-hand with the cognitive
process, or the way we think, about an issue or situation. Emotional
appeals are commonly found in advertising, health campaigns and
political messages. Recent examples include no-smoking health campaigns
and political campaign advertising emphasizing the fear of terrorism.
Attitudes and attitude objects are functions of cognitive, affective, and
conative components. Attitudes are part of the brain’s associative
networks, the spider-like structures residing in long term memory that
consist of affective and cognitive nodes.
By activating an affective or emotion node, attitude change may
be possible, though affective and cognitive components tend to be
intertwined. In primarily affective networks, it is more difficult to
produce cognitive counterarguments in the resistance to persuasion and
attitude change.
Affective forecasting,
otherwise known as intuition or the prediction of emotion, also impacts
attitude change. Research suggests that predicting emotions is an
important component of decision making, in addition to the cognitive
processes. How we feel about an outcome may override purely cognitive
rationales.
In terms of research methodology, the challenge for researchers
is measuring emotion and subsequent impacts on attitude. Since we cannot
see into the brain, various models and measurement tools have been
constructed to obtain emotion and attitude information. Measures may
include the use of physiological cues like facial expressions, vocal
changes, and other body rate measures. For instance, fear is associated
with raised eyebrows, increased heart rate and increase body tension
(Dillard, 1994). Other methods include concept or network mapping, and
using primes or word cues in the era.
Components of emotion appeals
Any
discrete emotion can be used in a persuasive appeal; this may include
jealousy, disgust, indignation, fear, blue, disturbed, haunted, and
anger. Fear is one of the most studied emotional appeals in
communication and social influence research.
Important consequences of fear appeals and other emotion appeals
include the possibility of reactance which may lead to either message
rejections or source rejection and the absence of attitude change. As
the EPPM suggests, there is an optimal emotion level in motivating
attitude change. If there is not enough motivation, an attitude will not
change; if the emotional appeal is overdone, the motivation can be
paralyzed thereby preventing attitude change.
Emotions perceived as negative or containing threat are often
studied more than perceived positive emotions like humor. Though the
inner-workings of humor are not agreed upon, humor appeals may work by
creating incongruities in the mind. Recent research has looked at the
impact of humor on the processing of political messages. While evidence
is inconclusive, there appears to be potential for targeted attitude
change is receivers with low political message involvement.
Important factors that influence the impact of emotion appeals
include self efficacy, attitude accessibility, issue involvement, and
message/source features. Self efficacy is a perception of one’s own
human agency; in other words, it is the perception of our own ability to
deal with a situation. It is an important variable in emotion appeal
messages because it dictates a person’s ability to deal with both the
emotion and the situation. For example, if a person is not
self-efficacious about their ability to impact the global environment,
they are not likely to change their attitude or behavior about global
warming.
Dillard (1994) suggests that message features such as source
non-verbal communication, message content, and receiver differences can
impact the emotion impact of fear appeals. The characteristics of a
message are important because one message can elicit different levels of
emotion for different people. Thus, in terms of emotion appeals
messages, one size does not fit all.
Attitude accessibility refers to the activation of an attitude
from memory in other words, how readily available is an attitude about
an object, issue, or situation. Issue involvement is the relevance and
salience of an issue or situation to an individual. Issue involvement
has been correlated with both attitude access and attitude strength.
Past studies conclude accessible attitudes are more resistant to change.
Attitude-behavior relationship
The effects of attitudes on behaviors is a growing research enterprise within psychology. Icek Ajzen has led research and helped develop two prominent theoretical approaches within this field: the theory of reasoned action and, its theoretical descendant, the theory of planned behavior. Both theories help explain the link between attitude and behavior as a controlled and deliberative process.
Theory of reasoned action
The
theory of reasoned action (TRA) is a model for the prediction of
behavioral intention, spanning predictions of attitude and predictions
of behavior. The subsequent separation of behavioral intention from
behavior allows for explanation of limiting factors on attitudinal
influence (Ajzen, 1980). The theory of reasoned action was developed by
Martin Fishbein and Icek Ajzen (1975, 1980), derived from previous
research that started out as the theory of attitude, which led to the
study of attitude and behavior. The theory was "born largely out of
frustration with traditional attitude–behavior research, much of which
found weak correlations between attitude measures and performance of
volitional behaviors" (Hale, Householder & Greene, 2003, p. 259).
Theory of planned behavior
The
theory of planned behavior was proposed by Icek Ajzen in 1985 through
his article "From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior."
The theory was developed from the theory of reasoned action, which was
proposed by Martin Fishbein together with Icek Ajzen in 1975. The theory
of reasoned action was in turn grounded in various theories of attitude
such as learning theories, expectancy-value theories, consistency
theories, and attribution theory. According to the theory of reasoned
action, if people evaluate the suggested behavior as positive
(attitude), and if they think their significant others want them to
perform the behavior (subjective norm), this results in a higher
intention (motivation) and they are more likely to do so. A high
correlation of attitudes and subjective norms to behavioral intention,
and subsequently to behavior, has been confirmed in many studies. The
theory of planned behavior contains the same component as the theory of
reasoned action, but adds the component of perceived behavioral control
to account for barriers outside one's own control.
Motivation and Opportunity as Determinants (MODE)
Russell H. Fazio
proposed an alternative theory called "Motivation and Opportunity as
Determinants" or MODE. Fazio believes that because there is deliberative
process happening, individuals must be motivated to reflect on their
attitudes and subsequent behaviors.
Simply put, when an attitude is automatically activated, the individual
must be motivated to avoid making an invalid judgement as well as have
the opportunity to reflect on their attitude and behavior.
A counter-argument against the high relationship between
behavioral intention and actual behavior has also been proposed, as the
results of some studies show that, because of circumstantial
limitations, behavioral intention does not always lead to actual
behavior. Namely, since behavioral intention cannot be the exclusive
determinant of behavior where an individual's control over the behavior
is incomplete, Ajzen introduced the theory of planned behavior by adding
a new component, "perceived behavioral control." By this, he extended
the theory of reasoned action to cover non-volitional behaviors for
predicting behavioral intention and actual behavior.
Measurement
In 1928 Louis Leon Thurstone
published an article titled "Attitudes Can Be Measured" in it he
proposed an elaborate procedure to assess people’s views on social
issues. Attitudes can be difficult to measure because measurement is
arbitrary, because attitudes are ultimately a hypothetical construct that cannot be observed directly.
But many measurements
and evidence proofed scales are used to examine attitudes. A Likert
scale taps agreement or disagreement with a series of belief statements.
The Guttman scale focuses on items that vary in their degree of
psychological difficulty. The semantic differential uses bipolar
adjectives to measure the meaning associated with attitude objects.
Supplementing these are several indirect techniques such as unobtrusive,
standard physiological, and neuroscientific measures. Following the explicit-implicit dichotomy, attitudes can be examined through direct and indirect measures.
Whether attitudes are explicit (i.e., deliberately formed) versus
implicit (i.e., subconscious) has been a topic of considerable
research. Research on implicit attitudes,
which are generally unacknowledged or outside of awareness, uses
sophisticated methods involving people's response times to stimuli to
show that implicit attitudes exist (perhaps in tandem with explicit
attitudes of the same object). Implicit and explicit attitudes seem to
affect people's behavior, though in different ways. They tend not to be
strongly associated with each other, although in some cases they are.
The relationship between them is poorly understood.
Explicit
Explicit
measures tend to rely on self-reports or easily observed behaviors.
These tend to involve bipolar scales (e.g., good-bad,
favorable-unfavorable, support-oppose, etc.).
Explicit measures can also be used by measuring the straightforward
attribution of characteristics to nominate groups. Explicit attitudes
that develop in response to recent information, automatic evaluation
were thought to reflect mental associations through early socialization
experiences. Once formed, these associations are highly robust and
resistant to change, as well as stable across both context and time.
Hence the impact of contextual influences was assumed to be obfuscate
assessment of a person's "true" and enduring evaluative disposition as
well as limit the capacity to predict subsequent behavior. Likert scales and other self-reports are also commonly used.
Implicit
Implicit
measures are not consciously directed and are assumed to be automatic,
which may make implicit measures more valid and reliable than explicit
measures (such as self-reports). For example, people can be motivated
such that they find it socially desirable to appear to have certain
attitudes. An example of this is that people can hold implicit prejudicial
attitudes, but express explicit attitudes that report little prejudice.
Implicit measures help account for these situations and look at
attitudes that a person may not be aware of or want to show. Implicit measures therefore usually rely on an indirect measure of attitude. For example, the Implicit Association Test
(IAT) examines the strength between the target concept and an attribute
element by considering the latency in which a person can examine two
response keys when each has two meanings. With little time to carefully
examine what the participant is doing they respond according to
internal keys. This priming can show attitudes the person has about a
particular object.
People are often unwilling to provide responses perceived as socially
undesirable and therefore tend to report what they think their attitudes
should be rather than what they know them to be. More complicated
still, people may not even be consciously aware that they hold biased
attitudes. Over the past few decades, scientists have developed new
measures to identify these unconscious biases.