fluid and crystallized intelligence test scores that were measured in many parts of the world over the 20th century. When intelligence quotient (IQ) tests are initially standardized using a sample of test-takers, by convention the average of the test results is set to 100 and their standard deviation
is set to 15 or 16 IQ points. When IQ tests are revised, they are again
standardized using a new sample of test-takers, usually born more
recently than the first. Again, the average result is set to 100.
However, when the new test subjects take the older tests, in almost
every case their average scores are significantly above 100.
The Flynn effect is the substantial and long-sustained increase in both
Test score increases have been continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to the present. For the Raven's Progressive Matrices test, a study published in the year 2009 found that British children's average scores rose by 14 IQ points from 1942 to 2008.
Similar gains have been observed in many other countries in which IQ
testing has long been widely used, including other Western European
countries, Japan, and South Korea.
There are numerous proposed explanations of the Flynn effect, as well as some skepticism about its implications. Similar improvements have been reported for other cognitions such as semantic and episodic memory. Research suggests that there is an ongoing reversed Flynn effect, i.e. a decline in IQ scores, in Norway, Denmark, Australia, Britain, the Netherlands, Sweden, Finland, France and German-speaking countries, a development which appears to have started in the 1990s.
There are numerous proposed explanations of the Flynn effect, as well as some skepticism about its implications. Similar improvements have been reported for other cognitions such as semantic and episodic memory. Research suggests that there is an ongoing reversed Flynn effect, i.e. a decline in IQ scores, in Norway, Denmark, Australia, Britain, the Netherlands, Sweden, Finland, France and German-speaking countries, a development which appears to have started in the 1990s.
Origin of term
The Flynn effect is named for James R. Flynn, who did much to document it and promote awareness of its implications. The term itself was coined by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, authors of The Bell Curve.
Although the general term for the phenomenon—referring to no researcher
in particular—continues to be "secular rise in IQ scores", many
textbooks on psychology and IQ testing have now followed the lead of
Herrnstein and Murray in calling the phenomenon the Flynn effect.
Rise in IQ
IQ tests are updated periodically. For example, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), originally developed in 1949, was updated in 1974, 1991, 2003 and again in 2014. The revised versions are standardized
based on the performance of test-takers in standardization samples. A
standard score of IQ 100 is defined as the median performance of the
standardization sample. Thus one way to see changes in norms over time
is to conduct a study in which the same test-takers take both an old and
new version of the same test. Doing so confirms IQ gains over time.
Some IQ tests, for example tests used for military draftees in NATO
countries in Europe, report raw scores, and those also confirm a trend
of rising scores over time. The average rate of increase seems to be
about three IQ points per decade in the United States, as scaled by the
Wechsler tests. The increasing test performance over time appears on
every major test, in every age range, at every ability level, and in
every modern industrialized country, although not necessarily at the
same rate as in the United States. The increase was continuous and
roughly linear from the earliest days of testing to the mid-1990s. Though the effect is most associated with IQ increases, a similar effect has been found with increases in attention and of semantic and episodic memory.
Ulric Neisser estimated that using the IQ values of 1997, the average IQ of the United States in 1932, according to the first Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales
standardization sample, was 80. Neisser states that "Hardly any of them
would have scored 'very superior', but nearly one-quarter would have
appeared to be 'deficient.'" He also wrote that "Test scores are
certainly going up all over the world, but whether intelligence itself
has risen remains controversial."
Trahan et al. (2014) found that the effect was about 2.93 points
per decade, based on both Stanford–Binet and Wechsler tests; they also
found no evidence the effect was diminishing.
In contrast, Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) reported, in their
meta-analysis of studies involving nearly 4 million participants, that
the Flynn effect had decreased in recent decades. They also reported
that the magnitude of the effect was different for different types of
intelligence ("0.41, 0.30, 0.28, and 0.21 IQ points annually for fluid,
spatial, full-scale, and crystallized IQ test performance,
respectively"), and that the effect was stronger for adults than for
children.
Raven (2000) found that, as Flynn suggested, data interpreted as
showing a decrease in many abilities with increasing age must be
re-interpreted as showing that there has been a dramatic increase of
these abilities with date of birth. On many tests this occurs at all
levels of ability.
Some studies have found the gains of the Flynn effect to be
particularly concentrated at the lower end of the distribution. Teasdale
and Owen (1989), for example, found the effect primarily reduced the
number of low-end scores, resulting in an increased number of moderately
high scores, with no increase in very high scores.
In another study, two large samples of Spanish children were assessed
with a 30-year gap. Comparison of the IQ distributions indicated that
the mean IQ scores on the test had increased by 9.7 points (the Flynn
effect), the gains were concentrated in the lower half of the
distribution and negligible in the top half, and the gains gradually
decreased as the IQ of the individuals increased. Some studies have found a reverse Flynn effect with declining scores for those with high IQ.
In 1987, Flynn took the position that the very large increase
indicates that IQ tests do not measure intelligence but only a minor
sort of "abstract problem-solving ability" with little practical
significance. He argued that if IQ gains do reflect intelligence
increases, there would have been consequent changes of our society that
have not been observed (a presumed non-occurrence of a "cultural
renaissance"). Flynn no longer endorses this view of intelligence and has since elaborated and refined his view of what rising IQ scores mean.
Precursors to Flynn's publications
Earlier
investigators had discovered rises in raw IQ test scores in some study
populations, but had not published general investigations of that issue
in particular. Historian Daniel C. Calhoun cited earlier psychology
literature on IQ score trends in his book The Intelligence of a People (1973). R. L. Thorndike drew attention to rises in Stanford-Binet scores in a 1975 review of the history of intelligence testing.
Intelligence
There is debate about whether the rise in IQ scores also corresponds
to a rise in general intelligence, or only a rise in special skills
related to taking IQ tests. Because children attend school longer now
and have become much more familiar with the testing of school-related
material, one might expect the greatest gains to occur on such school
content-related tests as vocabulary, arithmetic
or general information. Just the opposite is the case: abilities such
as these have experienced relatively small gains and even occasional
decreases over the years. Meta-analytic findings indicate that Flynn
effects occur for tests assessing both fluid and crystallized abilities.
For example, Dutch conscripts gained 21 points during only 30 years, or
7 points per decade, between 1952 and 1982.
But this rise in IQ test scores is not wholly explained by an increase
in general intelligence. Studies have shown that while test scores have
improved over time, the improvement is not fully correlated with latent
factors related to intelligence. Rushton has shown that the gains in IQ over time (the Lynn-Flynn effect) are unrelated to g.
Other researchers have shown that the IQ gains described by the Flynn
effect are due in part to increasing intelligence, and in part to
increases in test-specific skills.
In parallel with the measured gains in IQ scores, secular declines have
been found for "mental speed, digit span backwards, the use of
difficult words, and color acuity, all of which are related to
intelligence".
Proposed explanations
A 2017 survey of 75 experts in the field of intelligence research
suggested four key causes of the Flynn effect: Better health, better
nutrition, more and better education, and rising standards of living.
Genetic changes were seen as not important. The experts' views agreed with an independently performed meta-analysis on published Flynn effect data, except that the latter found life history speed to be the most important factor.
The expert survey explained the possible end or decline in the
Flynn effect by asymmetric fertility by means of genetic effects,
migration, asymmetric fertility by means of socialization effects,
declines in education, and the influence of media.
Schooling and test familiarity
Duration
of average schooling has increased steadily. One problem with this
explanation is that if in the US comparing older and more recent
subjects with similar educational levels, then the IQ gains appear
almost undiminished in each such group considered individually.
Many studies find that children who do not attend school score
drastically lower on the tests than their regularly attending peers.
During the 1960s, when some Virginia counties closed their public schools to avoid racial integration,
compensatory private schooling was available only for Caucasian
children. On average, the scores of African-American children who
received no formal education during that period decreased at a rate of
about six IQ points per year.
Another explanation is an increased familiarity of the general
population with tests and testing. For example, children who take the
very same IQ test a second time usually gain five or six points.
However, this seems to set an upper limit on the effects of test
sophistication. One problem with this explanation and others related to
schooling is that in the US, the groups with greater test familiarity
show smaller IQ increases.
Early intervention programs have shown mixed results. Some preschool (ages 3–4) intervention programs like "Head Start" do not produce lasting changes of IQ, although they may confer other benefits. The "Abecedarian Early Intervention Project", an all-day program that provided various forms of environmental enrichment
to children from infancy onward, showed IQ gains that did not diminish
over time. The IQ difference between the groups, although only five
points, was still present at age 12. Not all such projects have been
successful. Also, such IQ gains can diminish until age 18.
Citing a high correlation between rising literacy rates and gains in IQ, David Marks has argued that the Flynn effect is caused by changes in literacy rates.
Generally more stimulating environment
Still
another theory is that the general environment today is much more
complex and stimulating. One of the most striking 20th-century changes
of the human intellectual environment has come from the increase of
exposure to many types of visual media.
From pictures on the wall to movies to television to video games to
computers, each successive generation has been exposed to richer optical
displays than the one before and may have become more adept at visual
analysis. This would explain why visual tests like the Raven's have
shown the greatest increases. An increase only of particular form(s) of
intelligence would explain why the Flynn effect has not caused a
"cultural renaissance too great to be overlooked."
In 2001, Dickens and Flynn presented a model for resolving
several contradictory findings regarding IQ. They argue that the measure
"heritability" includes both a direct effect of the genotype on IQ and also indirect effects such that the genotype changes the environment,
thereby affecting IQ. That is, those with a greater IQ tend to seek
stimulating environments that further increase IQ. These reciprocal
effects result in gene environment correlation. The direct effect could initially have been very small, but feedback
can create large differences of IQ. In their model, an environmental
stimulus can have a very great effect on IQ, even for adults, but this
effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model
could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition during
early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can
be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all
people. The authors suggest that any program designed to increase IQ may
produce long-term IQ gains if that program teaches children how to
replicate the types of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ
gains outside the program. To maximize lifetime IQ, the programs should
also motivate them to continue searching for cognitively demanding
experiences after they have left the program.
Flynn in his 2007 book What Is Intelligence?
further expanded on this theory. Environmental changes resulting from
modernization—such as more intellectually demanding work, greater use of
technology and smaller families—have meant that a much larger
proportion of people are more accustomed to manipulating abstract
concepts such as hypotheses and categories than a century ago.
Substantial portions of IQ tests deal with these abilities. Flynn gives,
as an example, the question 'What do a dog and a rabbit have in
common?' A modern respondent might say they are both mammals (an
abstract, or a priori answer, which depends only on the meanings of the words dog and rabbit), whereas someone a century ago might have said that humans catch rabbits with dogs (a concrete, or a posteriori answer, which depended on what happened to be the case at that time).
Nutrition
Improved
nutrition is another possible explanation. Today's average adult from
an industrialized nation is taller than a comparable adult of a century
ago. That increase of stature, likely the result of general improvements
of nutrition and health, has been at a rate of more than a centimeter
per decade. Available data suggest that these gains have been
accompanied by analogous increases of head size, and by an increase in
the average size of the brain.
This argument had been thought to suffer the difficulty that groups who
tend to be of smaller overall body size (e.g. women, or people of Asian
ancestry) do not have lower average IQs. Richard Lynn,
however, claims that while people of East Asian origin may often have
smaller bodies, they tend to have larger brains and higher IQs than
average whites.
A 2005 study presented data supporting the nutrition hypothesis,
which predicts that gains will occur predominantly at the low end of the
IQ distribution, where nutritional deprivation is probably most severe. An alternative interpretation of skewed IQ gains could be that improved education has been particularly important for this group. Richard Lynn
makes the case for nutrition, arguing that cultural factors cannot
typically explain the Flynn effect because its gains are observed even
at infant and preschool levels, with rates of IQ test score increase
about equal to those of school students and adults. Lynn states that
"This rules out improvements in education, greater test sophistication,
etc. and most of the other factors that have been proposed to explain
the Flynn effect. He proposes that the most probable factor has been
improvements in pre-natal and early post-natal nutrition."
A century ago, nutritional
deficiencies may have limited body and organ functionality, including
skull volume. The first two years of life is a critical time for
nutrition. The consequences of malnutrition can be irreversible and may
include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic
productivity. On the other hand, Flynn has pointed to 20-point gains on Dutch military (Raven's
type) IQ tests between 1952, 1962, 1972, and 1982. He observes that the
Dutch 18-year-olds of 1962 had a major nutritional handicap. They were
either in the womb, or were recently born, during the great Dutch famine of 1944—when German troops monopolized food and 18,000 people died of starvation.
Yet, concludes Flynn, "they do not show up even as a blip in the
pattern of Dutch IQ gains. It is as if the famine had never occurred."
It appears that the effects of diet are gradual, taking effect over
decades (affecting mother as well as child) rather than a few months.
In support of the nutritional hypothesis, it is known that, in
the United States, the average height before 1900 was about 10 cm
(∼4 inches) shorter than it is today. Possibly related to the Flynn effect is a similar change of skull
size and shape during the last 150 years. Though the idea that brain
size is unrelated to race and intelligence was popularized in the 1980s,
studies continue to show significant correlations.
A Norwegian study found that height gains were strongly correlated with
intelligence gains until the cessation of height gains in military
conscript cohorts towards the end of the 1980s. Both height and skull size increases probably result from a combination of phenotypic plasticity and genetic selection over this period. With only five or six human generations in 150 years, time for natural selection has been very limited, suggesting that increased skeletal size resulting from changes in population phenotypes is more likely than recent genetic evolution.
It is well known that micronutrient deficiencies change the development of intelligence. For instance, one study has found that iodine deficiency causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points in China.
Scientists James Feyrer, Dimitra Politi, and David N. Weil have
found in the U.S. that the proliferation of iodized salt increased IQ by
15 points in some areas. Journalist Max Nisen has stated that, with
this type of salt becoming popular, that "the aggregate effect has been
extremely positive."
Daley et al. (2003) found a significant Flynn effect among children in rural Kenya,
and concluded that nutrition was one of the hypothesized explanations
that best explained their results (the others were parental literacy and
family structure).
Infectious diseases
Eppig,
Fincher, and Thornhill (2009) argue that "From an energetics
standpoint, a developing human will have difficulty building a brain and
fighting off infectious diseases
at the same time, as both are very metabolically costly tasks" and that
"the Flynn effect may be caused in part by the decrease in the
intensity of infectious diseases as nations develop." They suggest that
improvements in gross domestic product
(GDP), education, literacy, and nutrition may have an effect on IQ
mainly through reducing the intensity of infectious diseases.
Eppig, Fincher, and Thornhill (2011) in a similar study instead
looking at different US states found that states with a higher
prevalence of infectious diseases had lower average IQ. The effect
remained after controlling for the effects of wealth and educational
variation.
Atheendar Venkataramani (2010) studied the effect of malaria
on IQ in a sample of Mexicans. Exposure during the birth year to
malaria eradication was associated with increases in IQ. It also
increased the probability of employment in a skilled occupation. The
author suggests that this may be one explanation for the Flynn effect
and that this may be an important explanation for the link between
national malaria burden and economic development.
A literature review of 44 papers states that cognitive abilities and
school performance were shown to be impaired in sub-groups of patients
(with either cerebral malaria or uncomplicated malaria) when compared
with healthy controls. Studies comparing cognitive functions before and
after treatment for acute malarial illness continued to show
significantly impaired school performance and cognitive abilities even
after recovery. Malaria prophylaxis was shown to improve cognitive function and school performance in clinical trials when compared to placebo groups.
Heterosis
Heterosis,
or hybrid vigor associated with historical reductions of the levels of
inbreeding, has been proposed by Michael Mingroni as an alternative
explanation of the Flynn effect. However, James Flynn has pointed out
that even if everyone mated with a sibling in 1900, subsequent increases
in heterosis would not be a sufficient explanation of the observed IQ
gains.
Possible end of progression
Jon Martin Sundet and colleagues (2004) examined scores on intelligence tests given to Norwegian conscripts
between the 1950s and 2002. They found that the increase of scores of
general intelligence stopped after the mid-1990s and declined in
numerical reasoning sub-tests.
Teasdale and Owen (2005) examined the results of IQ tests given to Danish
male conscripts. Between 1959 and 1979 the gains were 3 points per
decade. Between 1979 and 1989 the increase approached 2 IQ points.
Between 1989 and 1998 the gain was about 1.3 points. Between 1998 and
2004 IQ declined by about the same amount as it gained between 1989 and
1998. They speculate that "a contributing factor in this recent fall
could be a simultaneous decline in proportions of students entering
3-year advanced-level school programs for 16–18-year-olds."
The same authors in a more comprehensive 2008 study, again on Danish
male conscripts, found that there was a 1.5-point increase between 1988
and 1998, but a 1.5-point decrease between 1998 and 2003/2004. A
possible contributing factor to the more recent decline may be changes
in the Danish educational system. Another may be the rising proportion
of immigrants or their immediate descendants in Denmark. This is
supported by data on Danish draftees where first or second generation
immigrants with Danish nationality score below average.
In Australia, the IQ of 6–12 year olds as measured by the Colored Progressive Matrices has shown no increase from 1975–2003.
In the United Kingdom, a study by Flynn (2009) found that tests
carried out in 1980 and again in 2008 show that the IQ score of an
average 14-year-old dropped by more than two points over the period. For
the upper half of the results the performance was even worse. Average
IQ scores declined by six points. However, children aged between five
and 10 saw their IQs increase by up to half a point a year over the
three decades. Flynn argues that the abnormal drop in British teenage IQ
could be due to youth culture having “stagnated” or even dumbed down.
He also states that the youth culture is more oriented towards computer
games than towards reading and holding conversations. Researcher Richard
Gray, commenting on the study, also mentions the computer culture
diminishing reading books as well as a tendency towards teaching to the test.
Lynn and Harvey argued in 2008 that the causes of the above are
difficult to interpret since these countries had had significant recent
immigration from countries with lower average national IQs.
Nevertheless, they expect that similar patterns will occur, or have
occurred, first in other developed nations and then in the developing
world as there is a limit to how much environmental factors can improve
intelligence. Furthermore, during the last century there is a negative
correlation between fertility and intelligence although there is not yet any conclusive evidence of the association between the two. They estimate that there has been a dysgenic decline in the world's genotypic IQ (masked by the Flynn effect for the phenotype) of 0.86 IQ points per decade for the years 1950–2000.
Bratsberg & Rogeberg (2018) present evidence that the Flynn
effect in Norway has reversed, and that both the original rise in mean
IQ scores and their subsequent decline were caused by environmental
factors.
IQ group differences
If the Flynn effect has ended in developed nations, then this may possibly allow national differences in IQ scores to diminish if the Flynn effect continues in nations with lower average national IQs.
Also, if the Flynn effect has ended for the majority in developed
nations, it may still continue for minorities, especially for groups
like immigrants where many may have received poor nutrition during early
childhood or have had other disadvantages. A study in the Netherlands found that children of non-Western immigrants had improvements for g,
educational achievements, and work proficiency compared to their
parents, although there were still remaining differences compared to
ethnic Dutch.
There is a controversy as to whether the US racial gap in IQ scores
is diminishing. If that is the case then this may or may not be related
to the Flynn effect. Flynn has commented that he never claimed that the
Flynn effect has the same causes as the black-white gap, but that it
shows that environmental factors can create IQ differences of a
magnitude similar to the gap. Research that has examined whether g factor
and IQ gains from the Flynn effect are related have found there is a
negative correlation between the two, which may indicate that group
differences and the Flynn effect are possibly due to differing causes.
The Flynn effect has also been part of the discussions regarding Spearman's hypothesis, which states that differences in the g factor are the major source of differences between blacks and whites observed in many studies of race and intelligence.