Social psychology is the scientific study of how people's thoughts, feelings and behaviors are influenced by the actual, imagined or implied presence of others. In this definition, scientific refers to the empirical investigation using the scientific method. The terms thoughts, feelings and behavior refer to psychological variables that can be measured in humans. The statement that others' presence may be imagined or implied
suggests that humans are malleable to social influences even when
alone, such as when watching television or following internalized cultural norms. Social psychologists typically explain human behavior as a result of the interaction of mental states and social situations.
Social psychologists examine factors that cause behaviors to
unfold in a given way in the presence of others. They study conditions
under which certain behavior, actions, and feelings occur. Social
psychology is concerned with the way these feelings, thoughts, beliefs,
intentions, and goals are cognitively constructed and how these mental
representations, in turn, influence our interactions with others.
Social psychology traditionally bridged the gap between psychology and sociology. During the years immediately following World War II there was frequent collaboration between psychologists and sociologists. The two disciplines, however, have become increasingly specialized and isolated from each other in recent years, with sociologists focusing on "macro variables" (e.g., social structure) to a much greater extent than psychologists.[citation needed] Nevertheless, sociological approaches to psychology remain an important counterpart to psychological research in this area.
In addition to the split between psychology and sociology, there has been a somewhat less pronounced difference in emphasis between American social psychologists and European social psychologists. As a generalization, American researchers traditionally have focused more on the individual, whereas Europeans have paid more attention to group level phenomena (see group dynamics).
Social psychology traditionally bridged the gap between psychology and sociology. During the years immediately following World War II there was frequent collaboration between psychologists and sociologists. The two disciplines, however, have become increasingly specialized and isolated from each other in recent years, with sociologists focusing on "macro variables" (e.g., social structure) to a much greater extent than psychologists.[citation needed] Nevertheless, sociological approaches to psychology remain an important counterpart to psychological research in this area.
In addition to the split between psychology and sociology, there has been a somewhat less pronounced difference in emphasis between American social psychologists and European social psychologists. As a generalization, American researchers traditionally have focused more on the individual, whereas Europeans have paid more attention to group level phenomena (see group dynamics).
History
Although there were some older writings about social psychology, such as those by Islamic philosopher Al-Farabi (Alpharabius),
the discipline of social psychology, as its modern-day definition,
began in the United States at the beginning of the 20th century. By that
time, though, the discipline had already developed a significant
foundation. Following the 18th century, those in the emerging field of
social psychology were concerned with developing concrete explanations
for different aspects of human nature.
They attempted to discover concrete cause and effect relationships that
explained the social interactions in the world around them. In order to
do so, they believed that the scientific method, an empirically based scientific measure, could be applied to human behavior.
The first published study in this area was an experiment in 1898 by Norman Triplett, on the phenomenon of social facilitation. During the 1930s, many Gestalt psychologists, most notably Kurt Lewin, fled to the United States from Nazi Germany. They were instrumental in developing the field as something separate from the behavioral and psychoanalytic schools that were dominant during that time, and social psychology has always maintained the legacy of their interests in perception and cognition. Attitudes and small group phenomena were the most commonly studied topics in this era.
During World War II, social psychologists studied persuasion and propaganda for the U.S. military. After the war, researchers became interested in a variety of social problems, including gender issues and racial prejudice. Most notable, revealing, and contentious of these were the Stanley Milgram shock experiments on obedience to authority. In the sixties, there was growing interest in new topics, such as cognitive dissonance, bystander intervention, and aggression.
By the 1970s, however, social psychology in America had reached a
crisis. There was heated debate over the ethics of laboratory
experimentation, whether or not attitudes really predicted behavior, and
how much science could be done in a cultural context. This was also the time when a radical situationist approach challenged the relevance of self and personality in psychology.
Throughout the 1980s and 1990s social psychology reached a more mature
level. Two of the areas social psychology matured in were theories and
methods. Careful ethical standards now regulate research. Pluralistic and multicultural perspectives have emerged. Modern researchers are interested in many phenomena, but attribution, social cognition, and the self-concept are perhaps the greatest areas of growth in recent years.[citation needed]
Social psychologists have also maintained their applied interests with
contributions in the social psychology of health, education, law, and
the workplace.
Intrapersonal phenomena
Attitudes
In social psychology, attitudes are defined as learned, global
evaluations of a person, object, place, or issue that influence thought
and action.
Put more simply, attitudes are basic expressions of approval or
disapproval, favorability or unfavorability, or as Bem put it, likes and
dislikes. Examples would include liking chocolate ice cream, or endorsing the values of a particular political party.
Social psychologists have studied attitude formation, the
structure of attitudes, attitude change, the function of attitudes, and
the relationship between attitudes and behavior. Because people are
influenced by the situation, general attitudes are not always good
predictors of specific behavior. For example, for a variety of reasons, a
person may value the environment but not recycle a can on a particular
day.
In recent times, research on attitudes has examined the
distinction between traditional, self-reported attitude measures and
"implicit" or unconscious attitudes. For example, experiments using the Implicit Association Test
have found that people often demonstrate implicit bias against other
races, even when their explicit responses reveal equal mindedness. One
study found that explicit attitudes correlate with verbal behavior in
interracial interactions, whereas implicit attitudes correlate with
nonverbal behavior.
One hypothesis on how attitudes are formed, first advanced by Abraham Tesser in 1983, is that strong likes and dislikes are ingrained in our genetic make-up. Tesser speculates that individuals are disposed to hold certain strong attitudes as a result of inborn physical, sensory, and cognitive skills, temperament, and personality traits.
Whatever disposition nature elects to give us, our most treasured
attitudes are often formed as a result of exposure to attitude objects;
our history of rewards and punishments; the attitude that our parents,
friends, and enemies express; the social and cultural context in which
we live; and other types of experiences we have. Obviously, attitudes
are formed through the basic process of learning. Numerous studies have
shown that people can form strong positive and negative attitudes toward
neutral objects that are in some way linked to emotionally charged
stimuli.
Attitudes are also involved in several other areas of the discipline, such as conformity, interpersonal attraction, social perception, and prejudice.
Persuasion
The topic of persuasion has received a great deal of attention in
recent years. Persuasion is an active method of influence that attempts
to guide people toward the adoption of an attitude, idea, or behavior by
rational or emotive means. Persuasion relies on "appeals" rather than
strong pressure or coercion.
Numerous variables have been found to influence the persuasion process;
these are normally presented in five major categories: who said what to whom and how.
- The Communicator, including credibility, expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness.
- The Message, including varying degrees of reason, emotion (such as fear), one-sided or two sided arguments, and other types of informational content.
- The Audience, including a variety of demographics, personality traits, and preferences.
- The Channel or Medium, including the printed word, radio, television, the internet, or face-to-face interactions.
- The Context, including the environment, group dynamics, and preamble to the message.
Dual-process theories of persuasion
maintain that the persuasive process is mediated by two separate
routes; central and peripheral. The central route of persuasion is more
fact-based and results in longer lasting change, but requires motivation
to process. The peripheral route is more superficial and results in
shorter lasting change, but does not require as much motivation to
process. An example of a peripheral route of persuasion might be a
politician using a flag lapel pin, smiling, and wearing a crisp, clean
shirt. Notice that this does not require motivation to be persuasive,
but should not last as long as persuasion based on the central route. If
that politician were to outline exactly what they believed, and their
previous voting record, this would be using the central route, and would
result in longer lasting change, but would require a good deal of
motivation to process.
Social cognition
Social cognition is a growing area of social psychology that studies
how people perceive, think about, and remember information about others. Much research rests on the assertion that people think about (other) people differently from non-social targets. This assertion is supported by the social cognitive deficits exhibited by people with Williams syndrome and autism. Person perception
is the study of how people form impressions of others. The study of how
people form beliefs about each other while interacting is known as interpersonal perception.
A major research topic in social cognition is attribution.
Attributions are the explanations we make for people's behavior, either
our own behavior or the behavior of others. One element of attribution
ascribes the locus of a behavior to either internal or external factors.
An internal, or dispositional, attribution assigns behavior to
causes related to inner traits such as personality, disposition,
character or ability. An external, or situational, attribution involves situational elements, such as the weather.
A second element of attribution ascribes the cause of behavior to
either stable or unstable factors (whether the behavior will be repeated
or changed under similar circumstances). Finally, we also attribute
causes of behavior to either controllable or uncontrollable factors: how
much control one has over the situation at hand.
Numerous biases in the attribution process have been discovered. For instance, the fundamental attribution error
is the tendency to make dispositional attributions for behavior,
overestimating the influence of personality and underestimating the
influence of situations.
The actor-observer difference is a refinement of this bias, the
tendency to make dispositional attributions for other people's behavior
and situational attributions for our own. The self-serving bias
is the tendency to attribute dispositional causes for successes, and
situational causes for failure, particularly when self-esteem is
threatened. This leads to assuming one's successes are from innate
traits, and one's failures are due to situations, including other
people. Other ways people protect their self-esteem are by believing in a just world, blaming victims for their suffering, and making defensive attributions, which explain our behavior in ways which defend us from feelings of vulnerability and mortality. Researchers have found that mildly depressed individuals often lack this bias and actually have more realistic perceptions of reality (as measured by the opinions of others).
Heuristics
are cognitive short cuts. Instead of weighing all the evidence when
making a decision, people rely on heuristics to save time and energy.
The availability heuristic occurs when people estimate the probability
of an outcome based on how easy that outcome is to imagine. As such,
vivid or highly memorable possibilities will be perceived as more likely
than those that are harder to picture or are difficult to understand,
resulting in a corresponding cognitive bias.
The representativeness heuristic is a shortcut people use to categorize
something based on how similar it is to a prototype they know of. Numerous other biases have been found by social cognition researchers. The hindsight bias is a false memory of having predicted events, or an exaggeration of actual predictions, after becoming aware of the outcome. The confirmation bias is a type of bias leading to the tendency to search for, or interpret information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions.
Another key concept in social cognition is the assumption that
reality is too complex to easily discern. As a result, we tend to see
the world according to simplified schemas or images of reality. Schemas are generalized mental representations that organize knowledge and guide information processing. Schemas often operate automatically
and unintentionally, and can lead to biases in perception and memory.
Expectations from schemas may lead us to see something that is not
there. One experiment found that people are more likely to misperceive a
weapon in the hands of a black man than a white man. This type of schema is actually a stereotype, a generalized set of beliefs about a particular group of people (when incorrect, an ultimate attribution error). Stereotypes are often related to negative or preferential attitudes (prejudice) and behavior (discrimination). Schemas for behaviors (e.g., going to a restaurant, doing laundry) are known as scripts.
Self-concept
Self-concept is a term referring to the whole sum of beliefs that
people have about themselves. However, what specifically does
self-concept consist of? According to Hazel Markus (1977), the
self-concept is made up of cognitive molecules called self-schemas –
beliefs that people have about themselves that guide the processing of
self-reliant information. For example, an athlete at a university would
have multiple selves that would process different information pertinent
to each self: the student would be one "self," who would process
information pertinent to a student (taking notes in class, completing a
homework assignment, etc.); the athlete would be the "self" who
processes information about things related to being an athlete
(recognizing an incoming pass, aiming a shot, etc.). These "selves" are
part of one's identity and the self-reliant information is the
information that relies on the proper "self" to process and react on it.
If a "self" is not part of one's identity, then it is much more
difficult for one to react. For example, a civilian may not know how to
handle a hostile threat as a trained Marine would. The Marine contains a
"self" that would enable him/her to process the information about the
hostile threat and react accordingly, whereas a civilian may not contain
that self, disabling them from properly processing the information from
the hostile threat and, furthermore, debilitating them from acting
accordingly. Self-schemas are to an individual’s total self–concept as a
hypothesis is to a theory, or a book is to a library. A good example is
the body weight self-schema; people who regard themselves as over or
underweight, or for those whom body image is a significant self-concept
aspect, are considered schematics with respect to weight. For
these people a range of otherwise mundane events – grocery shopping, new
clothes, eating out, or going to the beach – can trigger thoughts about
the self. In contrast, people who do not regard their weight as an
important part of their lives are a-schematic on that attribute.
It is rather clear that the self is a special object of our attention. Whether one is mentally focused on a memory, a conversation, a foul smell, the song that is stuck in one's head, or this sentence, consciousness is like a spotlight. This spotlight
can shine on only one object at a time, but it can switch rapidly from
one object to another and process the information out of awareness. In this spotlight the self is front and center: things relating to the self have the spotlight more often.
The self's ABCs are affect, behavior, and cognition. An affective
(or emotional) question: How do people evaluate themselves, enhance
their self-image, and maintain a secure sense of identity? A behavioral
question: How do people regulate their own actions and present
themselves to others according to interpersonal demands? A cognitive question: How do individuals become themselves, build a self-concept, and uphold a stable sense of identity?
Affective forecasting is the process of predicting how one would feel in response to future emotional events. Studies done by Timothy Wilson and Daniel Gilbert
in 2003 have shown that people overestimate the strength of reaction to
anticipated positive and negative life events that they actually feel
when the event does occur.
There are many theories on the perception of our own behavior.
Daryl Bem's (1972) self-perception theory claims that when internal cues
are difficult to interpret, people gain self-insight by observing their
own behavior. Leon Festinger's 1954 social comparison theory
is that people evaluate their own abilities and opinions by comparing
themselves to others when they are uncertain of their own ability or
opinions. There is also the facial feedback hypothesis: that changes in facial expression can lead to corresponding changes in emotion.
The fields of social psychology and personality
have merged over the years, and social psychologists have developed an
interest in self-related phenomena. In contrast with traditional
personality theory, however, social psychologists place a greater
emphasis on cognitions than on traits. Much research focuses on the self-concept, which is a person's understanding of their self. The self-concept is often divided into a cognitive component, known as the self-schema, and an evaluative component, the self-esteem. The need to maintain a healthy self-esteem is recognized as a central human motivation in the field of social psychology.
Self-efficacy
beliefs are associated with the self-schema. These are expectations
that performance on some task will be effective and successful. Social
psychologists also study such self-related processes as self-control and self-presentation.
People develop their self-concepts by varied means, including introspection, feedback from others, self-perception,
and social comparison. By comparing themselves to relevant others,
people gain information about themselves, and they make inferences that
are relevant to self-esteem. Social comparisons can be either "upward"
or "downward," that is, comparisons to people who are either higher in
status or ability, or lower in status or ability. Downward comparisons are often made in order to elevate self-esteem.
Self-perception is a specialized form of attribution that
involves making inferences about oneself after observing one's own
behavior. Psychologists have found that too many extrinsic rewards (e.g.
money) tend to reduce intrinsic motivation through the self-perception
process, a phenomenon known as overjustification. People's attention is directed to the reward and they lose interest in the task when the reward is no longer offered. This is an important exception to reinforcement theory.
Interpersonal phenomena
Social influence
Social influence is an overarching term given to describe the
persuasive effects people have on each other. It is seen as a
fundamental value in social psychology and overlaps considerably with
research on attitudes and persuasion. The three main areas of social
influence include: conformity, compliance, and obedience.
Social influence is also closely related to the study of group
dynamics, as most principles of influence are strongest when they take
place in social groups.
The first major area of social influence is conformity.
Conformity is defined as the tendency to act or think like other members
of a group. The identity of members within a group, i.e. status,
similarity, expertise, as well as cohesion, prior commitment, and
accountability to the group help to determine the level of conformity of
an individual. Individual variation among group members plays a key
role in the dynamic of how willing people will be to conform.
Conformity is usually viewed as a negative tendency in American
culture, but a certain amount of conformity is adaptive in some
situations, as is nonconformity in other situations.
The second major area of social influence research is compliance. Compliance refers to any change in behavior that is due to a request or suggestion from another person. The foot-in-the-door technique
is a compliance method in which the persuader requests a small favor
and then follows up with requesting a larger favor, e.g., asking for the
time and then asking for ten dollars. A related trick is the bait and switch.
The third major form of social influence is obedience;
this is a change in behavior that is the result of a direct order or
command from another person. Obedience as a form of compliance was
dramatically highlighted by the Milgram study, wherein people were ready to administer shocks to a person in distress on a researcher's command.
An unusual kind of social influence is the self-fulfilling prophecy. This is a prediction that, in being made, actually causes itself to become true. For example, in the stock market, if it is widely believed that a crash
is imminent, investors may lose confidence, sell most of their stock,
and thus actually cause the crash. Similarly, people may expect
hostility in others and actually induce this hostility by their own
behavior.
Group dynamics
A group can be defined as two or more individuals that are connected to each another by social relationships.
Groups tend to interact, influence each other, and share a common
identity. They have a number of emergent qualities that distinguish them
from aggregates:
- Norms: Implicit rules and expectations for group members to follow, e.g. saying thank you, shaking hands.
- Roles: Implicit rules and expectations for specific members within the group, e.g. the oldest sibling, who may have additional responsibilities in the family.
- Relations: Patterns of liking within the group, and also differences in prestige or status, e.g., leaders, popular people.
Temporary groups and aggregates share few or none of these features,
and do not qualify as true social groups. People waiting in line to get
on a bus, for example, do not constitute a group.
Groups are important not only because they offer social support,
resources, and a feeling of belonging, but because they supplement an
individual's self-concept. To a large extent, humans define themselves
by the group memberships which form their social identity. The shared social identity of individuals within a group influences inter-group behavior,
the way in which groups behave towards and perceive each other. These
perceptions and behaviors in turn define the social identity of
individuals within the interacting groups. The tendency to define
oneself by membership in a group may lead to intergroup discrimination,
which involves favorable perceptions and behaviors directed towards the
in-group, but negative perceptions and behaviors directed towards the
out-group.
On the other hand, such discrimination and segregation may sometimes
exist partly to facilitate a diversity which strengthens society. Inter-group discrimination leads to prejudice and stereotyping, while the processes of social facilitation and group polarization encourage extreme behaviors towards the out-group.
Groups often moderate and improve decision making,
and are frequently relied upon for these benefits, such as in
committees and juries. A number of group biases, however, can interfere
with effective decision making. For example, group polarization,
formerly known as the "risky shift," occurs when people polarize their
views in a more extreme direction after group discussion. More
problematic is the phenomenon of group-think.
This is a collective thinking defect that is characterized by a
premature consensus or an incorrect assumption of consensus, caused by
members of a group failing to promote views which are not consistent
with the views of other members. Group-think occurs in a variety of
situations, including isolation of a group and the presence of a highly
directive leader. Janis offered the 1961 Bay of Pigs Invasion as a historical case of group-think.
Groups also affect performance and productivity.
Social facilitation, for example, is a tendency to work harder and
faster in the presence of others. Social facilitation increases the dominant response's likelihood, which tends to improve performance on simple tasks and reduce it on complex tasks. In contrast, social loafing is the tendency of individuals to slack off
when working in a group. Social loafing is common when the task is
considered unimportant and individual contributions are not easy to see.
Social psychologists study group-related (collective) phenomena such as the behavior of crowds. An important concept in this area is deindividuation, a reduced state of self-awareness
that can be caused by feelings of anonymity. Deindividuation is
associated with uninhibited and sometimes dangerous behavior. It is
common in crowds and mobs, but it can also be caused by a disguise, a
uniform, alcohol, dark environments, or online anonymity.
Interpersonal attraction
A major area in the study of people's relations to each other is
interpersonal attraction. This refers to all forces that lead people to
like each other, establish relationships, and (in some cases) fall in love.
Several general principles of attraction have been discovered by social
psychologists, but many still continue to experiment and do research to
find out more. One of the most important factors in interpersonal
attraction is how similar two particular people are. The more similar
two people are in general attitudes, backgrounds, environments,
worldviews, and other traits, the more probable an attraction is
possible.
Physical attractiveness is an important element of romantic relationships, particularly in the early stages characterized by high levels of passion.
Later on, similarity and other compatibility factors become more
important, and the type of love people experience shifts from passionate to companionate. Robert Sternberg has suggested that there are actually three components of love: intimacy, passion, and commitment. When two (or more) people experience all three, they are said to be in a state of consummate love.
According to social exchange theory,
relationships are based on rational choice and cost-benefit analysis.
If one partner's costs begin to outweigh their benefits, that person may
leave the relationship, especially if there are good alternatives
available. This theory is similar to the minimax principle proposed by mathematicians and economists (despite the fact that human relationships are not zero-sum games). With time, long term relationships tend to become communal rather than simply based on exchange.
Research
Methods
Social psychology is an empirical
science that attempts to answer questions about human behavior by
testing hypotheses, both in the laboratory and in the field. Careful
attention to sampling, research design, and statistical analysis is important; results are published in peer reviewed journals such as the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin and the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Social psychology studies also appear in general science journals such as Psychological Science and Science.
Experimental methods
involve the researcher altering a variable in the environment and
measuring the effect on another variable. An example would be allowing
two groups of children to play violent or nonviolent video games, and
then observing their subsequent level of aggression during free-play
period. A valid experiment is controlled and uses random assignment.
Correlational methods
examine the statistical association between two naturally occurring
variables. For example, one could correlate the amount of violent
television children watch at home with the number of violent incidents
the children participate in at school. Note that this study would not
prove that violent TV causes aggression in children: it is quite
possible that aggressive children choose to watch more violent TV.
Observational methods are purely descriptive and include naturalistic observation,
"contrived" observation, participant observation, and archival
analysis. These are less common in social psychology but are sometimes
used when first investigating a phenomenon. An example would be to
unobtrusively observe children on a playground (with a videocamera,
perhaps) and record the number and types of aggressive actions
displayed.
Whenever possible, social psychologists rely on controlled
experimentation. Controlled experiments require the manipulation of one
or more independent variables in order to examine the effect on a dependent variable. Experiments are useful in social psychology because they are high in internal validity, meaning that they are free from the influence of confounding
or extraneous variables, and so are more likely to accurately indicate a
causal relationship. However, the small samples used in controlled
experiments are typically low in external validity,
or the degree to which the results can be generalized to the larger
population. There is usually a trade-off between experimental control (internal validity) and being able to generalize to the population (external validity).
Because it is usually impossible to test everyone, research tends to be conducted on a sample of persons from the wider population. Social psychologists frequently use survey research when they are interested in results that are high in external validity. Surveys use various forms of random sampling
to obtain a sample of respondents that are representative of a
population. This type of research is usually descriptive or
correlational because there is no experimental control over variables.
However, new statistical methods like structural equation modeling are being used to test for potential causal relationships in this type of data. Some psychologists, including Dr. David O. Sears,
have criticized social psychological research for relying too heavily
on studies conducted on university undergraduates in academic settings.
Over 70% of experiments in Sears' study used North American
undergraduates as subjects, a subset of the population that may not be
representative of the population as a whole.
Regardless of which method has been chosen to be used, the
results are of high importance. Results need to be used to evaluate the
hypothesis of the research that is done. These results should either
confirm or reject the original hypothesis that was predicted.There are
two different types of testing social psychologists use in order to test
their results. Statistics and probability testing define a significant finding that can be as low as 5% or less, likely to be due to chance. Replications are important, to ensure that the result is valid and not due to chance, or some feature of a particular sample. False positive conclusions, often resulting from the pressure to publish or the author's own confirmation bias, are a hazard in the field.
Ethics
The goal
of social psychology is to understand cognition and behavior as they
naturally occur in a social context, but the very act of observing
people can influence and alter their behavior. For this reason, many
social psychology experiments utilize deception
to conceal or distort certain aspects of the study. Deception may
include false cover stories, false participants (known as confederates
or stooges), false feedback given to the participants, and so on.
The practice of deception has been challenged by some
psychologists who maintain that deception under any circumstances is
unethical, and that other research strategies (e.g., role-playing)
should be used instead. Unfortunately, research has shown that
role-playing studies do not produce the same results as deception
studies and this has cast doubt on their validity.
In addition to deception, experimenters have at times put people into
potentially uncomfortable or embarrassing situations (e.g., the Milgram experiment), and this has also been criticized for ethical reasons.
To protect the rights and well-being of research participants,
and at the same time discover meaningful results and insights into human
behavior, virtually all social psychology research must pass an ethical review process. At most colleges and universities, this is conducted by an ethics committee or Institutional Review Board.
This group examines the proposed research to make sure that no harm is
likely to be done to the participants, and that the study's benefits
outweigh any possible risks or discomforts to people taking part in the
study.
Furthermore, a process of informed consent is often used to make sure that volunteers know what will happen in the experiment and understand that they are allowed to quit the experiment at any time. A debriefing
is typically done at the experiment's conclusion in order to reveal any
deceptions used and generally make sure that the participants are
unharmed by the procedures.
Today, most research in social psychology involves no more risk of harm
than can be expected from routine psychological testing or normal daily
activities.
Replication crisis
Social psychology has recently found itself at the center of a "replication crisis"
due to some research findings proving difficult to replicate.
Replication failures are not unique to social psychology and are found
in all fields of science. However, several factors have combined to put
social psychology at the center of the current controversy.
Firstly, questionable research practices
(QRP) have been identified as common in the field. Such practices,
while not necessarily intentionally fraudulent, involve converting
undesired statistical outcomes into desired outcomes via the
manipulation of statistical analyses, sample size or data management,
typically to convert non-significant findings into significant ones. Some studies have suggested that at least mild versions of QRP are highly prevalent. One of the critics of Daryl Bem in the feeling the future controversy has suggested that the evidence for precognition in this study could (at least in part) be attributed to QRP.
Secondly, social psychology has found itself at the center of
several recent scandals involving outright fraudulent research. Most
notably the admitted data fabrication by Diederik Stapel
as well as allegations against others. However, most scholars
acknowledge that fraud is, perhaps, the lesser contribution to
replication crises.
Third, several effects in social psychology have been found to be
difficult to replicate even before the current replication crisis. For
example, the scientific journal Judgment and Decision Making has published several studies over the years that fail to provide support for the unconscious thought theory.
Replications appear particularly difficult when research trials are
pre-registered and conducted by research groups not highly invested in
the theory under questioning.
These three elements together have resulted in renewed attention for replication supported by Daniel Kahneman.
Scrutiny of many effects have shown that several core beliefs are hard
to replicate. A recent special edition of the journal Social Psychology
focused on replication studies and a number of previously held beliefs
were found to be difficult to replicate. A 2012 special edition of the journal Perspectives on Psychological Science also focused on issues ranging from publication bias to null-aversion that contribute to the replication crises in psychology.
It is important to note that this replication crisis does not mean that social psychology is unscientific.
Rather this process is a healthy if sometimes acrimonious part of the
scientific process in which old ideas or those that cannot withstand
careful scrutiny are pruned.
The consequence is that some areas of social psychology once
considered solid, such as social priming, have come under increased
scrutiny due to failed replications.
Famous experiments
The Asch conformity experiments demonstrated the power of conformity in small groups with a line length estimation task that was designed to be extremely easy.
In well over a third of the trials, participants conformed to the
majority, who had been instructed to provide incorrect answers, even
though the majority judgment was clearly wrong. Seventy-five percent of
the participants conformed at least once during the experiment.
Additional manipulations to the experiment showed participant conformity
decreased when at least one other individual failed to conform, but
increased when the individual began conforming or withdrew from the
experiment.
Also, participant conformity increased substantially as the number of
incorrect individuals increased from one to three, and remained high as
the incorrect majority grew. Participants with three incorrect
opponents made mistakes 31.8% of the time, while those with one or two
incorrect opponents made mistakes only 3.6% and 13.6% of the time,
respectively.
Muzafer Sherif's Robbers' Cave Experiment
divided boys into two competing groups to explore how much hostility
and aggression would emerge. Sherif's explanation of the results became
known as realistic group conflict theory, because the intergroup
conflict was induced through competition over resources. Inducing cooperation and superordinate goals later reversed this effect.
In Leon Festinger's
cognitive dissonance experiment, participants were asked to perform a
boring task. They were divided into 2 groups and given two different pay
scales. At the study's end, some participants were paid $1 to say that
they enjoyed the task and another group of participants was paid $20 to
say the same lie. The first group ($1) later reported liking the task
better than the second group ($20). Festinger's explanation was that for
people in the first group being paid only $1 is not sufficient
incentive for lying and those who were paid $1 experienced dissonance.
They could only overcome that dissonance by justifying their lies by
changing their previously unfavorable attitudes about the task. Being
paid $20 provides a reason for doing the boring task, therefore no
dissonance.
One of the most notable experiments in social psychology was the Milgram experiment, which studied how far people would go to obey an authority figure. Following the events of The Holocaust
in World War II, the experiment showed that (most) normal American
citizens were capable of following orders from an authority even when
they believed they were causing an innocent person to suffer.
Albert Bandura's Bobo doll experiment demonstrated how aggression is learned by imitation. This set of studies fueled debates regarding media violence which continue to be waged among scholars.