Collective memory refers to the shared pool of memories, knowledge and information of a social group that is significantly associated with the group's identity. The English phrase "collective memory" and the equivalent French phrase "la mémoire collective" appeared in the second half of the nineteenth century. The philosopher and sociologist Maurice Halbwachs analyzed and advanced the concept of the collective memory in the book La mémoire collective (1950).
Collective memory can be constructed, shared, and passed on by large
and small social groups. Examples of these groups can include nations,
generations, communities among others. Collective memory has been a topic of interest and research across a number of disciplines, including psychology, sociology, history, philosophy and anthropology.
Conceptualization of collective memory
Attributes of collective memory
Collective
memory has been conceptualized in several ways and proposed to have
certain attributes. For instance, collective memory can refer to a
shared body of knowledge (e.g., memory of a nation's past leaders or
presidents);
the image, narrative, values and ideas of a social group; or the
continuous process by which collective memories of events change.
History versus collective memory
The
difference between history and collective memory is best understood
when comparing the aims and characteristics of each. A goal of history
broadly is to provide a comprehensive, accurate, and unbiased portrayal
of past events. This often includes the representation and comparison of
multiple perspectives and the integration of these perspectives and
details to provide a complete and accurate account. In contrast,
collective memory focuses on a single perspective, for instance, the
perspective of one social group, nation, or community. Consequently,
collective memory represents past events as associated with the values,
narratives and biases specific to that group.
Studies have found that people from different nations can have
major differences in their recollections of the past. In one study where
American and Russian students were instructed to recall significant
events from World War II and these lists of events were compared, the
majority of events recalled by the American and Russian students were
not shared.
Differences in the events recalled and emotional views towards the
Civil War, World War II and the Iraq War have also been found in a study
comparing collective memory between generations of Americans.
Perspectives on collective memory
The concept of collective memory, initially developed by Halbwachs, has been explored and expanded from various angles – a few of these are introduced below.
James E. Young has introduced the notion of 'collected memory'
(opposed to collective memory), marking memory's inherently fragmented,
collected and individual character, while Jan Assmann
develops the notion of 'communicative memory', a variety of collective
memory based on everyday communication. This form of memory is similar
to the exchanges in an oral culture or the memories collected (and made
collective) through oral history. As another subform of collective
memories Assmann mentions forms detached from the everyday, it can be
particular materialized and fixed points as, e.g. texts and monuments.
The theory of collective memory was also discussed by former Hiroshima resident and atomic bomb survivor, Kiyoshi Tanimoto,
in his tour of the United States as an attempt to rally support and
funding for the reconstruction of his Memorial Methodist Church in
Hiroshima. He theorized that the use of the atomic bomb had forever been
added to the world's collective memory and would serve in the future as
a warning against such devices.
The idea was also discussed more recently in The Celestine Prophecy and subsequent novels written by James Redfield
as a continuing process leading to the eventual transcendence of this
plane of existence. The idea that a futuristic development of the collective unconscious
and collective memories of society allowing for a medium with which one
can transcend ones existence is an idea expressed in certain variations
of new age religions.
The historian Guy Beiner,
an authority on memory and history on Ireland, has criticized the
unreflective use of the adjective "collective" in many studies of
memory:
The problem is with crude concepts of collectivity, which assume a homogeneity that is rarely, if ever, present, and maintain that, since memory is constructed, it is entirely subject to the manipulations of those invested in its maintenance, denying that there can be limits to the malleability of memory or to the extent to which artificial constructions of memory can be inculcated. In practice, the construction of a completely collective memory is at best an aspiration of politicians, which is never entirely fulfilled and is always subject to contestations.
In its place, Beiner has promoted the term "social memory" and has also demonstrated its limitations by developing a related concept of "social forgetting".
Collective memory and psychological research
Though
traditionally a topic studied in the humanities, collective memory has
become an area of interest in psychology. Common approaches taken in
psychology to study collective memory have included investigating the
cognitive mechanisms involved in the formation and transmission of
collective memory; and comparing the social representations of history
between social groups.
Social representations of history
Research
on collective memory have taken the approach to compare how different
social groups form their own representations of history and how such
collective memories can impact ideals, values, behaviors and vice versa.
Developing social identity and evaluating the past in order to prevent
past patterns of conflict and errors are proposed functions of why
groups form social representations of history. This research has focused
on surveying different groups or comparing differences in
recollections of historical events, such as the examples given earlier
when comparing history and collective memory.
Differences in collective memories between social groups, such as
nations or states, have been attributed to collective narcissism and
egocentric/ethnocentric bias. In one related study where participants
from 35 countries were questioned about their country's contribution to
world history and provided a percentage estimation from 0% to 100%,
evidence for collective narcissism was found as many countries gave
responses exaggerating their country's contribution. In another study
where American's from the 50 states were asked similar questions
regarding their state's contribution to the history of the United
States, patterns of overestimation and collective narcissism were also
found.
Cognitive mechanisms underlying collaborative recall
Certain
cognitive mechanisms involved during group recall and the interactions
between these mechanisms have been suggested to contribute to the
formation of collective memory. Below are some mechanisms involved
during when groups of individuals recall collaboratively.
Collaborative inhibition and retrieval disruption
When
groups collaborate to recall information, they experience collaborative
inhibition, a decrease in performance compared to the pooled memory
recall of an equal number of individuals. Weldon and Bellinger (1997)
and Basden, Basden, Bryner, and Thomas (1997) provided evidence that
retrieval interference underlies collaborative inhibition, as hearing
other members' thoughts and discussion about the topic at hand
interferes with one's own organization of thoughts and impairs memory. The main theoretical account for collaborative inhibition is retrieval disruption.
During the encoding of information, individuals form their own
idiosyncratic organization of the information. This organization is
later used when trying to recall the information. In a group setting as
members exchange information, the information recalled by group members
disrupts the idiosyncratic organization one had developed. As each
member's organization is disrupted, this results in the less information
recalled by the group compared to the pooled recall of participants who
had individually recalled (an equal number of participants as in the
group).
Despite the problem of collaborative inhibition, working in
groups may benefit an individual's memory in the long run, as group
discussion exposes one to many different ideas over time. Working alone
initially prior to collaboration seems to be the optimal way to increase
memory.
Early speculations about collaborative inhibition have included
explanations, such as diminished personal accountability, social loafing
and the diffusion of responsibility, however retrieval disruption
remains the leading explanation. Studies have found that collective
inhibition to sources other than social loafing, as offering a monetary
incentive have been evidenced to fail to produce an increase in memory
for groups.
Further evidence from this study suggest something other than social
loafing is at work, as reducing evaluation apprehension – the focus on
one's performance amongst other people – assisted in individuals'
memories but did not produce a gain in memory for groups. Personal
accountability – drawing attention to one's own performance and
contribution in a group – also did not reduce collaborative inhibition.
Therefore, group members' motivation to overcome the interference of
group recall cannot be achieved by several motivational factors.
Cross-cueing
Information
exchange among group members often helps individuals to remember things
that they would not have remembered had they been working alone. In
other words, the information provided by Person A may 'cue' memories in
Person B. This phenomenon results in enhanced recall.
Error pruning
Compared
to recalling individually, group members can provide opportunities for
error prune during recall to detect errors that would otherwise be
uncorrected by an individual.
Social contagion errors
Group
settings can also provide opportunities to exposure of erroneous
information that may be mistaken to be correct or previously studied.
Re-exposure effects
Listening
to group members recall the previously encoded information can enhance
memory as it provides a second exposure opportunity to the information.
Forgetting
Studies
have shown that information forgotten and excluded during group recall
can promote the forgetting of related information compared to
information unrelated to that which was excluded during group recall.
Selective forgetting has been suggested to be a critical mechanism
involved in the formation of collective memories and what details are
ultimately included and excluded by group members. This mechanism has
been studied using the socially shared retrieval induced forgetting paradigm, a variation of the retrieval induced forgetting method with individuals.
Synchronization of memories (from dyads to networks).
Bottom-up
approaches to the formation of collective memories investigate how
cognitive-level phenomena allow for people to synchronize their memories
following conversational remembering. Due to the malleability of human
memory, talking with one another about the past results in memory
changes that increase the similarity between the interactional partners'
memories.
When these dyadic interactions occur in a social network, one can
understand how large communities converge on a similar memory of the
past.
Research on larger interactions show that collective memory in larger
social networks can emerge due to cognitive mechanisms involved in small
group interactions.
Collective memory, culture and the public sphere
Memorialization of collective memory
The
collective memory of a nation is represented in part by the memorials
it chooses to erect. Public memory is enshrined in memorials from the Holocaust memorial in Berlin to the Vietnam Veterans Memorial
in Washington, DC. Whatever a nation chooses to memorialize in physical
monument, or perhaps more significantly, what not to memorialize, is an
indicator of the collective memory.
Collective memory is also sustained through a continuous
production of representational forms. In the media age – and maybe
particularly during the last decade of increasing digitization
– this generates a flow of, and production of, second hand memories
(see James E. Young below). Particular narratives and images are
reproduced and reframed, yet also questioned and contested through new
images and so forth. Collective memory today differs much from the
collective memories of an oral culture, where no printing technique or
transportation contributed to the production of imagined communities
in which people come to share a sense of heritage and commonality with
many human beings we have never met – as in the manner a citizen may
feel a sort of 'kinship' with people of the same nation, region or city.
Mass media and collective memory
Film and television
The arrival of film
created many images, film scenes, news scenes, photographs, quotes, and
songs, which became very familiar to regular moviegoers and remained in
their collective memory. Images of particular movie stars became part
of collective memory. During cinema visits, people could watch newsreels
of news stories from around the world. For the first time in history a
mass audience was able to view certain stories, events, and scenes, all
at the same time. They could all view how for instance the Hindenburg disaster was caught on camera and see and remember these scenes all at once.
When television became a global mass entertainment medium in the
1950s and 1960s the collective memory of former cinema visitors
increased when various films could be repeated endlessly and worldwide
on television broadcasts. For example, old films like The Wizard of Oz, King Kong and cartoons such as Looney Tunes and Tom and Jerry have been shown internationally and remained on television, through syndication.
Hereby particular film scenes have become well-known, even to people
who had not seen these films on their original cinematic release. The
same applies for television shows like I Love Lucy
which have been repeated so often over the decades that certain
episodes and scenes have become ingrained in the public's collective
memory.
When newsreels in the cinema gradually made place for television news broadcasting,
it became a habit for mass audiences to watch the daily news on
television. Worldwide this led to a new kind of collective memory where
various news events could be shown much quicker than with the cinema
newsreels. Therefore, certain filmed news stories could be shown on the
same day they happened and even live during the broadcast itself.
Millions of people have viewed the assassination of John F. Kennedy in 1963, the landing of Apollo 11 in 1969, the wedding of Prince Charles and Princess Diana in 1981, the death of Princess Diana, and the September 11 attacks on their televisions. In fact, certain questions like "What were you doing when.... happened?",
usually referring to a large, heavily-mediatized event, have become a
very important question in the history of the development of the
collective memory.
Many people can remember what they were doing when certain
internationally big media events occurred and these type of questions
are usually used as a sort of milestone in individual people's life. For example, "What were you doing when you heard that John Lennon was shot?". Due to television repeats, these moments could be relived even long after the actual event happened. The introduction of video stores and video recorders
in the 1980s, the internet in the 1990s and the DVD player and YouTube
in the 2000s even increased the opportunity to view and check out famous
and infamous movie and TV scenes.
Thanks to all these innovations certain scenes have become part
of audiences' collective memory. This makes it easy for journalists,
comedians, advertisers, politicians, etc. to make references to these
scenes, knowing that a large audience will recognise and understand them
without further explanation. For example, when president Ronald Reagan concluded a speech on March 13, 1985 against the increase of taxes he said "Make my day". Most people in the audience and TV viewers understood the reference to the Clint Eastwood film Sudden Impact and many laughed and cheered in response. The dance moves from Michael Jackson's music video for "Thriller"
have been repeatedly shown on TV so much that they are instantly
recognizable and therefore imitated frequently for comedic effect in
films, TV shows, commercials, etc.
Whenever a comedy show or film features a scene where someone is
killed or threatened in a shower, most people understand it as a parody
of Psycho. Various cartoons from Bugs Bunny to Shrek have spoofed famous fairy tales,
knowing that everybody is familiar with the original stories and will
immediately laugh at every deviation. The roar of movie monster Godzilla and Johnny Weissmuller's Tarzan yell have become instantly recognizable and easy to put into a context, even without the images.
Numerous TV shows and films such as The Simpsons, Family Guy, Scary Movie, the Shrek films, and the films of Mel Brooks, have referenced, parodied, imitated and recreated these famous scenes, often to the point of overkill. Certain observers, like Kenneth Tynan
in a quote from his diaries from October 19, 1975 have noted that due
to the heavy rotation and repeats of all these famous film scenes, often
even without their original context, they have become part of the
cultural consciousness. He wrote:
Nobody took into account the tremendous impact that would be made by the fact that films are permanent and easily accessible from childhood onward. As the sheer number of films piles up, their influence will increase, until we have a civilization entirely molded by cinematic values and behavior patterns.
The influence of television scenes on collective memory has been
noticeable with children who are able to quote lines and songs from
commercials, films and television shows they have watched regularly.
Some young children who have watched a large amount of television have
been known to react in an unnatural way to certain situations,
comparable with overacting,
because they recreate scenes they remember seeing in similar situations
on television. There have been cases reported of people who've compared
their own life too much with the romanticized, idealized life depicted
in films and television series. They try to recreate the happy families
and perfect love relationships they remember seeing on television or in
movies.
Not all scenes that were once collective memory are remembered as
well today. Certain shows, commercials and films that were popular in
one decade are shown less frequently on television in the next. Thus,
certain scenes do not rest in the collective memory of the next
generation. Many references in old Bugs Bunny
cartoons to Hollywood stars and radio shows who were famous in the
1940s, are almost obscure to modern viewers. On the other hand, certain
scenes have remained in the collective memory, due to being constantly
repeated in other media and are well known even for those unfamiliar
with the original. For example, even people who never saw the film King Kong know that there is a scene in which the large ape climbs the Empire State Building with a woman in his hand. This could be a negative side effect of the multi-referential nature films and television shows.
Younger audiences, unfamiliar with the original subject being
referenced in a contemporary film or TV series, do not recognize the
reference and assume that, for instance a Twilight Zone plot reference in The Simpsons has been thought up by the creators of The Simpsons
instead of the other way around. In some cases, references or parodies
of older movies in contemporary films and TV shows are almost comparable
to plagiarism since they just mimic or imitate a famous scene frame-by-frame instead of adding a funny new element.
In a more general and global perspective, the work of Jeffrey
Andrew Barash emphasizes the ways in which the mass media select,
articulate and transmit reported events and thus endow them with public
significance. Mass media representation of communicated events
configures them in accord with spatio-temporal patterns and a logic that
are not simple replicas of the order of everyday experience, since
disseminated information is charged with an autonomous symbolic sense
through which public awareness is channeled and sedimented in collective
memory. This autonomous symbolic sense draws its potency from an
uncanny ability to simulate direct experience while dissimulating
the gap which separates it from the immediate life world in which it
originates. The potency of the mass media format appears in a
particularly clear light in examples such as the televised Romanian
revolution, media representation of the Balkan wars, and the mediatized O. J. Simpson trial in the United States.
Music
This notion
of collective memory overflows into the music and film world. Certain
references and songs have permeated through culture and invoke certain
reactions in a wide social group. This makes it easy to make references
to these scenes and songs, knowing that a large audience will recognize
and understand them without further explanation.
Soundtracks have been instrumental to cinema and television as a
subtler form of expression and identity. Music, and more specifically
soundtracks, can be utilized as an outlet for hope, possibility and
resistance for everyday people. In Time Passages, George Lipsitz
acknowledged that "dominant ideology triumphed on television in the
1950s, just as it did in political and social life" (Lipsitz, 67).
However, recently movies and television shows such as Insecure, Super Fly, and Waiting to Exhale
have been able to incorporate music to spread “other” culture and
foster a community feel. The music not only grounds itself in time but
also helps personify the complex characters. The combination of new and
classic songs helps promote these ideals. Sharing music and exchanging
songs and in turn facilitating a collective memory also connects a
person to their larger community. In "Record and Hold," Jose Van Dijck
looked at how this “Shared listening, exchanging songs, and talking
about music create a sense of belonging, and connect a person’s sense of
self to a larger community and generation” (Van Dijck, 357). The
same song can elicit different memories and emotions from different
people – but they remain a sign of their time and location. Collective
memory highlights the power of television and popular culture to
influence politics and offer a glimpse into other people's social
realities. The music incorporated in popular television and film culture
can also play a role in young people's development of their identities.
Van Dijck wrote, "Recorded music also has a formative function: young
people in particular construct their identities while figuring out their
musical taste" (Van Dijck 359). Television and movies can have just as
big of an impact on cultural identities as any history book.