Cognitive neuroscience is the scientific field that is concerned with the study of the biological processes and aspects that underlie cognition, with a specific focus on the neural connections in the brain which are involved in mental processes.
It addresses the questions of how cognitive activities are affected or
controlled by neural circuits in the brain. Cognitive neuroscience is a
branch of both neuroscience and psychology, overlapping with disciplines such as behavioral neuroscience, cognitive psychology, physiological psychology and affective neuroscience. Cognitive neuroscience relies upon theories in cognitive science coupled with evidence from neurobiology, and computational modeling.
Parts of the brain play an important role in this field. Neurons play the most vital role, since the main point is to establish an understanding of cognition from a neural perspective, along with the different lobes of the cerebral cortex.
Methods employed in cognitive neuroscience include experimental procedures from psychophysics and cognitive psychology, functional neuroimaging, electrophysiology, cognitive genomics, and behavioral genetics.
Studies of patients with cognitive deficits due to brain lesions constitute an important aspect of cognitive neuroscience. The damages in lesioned brains provide a comparable basis with regards to healthy and fully functioning brains. These damages change the neural circuits in the brain and cause it to malfunction during basic cognitive processes, such as memory or learning. With the damage, we can compare how the healthy neural circuits are functioning, and possibly draw conclusions about the basis of the affected cognitive processes.
Also, cognitive abilities based on brain development are studied and examined under the subfield of developmental cognitive neuroscience. This shows brain development over time, analyzing differences and concocting possible reasons for those differences.
Theoretical approaches include computational neuroscience and cognitive psychology.
Parts of the brain play an important role in this field. Neurons play the most vital role, since the main point is to establish an understanding of cognition from a neural perspective, along with the different lobes of the cerebral cortex.
Methods employed in cognitive neuroscience include experimental procedures from psychophysics and cognitive psychology, functional neuroimaging, electrophysiology, cognitive genomics, and behavioral genetics.
Studies of patients with cognitive deficits due to brain lesions constitute an important aspect of cognitive neuroscience. The damages in lesioned brains provide a comparable basis with regards to healthy and fully functioning brains. These damages change the neural circuits in the brain and cause it to malfunction during basic cognitive processes, such as memory or learning. With the damage, we can compare how the healthy neural circuits are functioning, and possibly draw conclusions about the basis of the affected cognitive processes.
Also, cognitive abilities based on brain development are studied and examined under the subfield of developmental cognitive neuroscience. This shows brain development over time, analyzing differences and concocting possible reasons for those differences.
Theoretical approaches include computational neuroscience and cognitive psychology.
Historical origins
Cognitive neuroscience is an interdisciplinary area of study that has emerged from neuroscience and psychology.
There were several stages in these disciplines that changed the way
researchers approached their investigations and that led to the field
becoming fully established.
Although the task of cognitive neuroscience is to describe how
the brain creates the mind, historically it has progressed by
investigating how a certain area of the brain supports a given mental
faculty. However, early efforts to subdivide the brain proved to be
problematic. The phrenologist movement failed to supply a scientific
basis for its theories and has since been rejected. The aggregate field
view, meaning that all areas of the brain participated in all behavior, was also rejected as a result of brain mapping, which began with Hitzig and Fritsch’s experiments and eventually developed through methods such as positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Gestalt theory, neuropsychology, and the cognitive revolution
were major turning points in the creation of cognitive neuroscience as a
field, bringing together ideas and techniques that enabled researchers
to make more links between behavior and its neural substrates.
Origins in philosophy
Philosophers
have always been interested in the mind: "the idea that explaining a
phenomenon involves understanding the mechanism responsible for it has
deep roots in the History of Philosophy from atomic theories in 5th
century B.C. to its rebirth in the 17th and 18th century in the works of
Galileo, Descartes, and Boyle. Among others, it’s Descartes’ idea that
machines humans build could work as models of scientific explanation."
For example, Aristotle thought the brain was the body’s cooling system and the capacity for intelligence was located in the heart. It has been suggested that the first person to believe otherwise was the Roman physician Galen in the second century AD, who declared that the brain was the source of mental activity, although this has also been accredited to Alcmaeon. However, Galen believed that personality and emotion were not generated by the brain, but rather by other organs. Andreas Vesalius,
an anatomist and physician, was the first to believe that the brain and
the nervous system are the center of the mind and emotion. Psychology, a major contributing field to cognitive neuroscience, emerged from philosophical reasoning about the mind.
19th century
Phrenology
One of the predecessors to cognitive neuroscience was phrenology, a pseudoscientific approach that claimed that behavior could be determined by the shape of the scalp. In the early 19th century, Franz Joseph Gall and J. G. Spurzheim
believed that the human brain was localized into approximately 35
different sections. In his book, The Anatomy and Physiology of the
Nervous System in General, and of the Brain in Particular, Gall claimed
that a larger bump in one of these areas meant that that area of the
brain was used more frequently by that person. This theory gained
significant public attention, leading to the publication of phrenology
journals and the creation of phrenometers, which measured the bumps on a
human subject's head. While phrenology remained a fixture at fairs and
carnivals, it did not enjoy wide acceptance within the scientific
community. The major criticism of phrenology is that researchers were not able to test theories empirically.
Localizationist view
The
localizationist view was concerned with mental abilities being
localized to specific areas of the brain rather than on what the
characteristics of the abilities were and how to measure them. Studies performed in Europe, such as those of John Hughlings Jackson, supported this view. Jackson studied patients with brain damage, particularly those with epilepsy. He discovered that the epileptic patients often made the same clonic
and tonic movements of muscle during their seizures, leading Jackson to
believe that they must be occurring in the same place every time.
Jackson proposed that specific functions were localized to specific
areas of the brain, which was critical to future understanding of the brain lobes.
Aggregate field view
According to the aggregate field view, all areas of the brain participate in every mental function.
Pierre Flourens, a French experimental psychologist, challenged the localizationist view by using animal experiments. He discovered that removing the cerebellum
in rabbits and pigeons affected their sense of muscular coordination,
and that all cognitive functions were disrupted in pigeons when the cerebral hemispheres were removed. From this he concluded that the cerebral cortex, cerebellum, and brainstem functioned together as a whole.
His approach has been criticised on the basis that the tests were not
sensitive enough to notice selective deficits had they been present.
Emergence of neuropsychology
Perhaps the first serious attempts to localize mental functions to specific locations in the brain was by Broca and Wernicke. This was mostly achieved by studying the effects of injuries to different parts of the brain on psychological functions.
In 1861, French neurologist Paul Broca came across a man who was able
to understand language but unable to speak. The man could only produce
the sound "tan". It was later discovered that the man had damage to an
area of his left frontal lobe now known as Broca's area. Carl Wernicke, a German neurologist, found a patient who could speak fluently but non-sensibly. The patient had been the victim of a stroke,
and could not understand spoken or written language. This patient had a
lesion in the area where the left parietal and temporal lobes meet, now
known as Wernicke's area. These cases, which suggested that lesions caused specific behavioral changes, strongly supported the localizationist view.
Mapping the brain
In 1870, German physicians Eduard Hitzig and Gustav Fritsch
published their findings about the behavior of animals. Hitzig and
Fritsch ran an electric current through the cerebral cortex of a dog,
causing different muscles to contract depending on which areas of the
brain were electrically stimulated. This led to the proposition that
individual functions are localized to specific areas of the brain rather
than the cerebrum as a whole, as the aggregate field view suggests. Brodmann
was also an important figure in brain mapping; his experiments based on
Franz Nissl’s tissue staining techniques divided the brain into
fifty-two areas.
20th century
Cognitive revolution
At the start of the 20th century, attitudes in America were characterised by pragmatism, which led to a preference for behaviorism as the primary approach in psychology. J.B. Watson
was a key figure with his stimulus-response approach. By conducting
experiments on animals he was aiming to be able to predict and control
behaviour. Behaviourism eventually failed because it could not provide
realistic psychology of human action and thought – it focused primarily
on stimulus-response associations at the expense of explaining phenomena
like thought and imagination. This led to what is often termed as the
"cognitive revolution".
Neuron doctrine
In the early 20th century, Santiago Ramón y Cajal and Camillo Golgi
began working on the structure of the neuron. Golgi developed a silver staining method
that could entirely stain several cells in a particular area, leading
him to believe that neurons were directly connected with each other in
one cytoplasm. Cajal challenged this view after staining areas of the
brain that had less myelin and discovering that neurons were discrete
cells. Cajal also discovered that cells transmit electrical signals down
the neuron in one direction only. Both Golgi and Cajal were awarded a
Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1906 for this work on the
neuron doctrine.
Mid-late 20th century
Several
findings in the 20th century continued to advance the field, such as
the discovery of ocular dominance columns, recording of single nerve
cells in animals, and coordination of eye and head movements.
Experimental psychology was also significant in the foundation of
cognitive neuroscience. Some particularly important results were the
demonstration that some tasks are accomplished via discrete processing
stages, the study of attention,
and the notion that behavioural data do not provide enough information
by themselves to explain mental processes. As a result, some
experimental psychologists began to investigate neural bases of
behaviour.
Wilder Penfield created maps of primary sensory and motor areas of the
brain by stimulating cortices of patients during surgery. The work of
Sperry and Michael Gazzaniga on split brain patients in the 1950s was also instrumental in the progress of the field. The term cognitive neuroscience itself was coined by Gazzaniga and cognitive psychologist George Armitage Miller while sharing a taxi in 1976.
Brain mapping
New brain mapping technology, particularly fMRI and PET, allowed researchers to investigate experimental strategies of cognitive psychology
by observing brain function. Although this is often thought of as a new
method (most of the technology is relatively recent), the underlying
principle goes back as far as 1878 when blood flow was first associated
with brain function. Angelo Mosso,
an Italian psychologist of the 19th century, had monitored the
pulsations of the adult brain through neurosurgically created bony
defects in the skulls of patients. He noted that when the subjects
engaged in tasks such as mathematical calculations the pulsations of the
brain increased locally. Such observations led Mosso to conclude that
blood flow of the brain followed function.
Emergence of a new discipline
Birth of cognitive science
On September 11, 1956, a large-scale meeting of cognitivists took place at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. George A. Miller presented his "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two" paper while Noam Chomsky and Newell & Simon presented their findings on computer science. Ulric Neisser commented on many of the findings at this meeting in his 1967 book Cognitive Psychology.
The term "psychology" had been waning in the 1950s and 1960s, causing
the field to be referred to as "cognitive science". Behaviorists such as
Miller began to focus on the representation of language rather than
general behavior. David Marr
concluded that one should understand any cognitive process at three
levels of analysis. These levels include computational,
algorithmic/representational, and physical levels of analysis.
Combining neuroscience and cognitive science
Before the 1980s, interaction between neuroscience and cognitive science was scarce.
Cognitive neuroscience began to integrate the newly laid theoretical
ground in cognitive science, that emerged between the 1950s and 1960s,
with approaches in experimental psychology, neuropsychology and
neuroscience. (Neuroscience was not established as a unified discipline
until 1971).
In the very late 20th century new technologies evolved that are now the
mainstay of the methodology of cognitive neuroscience, including TMS (1985) and fMRI (1991). Earlier methods used in cognitive neuroscience include EEG (human EEG 1920) and MEG (1968). Occasionally cognitive neuroscientists utilize other brain imaging methods such as PET and SPECT. An upcoming technique in neuroscience is NIRS which uses light absorption to calculate changes in oxy- and deoxyhemoglobin in cortical areas. In some animals Single-unit recording can be used. Other methods include microneurography, facial EMG, and eye tracking. Integrative neuroscience
attempts to consolidate data in databases, and form unified descriptive
models from various fields and scales: biology, psychology, anatomy,
and clinical practice. In 2014, Stanislas Dehaene, Giacomo Rizzolatti and Trevor Robbins, were awarded the Brain Prize
"for their pioneering research on higher brain mechanisms underpinning
such complex human functions as literacy, numeracy, motivated behaviour
and social cognition, and for their efforts to understand cognitive and
behavioural disorders". Brenda Milner, Marcus Raichle and John O'Keefe received the Kavli Prize in Neuroscience “for the discovery of specialized brain networks for memory and cognition" and O'Keefe shared the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in the same year with May-Britt Moser and Edvard Moser "for their discoveries of cells that constitute a positioning system in the brain". In 2017, Wolfram Schultz, Peter Dayan and Ray Dolan
were awarded the Brain Prize "for their multidisciplinary analysis of
brain mechanisms that link learning to reward, which has far-reaching
implications for the understanding of human behaviour, including
disorders of decision-making in conditions such as gambling, drug
addiction, compulsive behaviour and schizophrenia".
Major contributors to the field
Hubel and Wiesel – 1960s
David H. Hubel and Torsten Wiesel, both neurophysiologists, studied the visual system in cats to better understand sensory processing. They performed experiments which demonstrated the specificity of the responding of neurons.
Their experiments showed that neurons fired rapidly at some angles, and
not so much at others. A difference was also found in light and dark
settings. Their studies gave rise to the idea of complex visual representations being formed from relatively simple stimuli.
They also discovered the simple cell and complex cell. These exist in the primary visual cortex and respond differentially to differently oriented presentations of light.
Recent trends
Recently
the foci of research have expanded from the localization of brain
area(s) for specific functions in the adult brain using a single
technology, studies have been diverging in several different directions:
exploring the interactions between different brain areas, using
multiple technologies and approaches to understand brain functions, and
using computational approaches.
Advances in non-invasive functional neuroimaging and associated data
analysis methods have also made it possible to use highly naturalistic
stimuli and tasks such as feature films depicting social interactions in
cognitive neuroscience studies.