https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Memetics
Memetics is the study of information and culture based on an analogy with Darwinian evolution. Proponents describe memetics as an approach to evolutionary models of cultural information transfer. Memetics describes how an idea can propagate successfully, but doesn't necessarily imply a concept is factual. Critics contend the theory is "untested, unsupported or incorrect".
The term meme was coined in Richard Dawkins' 1976 book The Selfish Gene, but Dawkins later distanced himself from the resulting field of study. Analogous to a gene, the meme was conceived as a "unit of culture" (an idea, belief, pattern of behaviour, etc.) which is "hosted" in the minds of one or more individuals, and which can reproduce itself in the sense of jumping from the mind of one person to the mind of another. Thus what would otherwise be regarded as one individual influencing another to adopt a belief is seen as an idea-replicator reproducing itself in a new host. As with genetics, particularly under a Dawkinsian interpretation, a meme's success may be due to its contribution to the effectiveness of its host.
The Usenet newsgroup alt.memetics started in 1993 with peak posting years in the mid to late 1990s. The Journal of Memetics was published electronically from 1997 to 2005.
Memetics is the study of information and culture based on an analogy with Darwinian evolution. Proponents describe memetics as an approach to evolutionary models of cultural information transfer. Memetics describes how an idea can propagate successfully, but doesn't necessarily imply a concept is factual. Critics contend the theory is "untested, unsupported or incorrect".
The term meme was coined in Richard Dawkins' 1976 book The Selfish Gene, but Dawkins later distanced himself from the resulting field of study. Analogous to a gene, the meme was conceived as a "unit of culture" (an idea, belief, pattern of behaviour, etc.) which is "hosted" in the minds of one or more individuals, and which can reproduce itself in the sense of jumping from the mind of one person to the mind of another. Thus what would otherwise be regarded as one individual influencing another to adopt a belief is seen as an idea-replicator reproducing itself in a new host. As with genetics, particularly under a Dawkinsian interpretation, a meme's success may be due to its contribution to the effectiveness of its host.
The Usenet newsgroup alt.memetics started in 1993 with peak posting years in the mid to late 1990s. The Journal of Memetics was published electronically from 1997 to 2005.
History
In his book The Selfish Gene (1976), the evolutionary biologist Richard Dawkins used the term meme to describe a unit of human cultural transmission analogous to the gene, arguing that replication also happens in culture, albeit in a different sense. In 1975, Dr. Ted Cloak outlined the "corpuscles of culture" - an inspiring hypothesis that Dawkins referenced. Cultural evolution itself is a much older topic, with a history that dates back at least as far as Darwin's era.
Dawkins (1976) proposed that the meme is a unit of information residing in the brain and is the mutating replicator in human cultural evolution.
It is a pattern that can influence its surroundings – that is, it has
causal agency – and can propagate. This proposal resulted in debate
among sociologists, biologists, and scientists of other disciplines.
Dawkins himself did not provide a sufficient explanation of how the
replication of units of information in the brain controls human
behaviour and ultimately culture, and the principal topic of the book
was genetics. Dawkins apparently did not intend to present a
comprehensive theory of memetics in The Selfish Gene, but rather coined the term meme in a speculative spirit. Accordingly, different researchers came to define the term "unit of information" in different ways.
The modern memetics movement dates from the mid-1980s. A January 1983 "Metamagical Themas" column by Douglas Hofstadter, in Scientific American, was influential – as was his 1985 book of the same name. "Memeticist" was coined as analogous to "geneticist" – originally in The Selfish Gene.
Later Arel Lucas suggested that the discipline that studies memes and
their connections to human and other carriers of them be known as
"memetics" by analogy with "genetics". Dawkins' The Selfish Gene
has been a factor in attracting the attention of people of disparate
intellectual backgrounds. Another stimulus was the publication in 1991
of Consciousness Explained by Tufts University philosopher Daniel Dennett, which incorporated the meme concept into a theory of the mind. In his 1991 essay "Viruses of the Mind",
Richard Dawkins used memetics to explain the phenomenon of religious
belief and the various characteristics of organised religions. By then,
memetics had also become a theme appearing in fiction (e.g. Neal
Stephenson's Snow Crash).
The idea of language as a virus had already been introduced by William S. Burroughs as early as 1962 in his book The Ticket That Exploded, and later in The Electronic Revolution, published in 1970 in The Job. Douglas Rushkoff explored the same concept in Media Virus: Hidden Agendas in Popular Culture in 1995.
However, the foundation of memetics in its full modern
incarnation originated in the publication in 1996 of two books by
authors outside the academic mainstream: Virus of the Mind: The New Science of the Meme by former Microsoft executive turned motivational speaker and professional poker-player Richard Brodie, and Thought Contagion: How Belief Spreads Through Society by Aaron Lynch, a mathematician and philosopher who worked for many years as an engineer at Fermilab.
Lynch claimed to have conceived his theory totally independently of any
contact with academics in the cultural evolutionary sphere, and
apparently was not aware of The Selfish Gene until his book was very close to publication.
Around the same time as the publication of the books by Lynch and Brodie the e-journal Journal of Memetics – Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission appeared on the web. It was first hosted by the Centre for Policy Modelling at Manchester Metropolitan University but later taken over by Francis Heylighen of the CLEA research institute at the Vrije Universiteit Brussel.
The e-journal soon became the central point for publication and debate
within the nascent memeticist community. (There had been a short-lived
paper-based memetics publication starting in 1990, the Journal of Ideas edited by Elan Moritz.) In 1999, Susan Blackmore, a psychologist at the University of the West of England, published The Meme Machine,
which more fully worked out the ideas of Dennett, Lynch, and Brodie and
attempted to compare and contrast them with various approaches from the
cultural evolutionary mainstream, as well as providing novel, and
controversial, memetics-based theories for the evolution of language and
the human sense of individual selfhood.
Etymology
The term meme derives from the Ancient Greek μιμητής (mimētḗs), meaning "imitator, pretender". The similar term mneme was used in 1904, by the German evolutionary biologist Richard Semon, best known for his development of the engram theory of memory, in his work Die mnemischen Empfindungen in ihren Beziehungen zu den Originalempfindungen, translated into English in 1921 as The Mneme. Until Daniel Schacter published Forgotten Ideas, Neglected Pioneers: Richard Semon and the Story of Memory in 2000, Semon's work had little influence, though it was quoted extensively in Erwin Schrödinger’s 1956 Tarner Lecture “Mind and Matter”. Richard Dawkins (1976) apparently coined the word meme independently of Semon, writing this:
"'Mimeme' comes from a suitable Greek root, but I want a monosyllable that sounds a bit like 'gene'. I hope my classicist friends will forgive me if I abbreviate mimeme to meme. If it is any consolation, it could alternatively be thought of as being related to 'memory', or to the French word même."
Maturity
In 2005, the Journal of Memetics – Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission
ceased publication and published a set of articles on the future of
memetics. The website states that although "there was to be a
relaunch...after several years nothing has happened". Susan Blackmore
has left the University of the West of England to become a freelance
science-writer and now concentrates more on the field of consciousness
and cognitive science. Derek Gatherer moved to work as a computer
programmer in the pharmaceutical industry, although he still
occasionally publishes on memetics-related matters. Richard Brodie is now climbing the world professional poker rankings. Aaron Lynch
disowned the memetics community and the words "meme" and "memetics"
(without disowning the ideas in his book), adopting the self-description
"thought contagionist". He died in 2005.
Susan Blackmore
(2002) re-stated the definition of meme as: whatever is copied from one
person to another person, whether habits, skills, songs, stories, or
any other kind of information. Further she said that memes, like genes,
are replicators in the sense as defined by Dawkins.
That is, they are information that is copied. Memes are copied by imitation,
teaching and other methods. The copies are not perfect: memes are
copied with variation; moreover, they compete for space in our memories
and for the chance to be copied again. Only some of the variants can
survive. The combination of these three elements (copies; variation;
competition for survival) forms precisely the condition for Darwinian evolution, and so memes (and hence human cultures) evolve. Large groups of memes that are copied and passed on together are called co-adapted meme complexes, or memeplexes. In Blackmore's definition, the way that a meme replicates is through imitation. This requires brain
capacity to generally imitate a model or selectively imitate the model.
Since the process of social learning varies from one person to another,
the imitation process cannot be said to be completely imitated. The
sameness of an idea may be expressed with different memes supporting it.
This is to say that the mutation
rate in memetic evolution is extremely high, and mutations are even
possible within each and every iteration of the imitation process. It
becomes very interesting when we see that a social system composed of a
complex network of microinteractions exists, but at the macro level an
order emerges to create culture.
Internalists and externalists
The
memetics movement split almost immediately into two. The first group
were those who wanted to stick to Dawkins' definition of a meme as "a
unit of cultural transmission". Gibron Burchett, another memeticist responsible for helping to research and co-coin the term memetic engineering, along with Leveious Rolando and Larry Lottman, has stated that a meme can be defined, more precisely, as "a unit of cultural information
that can be copied, located in the brain". This thinking is more in
line with Dawkins' second definition of the meme in his book The Extended Phenotype. The second group wants to redefine memes as observable cultural artifacts
and behaviors. However, in contrast to those two positions, Blackmore
does not reject either concept of external or internal memes.
These two schools became known as the "internalists" and the
"externalists." Prominent internalists included both Lynch and Brodie;
the most vocal externalists included Derek Gatherer, a geneticist from Liverpool John Moores University,
and William Benzon, a writer on cultural evolution and music. The main
rationale for externalism was that internal brain entities are not
observable, and memetics cannot advance as a science, especially a quantitative
science, unless it moves its emphasis onto the directly quantifiable
aspects of culture. Internalists countered with various arguments: that
brain states will eventually be directly observable with advanced
technology, that most cultural anthropologists agree that culture is
about beliefs
and not artifacts, or that artifacts cannot be replicators in the same
sense as mental entities (or DNA) are replicators. The debate became so
heated that a 1998 Symposium on Memetics, organised as part of the 15th
International Conference on Cybernetics,
passed a motion calling for an end to definitional debates. McNamara
demonstrated in 2011 that functional connectivity profiling using
neuroimaging tools enables the observation of the processing of internal
memes, "i-memes", in response to external "e-memes".
An advanced statement of the internalist school came in 2002 with the publication of The Electric Meme, by Robert Aunger, an anthropologist from the University of Cambridge.
Aunger also organised a conference in Cambridge in 1999, at which
prominent sociologists and anthropologists were able to give their
assessment of the progress made in memetics to that date. This resulted
in the publication of Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science, edited by Aunger and with a foreword by Dennett, in 2001.
Criticism
This evolutionary model of cultural information transfer is based on
the concept that units of information, or "memes", have an independent
existence, are self-replicating, and are subject to selective evolution
through environmental forces. Starting from a proposition put forward in the writings of Richard Dawkins,
this model has formed the basis of a new area of study, one that looks
at the self-replicating units of culture. It has been proposed that just
as memes are analogous to genes, memetics is analogous to genetics.
Critics contend that some proponents' assertions are "untested, unsupported or incorrect." Luis Benitez-Bribiesca, a critic of memetics, calls it "a pseudoscientific dogma" and "a dangerous idea that poses a threat to the serious study of consciousness and cultural evolution" among other things. As factual criticism, he refers to the lack of a code script
for memes, as the DNA is for genes, and to the fact that the meme
mutation mechanism (i.e., an idea going from one brain to another) is
too unstable (low replication accuracy and high mutation rate), which
would render the evolutionary process chaotic. This, however, has been demonstrated (e.g. by Daniel C. Dennett, in Darwin's Dangerous Idea)
to not be the case, in fact, due to the existence of self-regulating
correction mechanisms (vaguely resembling those of gene transcription)
enabled by the redundancy and other properties of most meme expression
languages, which do stabilize information transfer. (E.g. spiritual
narratives—including music and dance forms—can survive in full detail
across any number of generations even in cultures with oral tradition
only.) Memes for which stable copying methods are available will
inevitably get selected for survival more often than those which can
only have unstable mutations, therefore going extinct. (Notably,
Benitez-Bribiesca's claim of "no code script" is also irrelevant,
considering the fact that there is nothing preventing the information
contents of memes from being coded, encoded, expressed, preserved or
copied in all sorts of different ways throughout their life-cycles.)
Another criticism comes from semiotics, (e.g., Deacon, Kull) stating that the concept of meme is a primitivized concept of Sign.
Meme is thus described in memetics as a sign without its triadic
nature. In other words, meme is a degenerate sign, which includes only
its ability of being copied. Accordingly, in the broadest sense, the
objects of copying are memes, whereas the objects of translation and
interpretation are signs.
Mary Midgley criticises memetics for at least two reasons:
"One, culture is not best understood by examining its smallest parts,
as culture is pattern-like, comparable to an ocean current. Many more
factors, historical and others, should be taken into account than only
whatever particle culture is built from. Two, if memes are not thoughts
(and thus not cognitive phenomena), as Daniel C. Dennett insists in "Darwin's Dangerous Idea", then their ontological status is open to question, and memeticists (who are also reductionists)
may be challenged whether memes even exist. Questions can extend to
whether the idea of "meme" is itself a meme, or is a true concept.
Fundamentally, memetics is an attempt to produce knowledge through
organic metaphors, which as such is a questionable research approach, as
the application of metaphors has the effect of hiding that which does
not fit within the realm of the metaphor. Rather than study actual
reality, without preconceptions, memetics, as so many of the
socio-biological explanations of society, believe that saying that the
apple is like an orange is a valid analysis of the apple."
Henry Jenkins, Joshua Green, and Sam Ford, in their book Spreadable Media
(2013), criticize Dawkins' idea of the meme, writing that "while the
idea of the meme is a compelling one, it may not adequately account for
how content circulates through participatory culture." The three authors
also criticize other interpretations of memetics, especially those
which describe memes as "self-replicating", because they ignore the fact
that "culture is a human product and replicates through human agency."
Like other critics, Maria Kronfeldner has criticized memetics for
being based on an allegedly inaccurate analogy with the gene;
alternately, she claims it is "heuristically trivial", being a mere
redescription of what is already known without offering any useful
novelty.
New developments
Dawkins in A Devil's Chaplain
responded that there are actually two different types of memetic
processes (controversial and informative). The first is a type of
cultural idea, action, or expression, which does have high variance; for
instance, a student of his who had inherited some of the mannerisms of Wittgenstein. However, he also describes a self-correcting meme, highly resistant to mutation. As an example of this, he gives origami
patterns in elementary schools – except in rare cases, the meme is
either passed on in the exact sequence of instructions, or (in the case
of a forgetful child) terminates. This type of meme tends not to evolve,
and to experience profound mutations in the rare event that it does.
Another definition, given by Hokky Situngkir, tried to offer a more rigorous formalism for the meme, memeplexes, and the deme, seeing the meme as a cultural unit in a cultural complex system. It is based on the Darwinian genetic algorithm
with some modifications to account for the different patterns of
evolution seen in genes and memes. In the method of memetics as the way
to see culture as a complex adaptive system, he describes a way to see memetics as an alternative methodology of cultural evolution. However, there are as many possible definitions that are credited to the word "meme". For example, in the sense of computer simulation the term memetic algorithm is used to define a particular computational viewpoint.
The possibility of quantitative analysis of memes using neuroimaging tools and the suggestion that such studies have already been done was given by McNamara (2011).
This author proposes hyperscanning (concurrent scanning of two
communicating individuals in two separate MRI machines) as a key tool in
the future for investigating memetics.
Velikovsky (2013) proposed the "holon" as the structure of the meme, synthesizing the major theories on memes of Richard Dawkins, Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, E. O. Wilson, Frederick Turner (poet) and Arthur Koestler.
Proponents of memetics as described in the Journal of Memetics (out of print since 2005) – Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission
believe that 'memetics' has the potential to be an important and
promising analysis of culture using the framework of evolutionary
concepts.
Keith Henson in Memetics and the Modular-Mind (Analog Aug. 1987) makes the case that memetics needs to incorporate evolutionary psychology to understand the psychological traits of a meme's host.
DiCarlo (2010) has developed the idea of 'memetic equilibrium' to describe a cultural compatible state with biological equilibrium.
In "How Problem Solving and Neurotransmission in the Upper Paleolithic
led to The Emergence and Maintenance of Memetic Equilibrium in
Contemporary World Religions", diCarlo argues that as human
consciousness evolved and developed, so too did our ancestors' capacity
to consider and attempt to solve environmental problems in more
conceptually sophisticated ways. Understood in this way, problem solving
amongst a particular group, when considered satisfactory, often
produces a feeling of environmental control, stability, in short—memetic
equilibrium.
But the pay-off is not merely practical, providing purely functional
utility—it is biochemical and it comes in the form of neurotransmitters.
The relationship between a gradually emerging conscious awareness and
sophisticated languages in which to formulate representations combined
with the desire to maintain biological equilibrium, generated the
necessity for equilibrium to fill in conceptual gaps in terms of
understanding three very important aspects in the Upper Paleolithic:
causality, morality, and mortality. The desire to explain phenomena in
relation to maintaining survival and reproductive stasis, generated a
normative stance in the minds of our ancestors—Survival/Reproductive
Value (or S-R Value).
Houben (2014) has argued on several occasions that the exceptional resilience of Vedic ritual
and its interaction with a changing ecological and economic environment
over several millennia can be profitably dealt with in a ‘cultural
evolution’ perspective in which the Vedic mantra is the ‘meme’ or unit
of cultural replication.
This renders superfluous attempts to explain the phenomenon of Vedic tradition in genetic terms. The domain of Vedic ritual should be able to fulfil to a large extent the three challenges posed to memetics by B. Edmonds (2002 and 2005).
Applications
Research methodologies that apply memetics go by many names: Viral marketing,
cultural evolution, the history of ideas, social analytics, and more.
Many of these applications do not make reference to the literature on
memes directly but are built upon the evolutionary lens of idea
propagation that treats semantic units of culture as self-replicating
and mutating patterns of information that are assumed to be relevant for
scientific study. For example, the field of public relations is filled
with attempts to introduce new ideas and alter social discourse. One
means of doing this is to design a meme and deploy it through various
media channels. One historic example of applied memetics is the PR
campaign conducted in 1991 as part of the build-up to the first Gulf War
in the United States.
The application of memetics to a difficult complex social system problem, environmental sustainability, has recently been attempted at thwink.org
Using meme types and memetic infection in several stock and flow
simulation models, Jack Harich has demonstrated several interesting
phenomena that are best, and perhaps only, explained by memes. One
model, The Dueling Loops of the Political Powerplace,
argues that the fundamental reason corruption is the norm in politics
is due to an inherent structural advantage of one feedback loop pitted
against another. Another model, The Memetic Evolution of Solutions to
Difficult Problems, uses memes, the evolutionary algorithm, and the scientific method
to show how complex solutions evolve over time and how that process can
be improved. The insights gained from these models are being used to
engineer memetic solution elements to the sustainability problem.
Another application of memetics in the sustainability space is the crowdfunded Climate Meme Project[39]
conducted by Joe Brewer and Balazs Laszlo Karafiath in the spring of
2013. This study was based on a collection of 1000 unique text-based
expressions gathered from Twitter, Facebook, and structured interviews
with climate activists. The major finding was that the global warming
meme is not effective at spreading because it causes emotional duress in
the minds of people who learn about it. Five central tensions were
revealed in the discourse about [climate change], each of which
represents a resonance point through which dialogue can be engaged. The
tensions were Harmony/Disharmony (whether or not humans are part of the
natural world), Survival/Extinction (envisioning the future as either
apocalyptic collapse of civilization or total extinction of the human
race), Cooperation/Conflict (regarding whether or not humanity can come
together to solve global problems), Momentum/Hesitation (about whether
or not we are making progress at the collective scale to address climate
change), and Elitism/Heretic (a general sentiment that each side of the
debate considers the experts of its opposition to be untrustworthy).
Ben Cullen, in his book Contagious Ideas,
brought the idea of the meme into the discipline of archaeology. He
coined the term "Cultural Virus Theory", and used it to try to anchor
archaeological theory in a neo-Darwinian paradigm. Archaeological
memetics could assist the application of the meme concept to material
culture in particular.
Francis Heylighen of the Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies has postulated what he calls "memetic selection criteria". These criteria opened the way to a specialized field of applied memetics to find out if these selection criteria could stand the test of quantitative analyses. In 2003 Klaas Chielens carried out these tests in a Masters thesis project on the testability of the selection criteria.
In Selfish Sounds and Linguistic Evolution,
Austrian linguist Nikolaus Ritt has attempted to operationalise memetic
concepts and use them for the explanation of long term sound changes
and change conspiracies in early English. It is argued that a
generalised Darwinian framework for handling cultural change can provide
explanations where established, speaker centred approaches fail to do
so. The book makes comparatively concrete suggestions about the possible
material structure of memes, and provides two empirically rich case
studies.
Australian academic S.J. Whitty has argued that project management is a memeplex with the language and stories of its practitioners at its core.
This radical approach sees a project and its management as an illusion;
a human construct about a collection of feelings, expectations, and
sensations, which are created, fashioned, and labeled by the human
brain. Whitty's approach requires project managers to consider that the
reasons for using project management are not consciously driven to
maximize profit, and are encouraged to consider project management as
naturally occurring, self-serving, evolving process which shapes
organizations for its own purpose.
Swedish political scientist Mikael Sandberg argues against "Lamarckian" interpretations of institutional and technological evolution and studies creative innovation of information technologies in governmental and private organizations in Sweden in the 1990s from a memetic perspective.
Comparing the effects of active ("Lamarckian") IT strategy versus
user–producer interactivity (Darwinian co-evolution), evidence from
Swedish organizations shows that co-evolutionary interactivity is almost
four times as strong a factor behind IT creativity as the "Lamarckian"
IT strategy.