The salience (also called saliency) of an item is the state or quality by which it stands out from its neighbors. Saliency detection is considered to be a key attentional mechanism that facilitates learning and survival by enabling organisms to focus their limited perceptual and cognitive resources on the most pertinent subset of the available sensory data.
Saliency typically arises from contrasts between items and their neighborhood, such as a red dot surrounded by white dots, a flickering message indicator of an answering machine, or a loud noise in an otherwise quiet environment. Saliency detection is often studied in the context of the visual system, but similar mechanisms operate in other sensory systems. What is salient can be influenced by training: for example, for human subjects particular letters can become salient by training.
When attention deployment is driven by salient stimuli, it is considered to be bottom-up, memory-free, and reactive. Conversely, attention can also be guided by top-down, memory-dependent, or anticipatory mechanisms, such as when looking ahead of moving objects or sideways before crossing streets. Humans and other animals have difficulty paying attention to more than one item simultaneously, so they are faced with the challenge of continuously integrating and prioritizing different bottom-up and top-down influences.
Neuroanatomy
The brain component named the hippocampus
helps with the assessment of salience and context by using past
memories to filter new incoming stimuli, and placing those that are most
important into long term memory. The entorhinal
cortex is the pathway into and out of the hippocampus, and is an
important part of the brain's memory network; research shows that it is a
brain region that suffers damage early on in Alzheimer's disease, one of the effects of which is altered (diminished) salience.
The pulvinar nuclei (in the thalamus) modulates physical/perceptual salience in attentional selection.
One group of neurons (i.e., D1-type medium spiny neurons) within the nucleus accumbens shell (NAcc shell) assigns appetitive motivational salience ("want" and "desire", which includes a motivational component), aka incentive salience, to rewarding stimuli, while another group of neurons (i.e., D2-type medium spiny neurons) within the NAcc shell assigns aversive motivational salience to aversive stimuli.
The primary visual cortex (V1) generates a bottom-up saliency map
from visual inputs to guide reflexive attentional shifts or gaze
shifts. The saliency of a location is higher when V1 neurons give higher
responses to that location relative to V1 neurons' responses to other
visual locations.
For example, a unique red item among green items, or a unique
vertical bar among horizontal bars, is salient since it evokes higher V1
responses and attracts attention or gaze. The V1 neural responses are sent to the superior colluculus
to guide gaze shifts to the salient locations. A fingerprint of the
saliency map in V1 is that attention or gaze can be captured by the
location of an eye-of-origin singleton in visual inputs, e.g., a bar
uniquely shown to the left eye in a background of many other bars shown
to the right eye, even when observers cannot tell the difference between
the singleton and the background bars.
In psychology
The
term is widely used in the study of perception and cognition to refer
to any aspect of a stimulus that, for any of many reasons, stands out
from the rest. Salience may be the result of emotional, motivational or
cognitive factors and is not necessarily associated with physical
factors such as intensity, clarity or size. Although salience is thought
to determine attentional selection, salience associated with physical
factors does not necessarily influence selection of a stimulus.
Salience bias
Salience bias (also known as perceptual salience)
is the cognitive bias that predisposes individuals to focus on items
that are more prominent or emotionally striking and ignore those that
are unremarkable, even though this difference is often irrelevant by
objective standards. Salience bias is closely related to the concept of availability in behavioral economics:
Accessibility and salience are closely related to availability, and they are important as well. If you have personally experienced a serious earthquake, you’re more likely to believe that an earthquake is likely than if you read about it in a weekly magazine. Thus, vivid and easily imagined causes of death (for example, tornadoes) often receive inflated estimates of probability, and less-vivid causes (for example, asthma attacks) receive low estimates, even if they occur with a far greater frequency (here, by a factor of twenty). Timing counts too: more recent events have a greater impact on our behavior, and on our fears, than earlier ones.
— Richard H. Thaler, Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness (2008-04-08)
Aberrant salience hypothesis of schizophrenia
Kapur
(2003) proposed that a hyperdopaminergic state, at a "brain" level of
description, leads to an aberrant assignment of salience to the elements
of one's experience, at a "mind" level.
These aberrant salience attributions have been associated with altered
activities in the mesolimbic system, including the striatum, the
amygdala, the hippocampus, and the parahippocampal gyrus.
Dopamine mediates the conversion of the neural representation of an
external stimulus from a neutral bit of information into an attractive
or aversive entity, i.e. a salient event. Symptoms of schizophrenia
may arise out of 'the aberrant assignment of salience to external
objects and internal representations', and antipsychotic medications
reduce positive symptoms by attenuating aberrant motivational salience
via blockade of the dopamine D2 receptors (Kapur, 2003).
Alternative areas of investigation include supplementary motor
areas, frontal eye fields and parietal eye fields. These areas of the
brain are involved with calculating predictions and visual salience.
Changing expectations on where to look restructures these areas of the
brain. This cognitive repatterning can result in some of the symptoms
found in such disorders.
Visual saliency modeling
In the domain of psychology,
efforts have been made in modeling the mechanism of human attention,
including the learning of prioritizing the different bottom-up and
top-down influences.
In the domain of computer vision, efforts have been made in modeling the mechanism of human attention, especially the bottom-up attentional mechanism, including both spatial and temporal attention. Such a process is also called visual saliency detection.
Generally speaking, there are two kinds of models to mimic the
bottom-up saliency mechanism. One way is based on the spatial contrast
analysis: for example, a center-surround mechanism is used to define
saliency across scales, which is inspired by the putative neural
mechanism. The other way is based on the frequency domain analysis.
While they used the amplitude spectrum to assign saliency to rarely
occurring magnitudes, Guo et al. use the phase spectrum instead.
Recently, Li et al. introduced a system that uses both the amplitude and the phase information.
A key limitation in many such approaches is their computational
complexity leading to less than real-time performance, even on modern
computer hardware. Some recent work attempts to overcome these issues at the expense of saliency detection quality under some conditions.
Other work suggests that saliency and associated speed-accuracy
phenomena may be a fundamental mechanisms determined during recognition
through gradient descent, needing not be spatial in nature.