The forgetting curve hypothesizes the decline of memory
retention in time. This curve shows how information is lost over time
when there is no attempt to retain it. A related concept is the strength of memory that refers to the durability that memory traces in the brain. The stronger the memory, the longer period of time that a person is able to recall it. A typical graph of the forgetting
curve purports to show that humans tend to halve their memory of newly
learned knowledge in a matter of days or weeks unless they consciously
review the learned material.
The forgetting curve supports one of the seven kinds of memory
failures: transience, which is the process of forgetting that occurs
with the passage of time.
History
From 1880 to 1885, Hermann Ebbinghaus ran a limited, incomplete study on himself and published his hypothesis in 1885 as Über das Gedächtnis (later translated into English as Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology).
Ebbinghaus studied the memorisation of nonsense syllables, such as
"WID" and "ZOF" (CVCs or Consonant–Vowel–Consonant) by repeatedly
testing himself after various time periods and recording the results. He
plotted these results on a graph creating what is now known as the
"forgetting curve". Ebbinghaus investigated the rate of forgetting, but not the effect of spaced repetition on the increase in retrievability of memories.
Ebbinghaus's publication also included an equation to approximate his forgetting curve:
Here, represents 'Savings' expressed as a percentage, and
represents time in minutes. Savings is defined as the relative amount
of time saved on the second learning trial as a result of having had the
first. A savings of 100% would indicate that all items were still known
from the first trial. A 75% savings would mean that relearning missed
items required 25% as long as the original learning session (to learn
all items). 'Savings' is thus, analogous to retention rate.
In 2015, an attempt to replicate the forgetting curve with one
study subject has shown the experimental results similar to Ebbinghaus'
original data.
Hermann's experiment contributed a lot to experimental
psychology. He was the first to carry out a series of well-designed
experiments on the subject of forgetting, and he was one of the first to
choose artificial stimuli in the research of experimental psychology.
Since his introduction of nonsense syllables, a large number of
experiments in experimental psychology has been based on highly
controlled artificial stimuli.
Increasing rate of learning
Hermann
Ebbinghaus hypothesized that the speed of forgetting depends on a
number of factors such as the difficulty of the learned material (e.g.
how meaningful it is), its representation and other physiological
factors such as stress and sleep.
He further hypothesized that the basal forgetting rate differs little
between individuals. He concluded that the difference in performance
can be explained by mnemonic representation skills.
He went on to hypothesize that basic training in mnemonic
techniques can help overcome those differences in part. He asserted that
the best methods for increasing the strength of memory are:
- better memory representation (e.g. with mnemonic techniques)
- repetition based on active recall (especially spaced repetition).
His premise was that each repetition in learning increases the
optimum interval before the next repetition is needed (for near-perfect
retention, initial repetitions may need to be made within days, but
later they can be made after years). He discovered that information is
easier to recall when it’s built upon things you already know, and the
forgetting curve was flattened by every repetition. It appeared that by
applying frequent training in learning, the information was solidified
by repeated recalling.
Later research also suggested that, other than the two factors
Ebbinghaus proposed, higher original learning would also produce slower
forgetting. The more information was originally learned, the slower the
forgetting rate would be.
Spending time each day to remember information will greatly
decrease the effects of the forgetting curve. Some learning consultants
claim reviewing material in the first 24 hours after learning
information is the optimum time to re-read notes and reduce the amount
of knowledge forgotten.
Evidence suggests waiting 10–20% of the time towards when the
information will be needed is the optimum time for a single review.
However, some memories remain free from the detrimental effects
of interference and do not necessarily follow the typical forgetting
curve as various noise and outside factors influence what information
would be remembered.
There is debate among supporters of the hypothesis about the shape of
the curve for events and facts that are more significant to the subject. Some supporters, for example, suggest that memories of shocking events such as the Kennedy Assassination or 9/11 are vividly imprinted in memory (flashbulb memory).
Others have compared contemporaneous written recollections with
recollections recorded years later, and found considerable variations as
the subject's memory incorporates after-acquired information. There is considerable research in this area as it relates to eyewitness identification testimony, and eyewitness accounts are found demonstrably unreliable.
Equations
Many
equations have since been proposed to approximate forgetting, perhaps
the simplest being an exponential curve described by the equation:
where is retrievability (a measure of how easy it is to retrieve a piece of information from memory), is stability of memory (determines how fast falls over time in the absence of training, testing or other recall), and is time.
Simple equations such as this one were found by Rubin, Hinton, and Wenzel (1999) to provide a good fit to the available data.