Overview
Group
polarization is an important phenomenon in social psychology and is
observable in many social contexts. For example, a group of women who
hold moderately feminist views tend to demonstrate heightened
pro-feminist beliefs following group discussion.
Similarly, have shown that after deliberating together, mock jury
members often decided on punitive damage awards that were either larger
or smaller than the amount any individual juror had favored prior to
deliberation.
The studies indicated that when the jurors favored a relatively low
award, discussion would lead to an even more lenient result, while if
the jury was inclined to impose a stiff penalty, discussion would make
it even harsher. Moreover, in recent years, the Internet and online social media
have also presented opportunities to observe group polarization and
compile new research. Psychologists have found that social media outlets
such as Facebook and Twitter
demonstrate that group polarization can occur even when a group is not
physically together. As long as the group of individuals begins with the
same fundamental opinion on the topic and a consistent dialogue is kept
going, group polarization can occur.
Research has suggested that well-established groups suffer less
from polarization, as do groups discussing problems that are well known
to them. However, in situations where groups are somewhat newly formed
and tasks are new, group polarization can demonstrate a more profound
influence on the decision-making.
Attitude polarization
Attitude polarization, also known as belief polarization and polarization effect,
is a phenomenon in which a disagreement becomes more extreme as the
different parties consider evidence on the issue. It is one of the
effects of confirmation bias: the tendency of people to search for and interpret evidence selectively, to reinforce their current beliefs or attitudes.
When people encounter ambiguous evidence, this bias can potentially
result in each of them interpreting it as in support of their existing
attitudes, widening rather than narrowing the disagreement between them.
The effect is observed with issues that activate emotions, such as political "hot button" issues. For most issues, new evidence does not produce a polarization effect.
For those issues where polarization is found, mere thinking about the
issue, without contemplating new evidence, produces the effect.
Social comparison processes have also been invoked as an explanation
for the effect, which is increased by settings in which people repeat
and validate each other's statements. This apparent tendency is of interest not only to psychologists, but also to sociologists and philosophers.
Empirical findings
Since the late 1960s, psychologists have carried out a number of studies on various aspects of attitude polarization.
In 1979, Charles Lord, Lee Ross and Mark Lepper performed a study in which they selected two groups of people, one group strongly in favor of capital punishment,
the other strongly opposed. The researchers initially measured the
strength with which people held their position. Later, both the pro- and
anti-capital punishment people were put into small groups and shown one
of two cards, each containing a statement about the results of a
research project written on it. For example:
Kroner and Phillips (1977) compared murder rates for the year before and the year after adoption of capital punishment in 14 states. In 11 of the 14 states, murder rates were lower after adoption of the death penalty. This research supports the deterrent effect of the death penalty.
or:
Palmer and Crandall (1977) compared murder rates in 10 pairs of neighboring states with different capital punishment laws. In 8 of the 10 pairs, murder rates were higher in the state with capital punishment. This research opposes the deterrent effect of the death penalty.
The
researchers again asked people about the strength of their beliefs
about the deterrence effect of the death penalty, and, this time, also
asked them about the effect that the research had had on their
attitudes.
In the next stage of the research, the participants were given
more information about the study described on the card they received,
including details of the research, critiques of the research, and the
researchers' responses to those critiques. The participants' degree of
commitment to their original positions were re-measured, and the
participants were asked about the quality of the research and the effect
the research had on their beliefs. Finally, the trial was rerun on all
participants using a card that supported the opposite position to that
they had initially seen.
The researchers found that people tended to believe that research
that supported their original views had been better conducted and was
more convincing than research that didn't.
Whichever position they held initially, people tended to hold that
position more strongly after reading research that supported it. Lord et al.
point out that it is reasonable for people to be less critical of
research that supports their current position, but it seems less
rational for people to significantly increase the strength of their
attitudes when they read supporting evidence.
When people had read both the research that supported their views and
the research that did not, they tended to hold their original attitudes more strongly than before they received that information.
These results should be understood in the context of several problems
in the implementation of the study, including the fact the researchers
changed the scaling of the outcome of the variable, so measuring
attitude change was impossible, and measured polarization using a
subjective assessment of attitude change not a direct measure of how
much change had occurred.
Choice shifts
Group polarization and choice shifts
are similar in many ways; however, they differ in one distinct way.
Group polarization refers to attitude change on the individual level due
to the influence of the group, and choice shift refers to the outcome
of that attitude change; namely, the difference between the average
group members' pre-group discussion attitudes and the outcome of the
group decision.
Risky and cautious shifts are both a part of a more generalized
idea known as group-induced attitude polarization. Though group
polarization deals mainly with risk-involving decisions and/or opinions,
discussion-induced shifts have been shown to occur on several
non-risk-involving levels. This suggests that a general phenomenon of
choice-shifts exists apart from only risk-related decisions. Stoner
found that a decision is impacted by the values behind that
circumstances of the decision. The study found that situations that
normally favor the more risky alternative increased risky shifts. More
so, situations that normally favor the cautious alternative increased
cautious shifts. These findings also show the importance of previous
group shifts. Choice shifts are mainly explained by largely differing
human values and how highly these values are held by an individual.
According to Moscovici et al.
interaction within a group and differences of opinion are necessary
for group polarization to take place. While an extremist in the group
may sway opinion, the shift can only occur with sufficient and proper
interaction within the group. In other words, the extremist will have
no impact without interaction. Also, Moscovici et al.
found individual preferences to be irrelevant; it is differences of
opinion which will cause the shift. This finding demonstrates how one
opinion in the group will not sway the group; it is the combination of
all the individual opinions that will make an impact.
History and origins
The
study of group polarization can be traced back to an unpublished 1961
Master's thesis by MIT student James Stoner, who observed the so-called
"risky shift".
The concept of risky shift maintains that a group's decisions are
riskier than the average of the individual decisions of members before
the group met.
In early studies, the risky-shift phenomenon was measured using a
scale known as the Choice-Dilemmas Questionnaire. This measure required
participants to consider a hypothetical scenario in which an individual
is faced with a dilemma and must make a choice to resolve the issue at
hand. Participants were then asked to estimate the probability that a
certain choice would be of benefit or risk to the individual being
discussed. Consider the following example:
"Mr. A, an electrical engineer, who is married and has one child,
has been working for a large electronics corporation since graduating
from college five years ago. He is assured of a lifetime job with a
modest, though adequate, salary and liberal pension benefits upon
retirement. On the other hand, it is very unlikely that his salary will
increase much before he retires. While attending a convention, Mr. A is
offered a job with a small, newly founded company which has a highly
uncertain future. The new job would pay more to start and would offer
the possibility of a share in the owner- ship if the company survived
the competition of the larger firms."
Participants were then asked to imagine that they were advising Mr. A.
They would then be provided with a series of probabilities that indicate
whether the new company that offered him a position is financially
stable.
It would read as following: "Please check the lowest probability that you would consider acceptable to make it worthwhile for Mr. A to take the new job."
____The chances are 1 in 10 that the company will prove financially sound.
____The chances are 3 in 10 that the company will prove financially sound.
____The chances are 5 in 10 that the company will prove financially sound.
____The chances are 7 in 10 that the company will prove financially sound.
____The chances are 9 in 10 that the company will prove financially sound.
____Place a check here if you think Mr. A should not take the new job no matter what the probabilities.
Individuals completed the questionnaire and made their decisions
independently of others. Later, they would be asked to join a group to
reassess their choices. Indicated by shifts in the mean value, initial
studies using this method revealed that group decisions tended to be
relatively riskier than those that were made by individuals. This
tendency also occurred when individual judgments were collected after
the group discussion and even when the individual post-discussion
measures were delayed two to six weeks.
The discovery of the risky shift was considered surprising and
counter-intuitive, especially since earlier work in the 1920s and 1930s
by Allport and other researchers suggested that individuals made more
extreme decisions than did groups, leading to the expectation that
groups would make decisions that would conform to the average risk level
of its members.
The seemingly counter-intuitive findings of Stoner led to a spurt of
research around the risky shift, which was originally thought to be a
special case exception to the standard decision-making practice. Many
people had concluded that people in a group setting would make decisions
based on what they assumed to be the overall risk level of a group;
because Stoner's work did not necessarily address this specific theme,
and because it does seem to contrast Stoner's initial definition of
risky shift, additional controversy arose leading researchers to further
examine the topic. By the late 1960s, however, it had become clear that
the risky shift was just one type of many attitudes that became more
extreme in groups, leading Moscovici and Zavalloni to term the overall
phenomenon "group polarization".
Subsequently, a decade-long period of examination of the
applicability of group polarization to a number of fields in both lab
and field settings began. There is a substantial amount of empirical
evidence demonstrating the phenomenon of group polarization. Group
polarization has been widely considered as a fundamental group
decision-making process and was well established, but remained
non-obvious and puzzling because its mechanisms were not fully
understood.
Major theoretical approaches
Almost
as soon as the phenomenon of group polarization was discovered, a
number of theories were offered to help explain and account for it.
These explanations were gradually narrowed down and grouped together
until two primary mechanisms remained, social comparison and informational influence.
Social comparison theory
The social comparison theory,
or normative influence theory, has been widely used to explain group
polarization. According to the social comparison interpretation, group
polarization occurs as a result of individuals' desire to gain
acceptance and be perceived in a favorable way by their group. The
theory holds that people first compare their own ideas with those held
by the rest of the group; they observe and evaluate what the group
values and prefers. In order to gain acceptance, people then take a
position that is similar to everyone else's but slightly more extreme.
In doing so, individuals support the group's beliefs while still
presenting themselves as admirable group "leaders". The presence of a
member with an extreme viewpoint or attitude does not further polarize
the group.
Studies regarding the theory have demonstrated that normative influence
is more likely with judgmental issues, a group goal of harmony,
person-oriented group members, and public responses.
Informational influence
Informational
influence, or persuasive arguments theory, has also been used to
explain group polarization, and is most recognized by psychologists
today. The persuasive arguments interpretation holds that individuals
become more convinced of their views when they hear novel arguments in
support of their position. The theory posits that each group member
enters the discussion aware of a set of items of information or
arguments favoring both sides of the issue, but lean toward that side
that boasts the greater amount of information. In other words,
individuals base their individual choices by weighing remembered pro and
con arguments. Some of these items or arguments are shared among the
members while some items are unshared, in which all but one member has
considered these arguments before. Assuming most or all group members
lean in the same direction, during discussion, items of unshared
information supporting that direction are expressed, which provides
members previously unaware of them more reason to lean in that
direction. Group discussion shifts the weight of evidence as each group
member expresses their arguments, shedding light onto a number of
different positions and ideas.
Research has indicated that informational influence is more likely with
intellective issues, a group goal of making correct decision,
task-oriented group members, and private responses.
Furthermore, research suggests that it is not simply the sharing of
information that predicts group polarization. Rather, the amount of
information and persuasiveness of the arguments mediate the level of
polarization experienced.
In the 1970s, significant arguments occurred over whether persuasive argumentation alone accounted for group polarization. Daniel Isenberg's
1986 meta-analysis of the data gathered by both the persuasive argument
and social comparison camps succeeded, in large part, in answering the
questions about predominant mechanisms. Isenberg concluded that there
was substantial evidence that both effects were operating
simultaneously, and that persuasive arguments theory operated when
social comparison did not, and vice versa.
Self-categorization and social identity
While
these two theories are the most widely accepted as explanations for
group polarization, alternative theories have been proposed. The most
popular of these theories is self-categorization theory. Self-categorization theory stems from social identity theory,
which holds that conformity stems from psychological processes; that
is, being a member of a group is defined as the subjective perception of
the self as a member of a specific category.
Accordingly, proponents of the self-categorization model hold that
group polarization occurs because individuals identify with a particular
group and conform to a prototypical group position that is more extreme
than the group mean. In contrast to social comparison theory and
persuasive argumentation theory, the self-categorization model maintains
that inter-group categorization processes are the cause of group
polarization.
Support for the self-categorization theory,
which explains group polarization as conformity to a polarized norm,
was found by Hogg, Turner, and Davidson in 1990. In their experiment,
participants gave pre-test, post-test, and group consensus
recommendations on three choice dilemma item-types (risky, neutral, or
cautious). The researchers hypothesized that an ingroup
confronted by a risky outgroup will polarize toward caution, an ingroup
confronted by a caution outgroup will polarize toward risk, and an
ingroup in the middle of the social frame of reference, confronted by
both risky and cautious outgroups, will not polarize but will converge
on its pre-test mean.
The results of the study supported their hypothesis in that
participants converged on a norm polarized toward risk on risky items
and toward caution on cautious items.
Another similar study found that in-group prototypes become more
polarized as the group becomes more extreme in the social context. This further lends support to the self-categorization explanation of group polarization.
Real-life applications
The Internet
The
rising popularity and increased number of online social media
platforms, such as Facebook and Twitter, has enabled people to seek out
and share ideas with others who have similar interests and values,
making group polarization effects increasingly evident, particularly in
generation Y individuals.
Owing to this technology, it is possible for individuals to curate
their sources of information and the opinions to which they are exposed,
thereby reinforcing and strengthening their own views while effectively
avoiding information and perspectives with which they disagree.
One study analyzed over 30,000 tweets on Twitter regarding the shooting of George Tiller,
a late term abortion doctor, where the tweets analyzed were
conversations among pro-life and pro-choice advocates post shooting. The
study found that like-minded individuals strengthened group identity
whereas replies between different-minded individuals reinforced a split
in affiliation.
In a study conducted by Sia et al. in 2002, group polarization
was found to occur with online (computer-mediated) discussions. In
particular, this study found that group discussions, conducted when
discussants are in a distributed (cannot see one another) or anonymous
(cannot identify one another) environment, can lead to even higher
levels of group polarization compared to traditional meetings. This is
attributed to the greater numbers of novel arguments generated (due to
persuasive arguments theory) and higher incidence of one-upmanship
behaviors (due to social comparison).
However, some research suggests that important differences arise
in measuring group polarization in laboratory versus field experiments. A
study conducted by Taylor & MacDonald in 2002
featured a realistic setting of a computer-mediated discussion, but
group polarization did not occur at the level expected. The study's
results also showed that groupthink occurs less in computer-mediated
discussions than when people are face to face. Moreover,
computer-mediated discussions often fail to result in a group consensus,
or lead to less satisfaction with the consensus that was reached,
compared to groups operating in a natural environment. Furthermore, the
experiment took place over a two-week period, leading the researchers to
suggest that group polarization may occur only in the short-term.
Overall, the results suggest that not only may group polarization not be
as prevalent as previous studies suggest, but group theories, in
general, may not be predictable when seen in a computer-related
discussion.
Politics and law
Group polarization has been widely discussed in terms of political behavior.
Researchers have identified an increase in affective polarization among
the United States electorate, and report that hostility and
discrimination towards the opposing political party has increased
dramatically over time.
Group polarization is similarly influential in legal contexts. A
study that assessed whether Federal district court judges behaved
differently when they sat alone, or in small groups, demonstrated that
those judges who sat alone took extreme action 35% of the time, whereas
judges who sat in a group of three took extreme action 65% of the time.
These results are noteworthy because they indicate that even trained,
professional decision-makers are subject to the influences of group
polarization.
War and violent behavior
Group
polarization has been reported to occur during wartime and other times
of conflict and helps to account partially for violent behavior and
conflict.
Researchers have suggested, for instance, that ethnic conflict
exacerbates group polarization by enhancing identification with the
ingroup and hostility towards the outgroup.
While polarization can occur in any type of conflict, it has its most
damaging effects in large-scale inter-group, public policy, and
international conflicts.
College life
On a smaller scale, group polarization can also be seen in the everyday lives of students in higher education.
A study by Myers in 2005 reported that initial differences among
American college students become more accentuated over time. For
example, students who do not belong to fraternities and sororities tend
to be more liberal politically, and this difference increases over the
course of their college careers. Researchers theorize that this is at
least partially explained by group polarization, as group members tend
to reinforce one another's proclivities and opinions.