Hypnosis | |
---|---|
Charcot demonstrating hypnosis on a "hysterical" Salpêtrière patient, "Blanche" (Marie Wittmann), who is supported by Joseph Babiński.
| |
MeSH | D006990 |
Hypnosis is a human condition involving focused attention, reduced peripheral awareness, and an enhanced capacity to respond to suggestion.
There are competing theories explaining hypnosis and related phenomena. Altered state theories see hypnosis as an altered state of mind or trance, marked by a level of awareness different from the ordinary state of consciousness. In contrast, nonstate theories see hypnosis as, variously, a type of placebo effect, a redefinition of an interaction with a therapist or form of imaginative role enactment.
During hypnosis, a person is said to have heightened focus and concentration. Hypnotized subjects are said to show an increased response to suggestions.
Hypnosis usually begins with a hypnotic induction
involving a series of preliminary instructions and suggestion. The use
of hypnotism for therapeutic purposes is referred to as "hypnotherapy", while its use as a form of entertainment for an audience is known as "stage hypnosis". Stage hypnosis is often performed by mentalists practicing the art form of mentalism.
Hypnosis for pain management "is likely to decrease acute and chronic pain in most individuals."
The use of hypnosis in other contexts, such as a form of therapy to
retrieve and integrate early trauma, is controversial within the medical
or psychological mainstream. Research indicates that hypnotizing an
individual may aid the formation of false memories, and that hypnosis "does not help people recall events more accurately."
Etymology
The term "hypnosis" comes from the ancient Greek word ὑπνος hypnos, "sleep", and the suffix -ωσις -osis, or from ὑπνόω hypnoō, "put to sleep" (stem of aorist hypnōs-) and the suffix -is. The words "hypnosis" and "hypnotism" both derive from the term "neuro-hypnotism" (nervous sleep), all of which were coined by Étienne Félix d'Henin de Cuvillers in 1820. These words were popularized in English by the Scottish surgeon James Braid (to whom they are sometimes wrongly attributed) around 1841. Braid based his practice on that developed by Franz Mesmer and his followers (which was called "Mesmerism" or "animal magnetism"), but differed in his theory as to how the procedure worked.
Characteristics
A person in a state of hypnosis has focused attention, and has increased suggestibility.
The hypnotized individual appears to heed only the communications of the hypnotist and typically responds in an uncritical, automatic fashion while ignoring all aspects of the environment other than those pointed out by the hypnotist. In a hypnotic state an individual tends to see, feel, smell, and otherwise perceive in accordance with the hypnotist's suggestions, even though these suggestions may be in apparent contradiction to the actual stimuli present in the environment. The effects of hypnosis are not limited to sensory change; even the subject's memory and awareness of self may be altered by suggestion, and the effects of the suggestions may be extended (posthypnotically) into the subject's subsequent waking activity.
It could be said that hypnotic suggestion is explicitly intended to make use of the placebo effect. For example, in 1994, Irving Kirsch
characterised hypnosis as a "nondeceptive placebo", i.e., a method that
openly makes use of suggestion and employs methods to amplify its
effects.
In Trance on Trial, a 1989 text directed at the legal profession, legal scholar Alan W. Scheflin and psychologist Jerrold Lee Shapiro
observed that the "deeper" the hypnotism, the more likely a particular
characteristic is to appear, and the greater extent to which it is
manifested. Scheflin and Shapiro identified 20 separate characteristics
that hypnotized subjects might display: "dissociation"; "detachment"; "suggestibility", "ideosensory activity"; "catalepsy"; "ideomotor responsiveness"; "age regression"; "revivification"; "hypermnesia"; "[automatic or suggested] amnesia"; "posthypnotic responses"; "hypnotic analgesia and anesthesia"; "glove anesthesia"; "somnambulism"; "automatic writing"; "time distortion"; "release of inhibitions"; "change in capacity for volitional activity"; "trance logic"; and "effortless imagination".
Definitions
Historical definitions
The earliest definition of hypnosis was given by Braid, who coined the term "hypnotism" as an abbreviation for "neuro-hypnotism", or nervous sleep, which he contrasted with normal
sleep, and defined as: "a peculiar condition of the nervous system,
induced by a fixed and abstracted attention of the mental and visual
eye, on one object, not of an exciting nature."
Braid elaborated upon this brief definition in a later work, Hypnotic Therapeutics:
The real origin and essence of the hypnotic condition, is the induction of a habit of abstraction or mental concentration, in which, as in reverie or spontaneous abstraction, the powers of the mind are so much engrossed with a single idea or train of thought, as, for the nonce, to render the individual unconscious of, or indifferently conscious to, all other ideas, impressions, or trains of thought. The hypnotic sleep, therefore, is the very antithesis or opposite mental and physical condition to that which precedes and accompanies common sleep
Therefore, Braid defined hypnotism as a state of mental concentration
that often leads to a form of progressive relaxation, termed "nervous
sleep". Later, in his The Physiology of Fascination (1855), Braid
conceded that his original terminology was misleading, and argued that
the term "hypnotism" or "nervous sleep" should be reserved for the
minority (10%) of subjects who exhibit amnesia,
substituting the term "monoideism", meaning concentration upon a single
idea, as a description for the more alert state experienced by the
others.
A new definition of hypnosis, derived from academic psychology, was provided in 2005, when the Society for Psychological Hypnosis, Division 30 of the American Psychological Association (APA), published the following formal definition:
Hypnosis typically involves an introduction to the procedure during which the subject is told that suggestions for imaginative experiences will be presented. The hypnotic induction is an extended initial suggestion for using one's imagination, and may contain further elaborations of the introduction. A hypnotic procedure is used to encourage and evaluate responses to suggestions. When using hypnosis, one person (the subject) is guided by another (the hypnotist) to respond to suggestions for changes in subjective experience, alterations in perception, sensation, emotion, thought or behavior. Persons can also learn self-hypnosis, which is the act of administering hypnotic procedures on one's own. If the subject responds to hypnotic suggestions, it is generally inferred that hypnosis has been induced. Many believe that hypnotic responses and experiences are characteristic of a hypnotic state. While some think that it is not necessary to use the word "hypnosis" as part of the hypnotic induction, others view it as essential.
Michael Nash provides a list of eight definitions of hypnosis by
different authors, in addition to his own view that hypnosis is "a
special case of psychological regression":
- Janet, near the turn of the century, and more recently Ernest Hilgard ..., have defined hypnosis in terms of dissociation.
- Social psychologists Sarbin and Coe ... have described hypnosis in terms of role theory. Hypnosis is a role that people play; they act "as if" they were hypnotised.
- T. X. Barber ... defined hypnosis in terms of nonhypnotic behavioural parameters, such as task motivation and the act of labeling the situation as hypnosis.
- In his early writings, Weitzenhoffer ... conceptualised hypnosis as a state of enhanced suggestibility. Most recently ... he has defined hypnotism as "a form of influence by one person exerted on another through the medium or agency of suggestion."
- Psychoanalysts Gill and Brenman ... described hypnosis by using the psychoanalytic concept of "regression in the service of the ego".
- Edmonston ... has assessed hypnosis as being merely a state of relaxation.
- Spiegel and Spiegel... have implied that hypnosis is a biological capacity.
- Erickson ... is considered the leading exponent of the position that hypnosis is a special, inner-directed, altered state of functioning.
Joe Griffin and Ivan Tyrrell (the originators of the human givens approach)
define hypnosis as "any artificial way of accessing the REM state, the
same brain state in which dreaming occurs" and suggest that this
definition, when properly understood, resolves "many of the mysteries
and controversies surrounding hypnosis".
They see the REM state as being vitally important for life itself, for
programming in our instinctive knowledge initially (after Dement and Jouvet)
and then for adding to this throughout life. They explain this by
pointing out that, in a sense, all learning is post-hypnotic, which
explains why the number of ways people can be put into a hypnotic state
are so varied: anything that focuses a person's attention, inward or
outward, puts them into a trance.
Hypnotic induction
Hypnosis is normally preceded by a "hypnotic induction" technique.
Traditionally, this was interpreted as a method of putting the subject
into a "hypnotic trance"; however, subsequent "nonstate" theorists have
viewed it differently, seeing it as a means of heightening client
expectation, defining their role, focusing attention, etc. There are
several different induction techniques. One of the most influential
methods was Braid's "eye-fixation" technique, also known as "Braidism".
Many variations of the eye-fixation approach exist, including the
induction used in the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS), the most widely used research tool in the field of hypnotism. Braid's original description of his induction is as follows:
Take any bright object (e.g. a lancet case) between the thumb and fore and middle fingers of the left hand; hold it from about eight to fifteen inches from the eyes, at such position above the forehead as may be necessary to produce the greatest possible strain upon the eyes and eyelids, and enable the patient to maintain a steady fixed stare at the object.
The patient must be made to understand that he is to keep the eyes steadily fixed on the object, and the mind riveted on the idea of that one object. It will be observed, that owing to the consensual adjustment of the eyes, the pupils will be at first contracted: They will shortly begin to dilate, and, after they have done so to a considerable extent, and have assumed a wavy motion, if the fore and middle fingers of the right hand, extended and a little separated, are carried from the object toward the eyes, most probably the eyelids will close involuntarily, with a vibratory motion. If this is not the case, or the patient allows the eyeballs to move, desire him to begin anew, giving him to understand that he is to allow the eyelids to close when the fingers are again carried towards the eyes, but that the eyeballs must be kept fixed, in the same position, and the mind riveted to the one idea of the object held above the eyes. In general, it will be found, that the eyelids close with a vibratory motion, or become spasmodically closed.
Braid later acknowledged that the hypnotic induction technique was
not necessary in every case, and subsequent researchers have generally
found that on average it contributes less than previously expected to
the effect of hypnotic suggestions.
Variations and alternatives to the original hypnotic induction
techniques were subsequently developed. However, this method is still
considered authoritative.
In 1941, Robert White wrote: "It can be safely stated that nine out of
ten hypnotic techniques call for reclining posture, muscular relaxation,
and optical fixation followed by eye closure."
Suggestion
When James Braid
first described hypnotism, he did not use the term "suggestion" but
referred instead to the act of focusing the conscious mind of the
subject upon a single dominant idea. Braid's main therapeutic strategy
involved stimulating or reducing physiological functioning in different
regions of the body. In his later works, however, Braid placed
increasing emphasis upon the use of a variety of different verbal and
non-verbal forms of suggestion, including the use of "waking suggestion"
and self-hypnosis. Subsequently, Hippolyte Bernheim shifted the emphasis from the physical state of hypnosis on to the psychological process of verbal suggestion:
I define hypnotism as the induction of a peculiar psychical [i.e., mental] condition which increases the susceptibility to suggestion. Often, it is true, the [hypnotic] sleep that may be induced facilitates suggestion, but it is not the necessary preliminary. It is suggestion that rules hypnotism.
Bernheim's conception of the primacy of verbal suggestion in
hypnotism dominated the subject throughout the 20th century, leading
some authorities to declare him the father of modern hypnotism.
Contemporary hypnotism uses a variety of suggestion forms
including direct verbal suggestions, "indirect" verbal suggestions such
as requests or insinuations, metaphors and other rhetorical figures of
speech, and non-verbal suggestion in the form of mental imagery, voice
tonality, and physical manipulation. A distinction is commonly made
between suggestions delivered "permissively" and those delivered in a
more "authoritarian" manner. Harvard hypnotherapist Deirdre Barrett
writes that most modern research suggestions are designed to bring
about immediate responses, whereas hypnotherapeutic suggestions are
usually post-hypnotic ones that are intended to trigger responses
affecting behaviour for periods ranging from days to a lifetime in
duration. The hypnotherapeutic ones are often repeated in multiple
sessions before they achieve peak effectiveness.
Conscious and unconscious mind
Some hypnotists view suggestion as a form of communication that is directed primarily to the subject's conscious mind, whereas others view it as a means of communicating with the "unconscious" or "subconscious" mind. These concepts were introduced into hypnotism at the end of the 19th century by Sigmund Freud and Pierre Janet.
Sigmund Freud's psychoanalytic theory describes conscious thoughts as
being at the surface of the mind and unconscious processes as being
deeper in the mind.
Braid, Bernheim, and other Victorian pioneers of hypnotism did not
refer to the unconscious mind but saw hypnotic suggestions as being
addressed to the subject's conscious mind. Indeed, Braid actually
defines hypnotism as focused (conscious) attention upon a dominant idea
(or suggestion). Different views regarding the nature of the mind have
led to different conceptions of suggestion. Hypnotists who believe that
responses are mediated primarily by an "unconscious mind", like Milton Erickson,
make use of indirect suggestions such as metaphors or stories whose
intended meaning may be concealed from the subject's conscious mind. The
concept of subliminal suggestion
depends upon this view of the mind. By contrast, hypnotists who believe
that responses to suggestion are primarily mediated by the conscious
mind, such as Theodore Barber and Nicholas Spanos, have tended to make more use of direct verbal suggestions and instructions.
Ideo-dynamic reflex
The first neuropsychological theory of hypnotic suggestion was
introduced early by James Braid who adopted his friend and colleague William Carpenter's theory of the ideo-motor reflex response
to account for the phenomenon of hypnotism. Carpenter had observed from
close examination of everyday experience that, under certain
circumstances, the mere idea of a muscular movement could be sufficient
to produce a reflexive, or automatic, contraction or movement of the
muscles involved, albeit in a very small degree. Braid extended
Carpenter's theory to encompass the observation that a wide variety of
bodily responses besides muscular movement can be thus affected, for
example, the idea of sucking a lemon can automatically stimulate
salivation, a secretory response. Braid, therefore, adopted the term
"ideo-dynamic", meaning "by the power of an idea", to explain a broad
range of "psycho-physiological" (mind–body) phenomena. Braid coined the
term "mono-ideodynamic" to refer to the theory that hypnotism operates
by concentrating attention on a single idea in order to amplify the
ideo-dynamic reflex response. Variations of the basic ideo-motor, or
ideo-dynamic, theory of suggestion have continued to exercise
considerable influence over subsequent theories of hypnosis, including
those of Clark L. Hull, Hans Eysenck, and Ernest Rossi. In Victorian psychology the word "idea" encompasses any mental representation, including mental imagery, memories, etc.
Susceptibility
Braid made a rough distinction between different stages of hypnosis,
which he termed the first and second conscious stage of hypnotism; he later replaced this with a distinction between "sub-hypnotic", "full hypnotic", and "hypnotic coma" stages. Jean-Martin Charcot made a similar distinction between stages which he named somnambulism, lethargy, and catalepsy. However, Ambroise-Auguste Liébeault
and Hippolyte Bernheim introduced more complex hypnotic "depth" scales
based on a combination of behavioural, physiological, and subjective
responses, some of which were due to direct suggestion and some of which
were not. In the first few decades of the 20th century, these early
clinical "depth" scales were superseded by more sophisticated "hypnotic
susceptibility" scales based on experimental research. The most
influential were the Davis–Husband and Friedlander–Sarbin scales
developed in the 1930s. André Weitzenhoffer and Ernest R. Hilgard
developed the Stanford Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility in 1959,
consisting of 12 suggestion test items following a standardised hypnotic
eye-fixation induction script, and this has become one of the most
widely referenced research tools in the field of hypnosis. Soon after,
in 1962, Ronald Shor and Emily Carota Orne developed a similar group
scale called the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (HGSHS).
Whereas the older "depth scales" tried to infer the level of
"hypnotic trance" from supposed observable signs such as spontaneous
amnesia, most subsequent scales have measured the degree of observed or
self-evaluated responsiveness to specific suggestion tests such
as direct suggestions of arm rigidity (catalepsy). The Stanford,
Harvard, HIP, and most other susceptibility scales convert numbers into
an assessment of a person's susceptibility as "high", "medium", or
"low". Approximately 80% of the population are medium, 10% are high, and
10% are low. There is some controversy as to whether this is
distributed on a "normal" bell-shaped curve or whether it is bi-modal
with a small "blip" of people at the high end.
Hypnotizability Scores are highly stable over a person's lifetime.
Research by Deirdre Barrett has found that there are two distinct types
of highly susceptible subjects, which she terms fantasizers and
dissociaters. Fantasizers score high on absorption scales, find it easy
to block out real-world stimuli without hypnosis, spend much time
daydreaming, report imaginary companions as a child, and grew up with
parents who encouraged imaginary play. Dissociaters often have a history
of childhood abuse or other trauma, learned to escape into numbness,
and to forget unpleasant events. Their association to "daydreaming" was
often going blank rather than creating vividly recalled fantasies. Both
score equally high on formal scales of hypnotic susceptibility.
Individuals with dissociative identity disorder have the highest hypnotisability of any clinical group, followed by those with posttraumatic stress disorder.
History
Precursors
People
have been entering into hypnotic-type trances for thousands of years.
In many cultures and religions, it was regarded as a form of meditation.
Modern day hypnosis, however, started in the late 18th century and was
made popular by Franz Mesmer,
a German physician who became known as the father of 'modern
hypnotism'. In fact, hypnosis used to be known as 'Mesmerism' as it was
named after Mesmer.
Mesmer held the opinion that hypnosis was a sort of mystical
force that flows from the hypnotist to the person being hypnotized, but
his theory was dismissed by critics who asserted that there is no
magical element to hypnotism.
Before long, hypnotism started finding its way into the world of
modern medicine. The use of hypnotism in the medical field was made
popular by surgeons and physicians like Elliotson and James Esdaile and researchers like James Braid who helped to reveal the biological and physical benefits of hypnotism. According to his writings, Braid began to hear reports concerning various Oriental meditative practices soon after the release of his first publication on hypnotism, Neurypnology (1843). He first discussed some of these oriental practices in a series of articles entitled Magic, Mesmerism, Hypnotism, etc., Historically & Physiologically Considered.
He drew analogies between his own practice of hypnotism and various
forms of Hindu yoga meditation and other ancient spiritual practices,
especially those involving voluntary burial and apparent human hibernation. Braid's interest in these practices stems from his studies of the Dabistān-i Mazāhib,
the "School of Religions", an ancient Persian text describing a wide
variety of Oriental religious rituals, beliefs, and practices.
Last May [1843], a gentleman residing in Edinburgh, personally unknown to me, who had long resided in India, favored me with a letter expressing his approbation of the views which I had published on the nature and causes of hypnotic and mesmeric phenomena. In corroboration of my views, he referred to what he had previously witnessed in oriental regions, and recommended me to look into the Dabistan, a book lately published, for additional proof to the same effect. On much recommendation I immediately sent for a copy of the Dabistan, in which I found many statements corroborative of the fact, that the eastern saints are all self-hypnotisers, adopting means essentially the same as those which I had recommended for similar purposes.
Although he rejected the transcendental/metaphysical interpretation
given to these phenomena outright, Braid accepted that these accounts of
Oriental practices supported his view that the effects of hypnotism
could be produced in solitude, without the presence of any other person
(as he had already proved to his own satisfaction with the experiments
he had conducted in November 1841); and he saw correlations between many
of the "metaphysical" Oriental practices and his own "rational"
neuro-hypnotism, and totally rejected all of the fluid theories and
magnetic practices of the mesmerists. As he later wrote:
In as much as patients can throw themselves into the nervous sleep, and manifest all the usual phenomena of Mesmerism, through their own unaided efforts, as I have so repeatedly proved by causing them to maintain a steady fixed gaze at any point, concentrating their whole mental energies on the idea of the object looked at; or that the same may arise by the patient looking at the point of his own finger, or as the Magi of Persia and Yogi of India have practised for the last 2,400 years, for religious purposes, throwing themselves into their ecstatic trances by each maintaining a steady fixed gaze at the tip of his own nose; it is obvious that there is no need for an exoteric influence to produce the phenomena of Mesmerism. [...] The great object in all these processes is to induce a habit of abstraction or concentration of attention, in which the subject is entirely absorbed with one idea, or train of ideas, whilst he is unconscious of, or indifferently conscious to, every other object, purpose, or action.
Avicenna
Avicenna
(980–1037), a Persian physician, documented the characteristics of the
"trance" (Hypnotic Trance) state in 1027. At that time, hypnosis as a
medical treatment was seldom used; the German doctor Franz Mesmer
reintroduced it in the 18th century.
Franz Mesmer
Franz Mesmer (1734–1815) believed that there is a magnetic force
or "fluid" called "animal magnetism" within the universe that
influences the health of the human body. He experimented with magnets to
affect this field in order to produce healing. By around 1774, he had
concluded that the same effect could be created by passing the hands in
front of the subject's body, later referred to as making "Mesmeric
passes". The word "mesmerize", formed from the last name of Franz
Mesmer, was intentionally used to separate practitioners of mesmerism
from the various "fluid" and "magnetic" theories included within the
label "magnetism".
In 1784, at the request of King Louis XVI,
a Board of Inquiry started to investigate whether animal magnetism
existed. Among the board members were founding father of modern
chemistry Antoine Lavoisier, Benjamin Franklin, and an expert in pain control, Joseph-Ignace Guillotin.
They investigated the practices of a disaffected student of Mesmer, one
Charles d'Eslon (1750–1786), and though they concluded that Mesmer's
results were valid, their placebo-controlled experiments using d'Eslon's
methods convinced them that mesmerism was most likely due to belief and
imagination rather than to an invisible energy ("animal magnetism")
transmitted from the body of the mesmerist.
In writing the majority opinion, Franklin said: "This fellow
Mesmer is not flowing anything from his hands that I can see. Therefore,
this mesmerism must be a fraud." Mesmer left Paris and went back to
Vienna to practise mesmerism.
James Braid
Following the French committee's findings, Dugald Stewart, an influential academic philosopher of the "Scottish School of Common Sense", encouraged physicians in his Elements of the Philosophy of the Human Mind (1818)
to salvage elements of Mesmerism by replacing the supernatural theory
of "animal magnetism" with a new interpretation based upon "common
sense" laws of physiology and psychology. Braid quotes the following passage from Stewart:
It appears to me, that the general conclusions established by Mesmer's practice, with respect to the physical effects of the principle of imagination (more particularly in cases where they co-operated together), are incomparably more curious than if he had actually demonstrated the existence of his boasted science [of "animal magnetism"]: nor can I see any good reason why a physician, who admits the efficacy of the moral [i.e., psychological] agents employed by Mesmer, should, in the exercise of his profession, scruple to copy whatever processes are necessary for subjecting them to his command, any more than that he should hesitate about employing a new physical agent, such as electricity or galvanism.
In Braid's day, the Scottish School of Common Sense provided the
dominant theories of academic psychology, and Braid refers to other
philosophers within this tradition throughout his writings. Braid
therefore revised the theory and practice of Mesmerism and developed his
own method of hypnotism as a more rational and common sense
alternative.
It may here be requisite for me to explain, that by the term Hypnotism, or Nervous Sleep, which frequently occurs in the following pages, I mean a peculiar condition of the nervous system, into which it may be thrown by artificial contrivance, and which differs, in several respects, from common sleep or the waking condition. I do not allege that this condition is induced through the transmission of a magnetic or occult influence from my body into that of my patients; nor do I profess, by my processes, to produce the higher [i.e., supernatural] phenomena of the Mesmerists. My pretensions are of a much more humble character, and are all consistent with generally admitted principles in physiological and psychological science. Hypnotism might therefore not inaptly be designated, Rational Mesmerism, in contra-distinction to the Transcendental Mesmerism of the Mesmerists.
Despite briefly toying with the name "rational Mesmerism", Braid
ultimately chose to emphasise the unique aspects of his approach,
carrying out informal experiments throughout his career in order to
refute practices that invoked supernatural forces and demonstrating
instead the role of ordinary physiological and psychological processes
such as suggestion and focused attention in producing the observed
effects.
Braid worked very closely with his friend and ally the eminent physiologist Professor William Benjamin Carpenter,
an early neuro-psychologist who introduced the "ideo-motor reflex"
theory of suggestion. Carpenter had observed instances of expectation
and imagination apparently influencing involuntary muscle movement. A
classic example of the ideo-motor principle in action is the so-called
"Chevreul pendulum" (named after Michel Eugène Chevreul).
Chevreul claimed that divinatory pendulae were made to swing by
unconscious muscle movements brought about by focused concentration
alone.
Braid soon assimilated Carpenter's observations into his own
theory, realising that the effect of focusing attention was to enhance
the ideo-motor reflex response. Braid extended Carpenter's theory to
encompass the influence of the mind upon the body more generally, beyond
the muscular system, and therefore referred to the "ideo-dynamic"
response and coined the term "psycho-physiology" to refer to the study
of general mind/body interaction.
In his later works, Braid reserved the term "hypnotism" for cases
in which subjects entered a state of amnesia resembling sleep. For
other cases, he spoke of a "mono-ideodynamic" principle to emphasise
that the eye-fixation induction technique worked by narrowing the
subject's attention to a single idea or train of thought ("monoideism"),
which amplified the effect of the consequent "dominant idea" upon the
subject's body by means of the ideo-dynamic principle.
Hysteria vs. suggestion
For
several decades Braid's work became more influential abroad than in his
own country, except for a handful of followers, most notably Dr. John Milne Bramwell. The eminent neurologist Dr. George Miller Beard took Braid's theories to America. Meanwhile, his works were translated into German by William Thierry Preyer, Professor of Physiology at Jena University. The psychiatrist Albert Moll subsequently continued German research, publishing Hypnotism in 1889. France became the focal point for the study of Braid's ideas after the eminent neurologist Dr. Étienne Eugène Azam translated Braid's last manuscript (On Hypnotism, 1860) into French and presented Braid's research to the French Academy of Sciences. At the request of Azam, Paul Broca, and others, the French Academy of Science, which had investigated Mesmerism in 1784, examined Braid's writings shortly after his death.
Azam's enthusiasm for hypnotism influenced Ambroise-Auguste Liébeault, a country doctor. Hippolyte Bernheim
discovered Liébeault's enormously popular group hypnotherapy clinic and
subsequently became an influential hypnotist. The study of hypnotism
subsequently revolved around the fierce debate between Bernheim and Jean-Martin Charcot, the two most influential figures in late 19th-century hypnotism.
Charcot operated a clinic at the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital (thus, known as the "Paris School" or the "Salpêtrière School"), while Bernheim had a clinic in Nancy (known as the "Nancy School").
Charcot, who was influenced more by the Mesmerists, argued that
hypnotism was an abnormal state of nervous functioning found only in
certain hysterical
women. He claimed that it manifested in a series of physical reactions
that could be divided into distinct stages. Bernheim argued that anyone
could be hypnotised, that it was an extension of normal psychological
functioning, and that its effects were due to suggestion. After decades
of debate, Bernheim's view dominated. Charcot's theory is now just a
historical curiosity.
Pierre Janet
Pierre Janet
(1859–1947) reported studies on a hypnotic subject in 1882. Charcot
subsequently appointed him director of the psychological laboratory at
the Salpêtrière in 1889, after Janet had completed his PhD, which dealt
with psychological automatism. In 1898, Janet was appointed psychology lecturer at the Sorbonne, and in 1902 he became chair of experimental and comparative psychology at the Collège de France. Janet reconciled elements of his views with those of Bernheim and his followers, developing his own sophisticated hypnotic psychotherapy based upon the concept of psychological dissociation, which, at the turn of the century, rivalled Freud's attempt to provide a more comprehensive theory of psychotherapy.
Sigmund Freud
Sigmund Freud (1856–1939), the founder of psychoanalysis, studied hypnotism at the Paris School and briefly visited the Nancy School.
At first, Freud was an enthusiastic proponent of hypnotherapy. He
"initially hypnotised patients and pressed on their foreheads to help
them concentrate while attempting to recover (supposedly) repressed
memories", and he soon began to emphasise hypnotic regression and ab reaction (catharsis)
as therapeutic methods. He wrote a favorable encyclopedia article on
hypnotism, translated one of Bernheim's works into German, and published
an influential series of case studies with his colleague Joseph Breuer
entitled Studies on Hysteria (1895). This became the founding text of the subsequent tradition known as "hypno-analysis" or "regression hypnotherapy".
However, Freud gradually abandoned hypnotism in favour of
psychoanalysis, emphasizing free association and interpretation of the
unconscious. Struggling with the great expense of time that
psychoanalysis required, Freud later suggested that it might be combined
with hypnotic suggestion to hasten the outcome of treatment, but that
this would probably weaken the outcome: "It is very probable, too, that
the application of our therapy to numbers will compel us to alloy the
pure gold of analysis plentifully with the copper of direct [hypnotic]
suggestion."
Only a handful of Freud's followers, however, were sufficiently
qualified in hypnosis to attempt the synthesis. Their work had a limited
influence on the hypno-therapeutic approaches now known variously as
"hypnotic regression", "hypnotic progression", and "hypnoanalysis".
Émile Coué
Émile Coué
(1857–1926) assisted Ambroise-Auguste Liébeault for around two years at
Nancy. After practising for several months employing the "hypnosis" of
Liébeault and Bernheim's Nancy School, he abandoned their approach
altogether. Later, Coué developed a new approach (c.1901) based on
Braid-style "hypnotism", direct hypnotic suggestion, and
ego-strengthening which eventually became known as La méthode Coué. According to Charles Baudouin,
Coué founded what became known as the New Nancy School, a loose
collaboration of practitioners who taught and promoted his views. Coué's method did not emphasise "sleep" or deep relaxation, but instead focused upon autosuggestion involving a specific series of suggestion tests. Although Coué argued that he was no longer using hypnosis, followers such as Charles Baudouin viewed his approach as a form of light self-hypnosis. Coué's method became a renowned self-help and psychotherapy technique, which contrasted with psychoanalysis and prefigured self-hypnosis and cognitive therapy.
Clark L. Hull
The next major development came from behavioural psychology in American university research. Clark L. Hull (1884–1952), an eminent American psychologist, published the first major compilation of laboratory studies on hypnosis, Hypnosis & Suggestibility
(1933), in which he proved that hypnosis and sleep had nothing in
common. Hull published many quantitative findings from hypnosis and
suggestion experiments and encouraged research by mainstream
psychologists. Hull's behavioural psychology interpretation of hypnosis,
emphasising conditioned reflexes, rivalled the Freudian psycho-dynamic
interpretation which emphasised unconscious transference.
Dave Elman
Although Dave Elman
(1900–1967) was a noted radio host, comedian, and songwriter, he also
made a name as a hypnotist. He led many courses for physicians, and in
1964 wrote the book Findings in Hypnosis, later to be retitled Hypnotherapy
(published by Westwood Publishing). Perhaps the most well-known aspect
of Elman's legacy is his method of induction, which was originally
fashioned for speed work and later adapted for the use of medical
professionals.
Milton Erickson
Milton Erickson (1901–1980), the founding president of the American Society for Clinical Hypnosis and a fellow of the American Psychiatric Association, the American Psychological Association, and the American Psychopathological Association,
was one of the most influential post-war hypnotherapists. He wrote
several books and journal articles on the subject. During the 1960s,
Erickson popularized a new branch of hypnotherapy, known as Ericksonian therapy, characterised primarily by indirect suggestion, "metaphor" (actually analogies), confusion techniques, and double binds
in place of formal hypnotic inductions. However, the difference between
Erickson's methods and traditional hypnotism led contemporaries such as
André Weitzenhoffer to question whether he was practising "hypnosis" at all, and his approach remains in question.
Erickson had no hesitation in presenting any suggested effect as being "hypnosis", whether or not the subject was in a hypnotic state. In fact, he was not hesitant in passing off behaviour that was dubiously hypnotic as being hypnotic.
But during numerous witnessed and recorded encounters in clinical,
experimental, and academic settings Erickson was able to evoke examples
of classic hypnotic phenomena such as positive and negative
hallucinations, anesthesia, analgesia (in childbirth and even terminal
cancer patients), catalepsy, regression to provable events in subjects'
early lives and even into infantile reflexology. Erickson stated in his
own writings that there was no correlation between hypnotic depth and
therapeutic success and that the quality of the applied psychotherapy
outweighed the need for deep hypnosis in many cases. Hypnotic depth was
to be pursued for research purposes.
Cognitive-behavioural
In
the latter half of the 20th century, two factors contributed to the
development of the cognitive-behavioural approach to hypnosis:
- Cognitive and behavioural theories of the nature of hypnosis (influenced by the theories of Sarbin and Barber) became increasingly influential.
- The therapeutic practices of hypnotherapy and various forms of cognitive behavioural therapy overlapped and influenced each other.
Although cognitive-behavioural theories of hypnosis must be
distinguished from cognitive-behavioural approaches to hypnotherapy,
they share similar concepts, terminology, and assumptions and have been
integrated by influential researchers and clinicians such as Irving Kirsch, Steven Jay Lynn, and others.
At the outset of cognitive behavioural therapy during the 1950s, hypnosis was used by early behaviour therapists such as Joseph Wolpe and also by early cognitive therapists such as Albert Ellis. Barber, Spanos, and Chaves introduced the term "cognitive-behavioural" to describe their "nonstate" theory of hypnosis in Hypnosis, imagination, and human potentialities. However, Clark L. Hull had introduced a behavioural psychology as far back as 1933, which in turn was preceded by Ivan Pavlov.
Indeed, the earliest theories and practices of hypnotism, even those of
Braid, resemble the cognitive-behavioural orientation in some respects.
Applications
There
are numerous applications for hypnosis across multiple fields of
interest, including medical/psychotherapeutic uses, military uses,
self-improvement, and entertainment. The American Medical Association
currently has no official stance on the medical use of hypnosis.
However, a study published in 1958 by the Council on Mental Health of
the American Medical Association documented the efficacy of hypnosis in
clinical settings.
Hypnosis has been used as a supplemental approach to cognitive behavioral therapy since as early as 1949. Hypnosis was defined in relation to classical conditioning;
where the words of the therapist were the stimuli and the hypnosis
would be the conditioned response. Some traditional cognitive behavioral
therapy methods were based in classical conditioning. It would include
inducing a relaxed state and introducing a feared stimuli. One way of inducing the relaxed state was through hypnosis.
Hypnotism has also been used in forensics, sports, education, physical therapy, and rehabilitation. Hypnotism has also been employed by artists for creative purposes, most notably the surrealist circle of André Breton who employed hypnosis, automatic writing, and sketches for creative purposes. Hypnotic methods have been used to re-experience drug states and mystical experiences. Self-hypnosis is popularly used to quit smoking, alleviate stress and anxiety, promote weight loss, and induce sleep hypnosis. Stage hypnosis can persuade people to perform unusual public feats.
Some people have drawn analogies between certain aspects of
hypnotism and areas such as crowd psychology, religious hysteria, and
ritual trances in preliterate tribal cultures.
Hypnotherapy
Hypnotherapy is a use of hypnosis in psychotherapy.
It is used by licensed physicians, psychologists, and others.
Physicians and psychologists may use hypnosis to treat depression,
anxiety, eating disorders, sleep disorders, compulsive gambling, and posttraumatic stress, while certified hypnotherapists who are not physicians or psychologists often treat smoking and weight management.
Hypnotherapy is viewed as a helpful adjunct by proponents, having
additive effects when treating psychological disorders, such as these,
along with scientifically proven cognitive therapies.
Hypnotherapy should not be used for repairing or refreshing memory
because hypnosis results in memory hardening, which increases the
confidence in false memories. The effectiveness of hypnotherapy has not yet been accurately assessed, and, due to the lack of evidence indicating any level of efficiency, it is regarded as a type of alternative medicine by numerous reputable medical organisations, such as the NHS.
Preliminary research has expressed brief hypnosis interventions
as possibly being a useful tool for managing painful HIV-DSP because of
its history of usefulness in pain management,
its long-term effectiveness of brief interventions, the ability to
teach self-hypnosis to patients, the cost-effectiveness of the
intervention, and the advantage of using such an intervention as opposed
to the use of pharmaceutical drugs.
Modern hypnotherapy has been used, with varying success, in a variety of forms, such as:
- Addictions
- Age regression hypnotherapy (or "hypnoanalysis")
- Cognitive-behavioural hypnotherapy, or clinical hypnosis combined with elements of cognitive behavioural therapy
- Ericksonian hypnotherapy
- Fears and phobias
- Habit control
- Pain management
- Psychotherapy
- Relaxation
- Reduce patient behavior (e.g., scratching) that hinders the treatment of skin disease
- Soothing anxious surgical patients
- Sports performance
- Weight loss
In a January 2001 article in Psychology Today, Harvard psychologist Deirdre Barrett wrote:
A hypnotic trance is not therapeutic in and of itself, but specific suggestions and images fed to clients in a trance can profoundly alter their behavior. As they rehearse the new ways they want to think and feel, they lay the groundwork for changes in their future actions...
Barrett described specific ways this is
operationalized for habit change and amelioration of phobias. In her
1998 book of hypnotherapy case studies,
she reviews the clinical research on hypnosis with dissociative
disorders, smoking cessation, and insomnia, and describes successful
treatments of these complaints.
In a July 2001 article for Scientific American
titled "The Truth and the Hype of Hypnosis", Michael Nash wrote that,
"using hypnosis, scientists have temporarily created hallucinations,
compulsions, certain types of memory loss, false memories, and delusions
in the laboratory so that these phenomena can be studied in a
controlled environment."
Irritable bowel syndrome
Hypnotherapy has been studied for the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome. Hypnosis for IBS has received moderate support in the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence guidance published for UK health services. It has been used as an aid or alternative to chemical anesthesia, and it has been studied as a way to soothe skin ailments.
Pain management
A number of studies show that hypnosis can reduce the pain experienced during burn-wound debridement, bone marrow aspirations, and childbirth. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis found that hypnosis relieved the pain of 75% of 933 subjects participating in 27 different experiments.
Hypnosis is effective in decreasing the fear of cancer treatment reducing pain from and coping with cancer and other chronic conditions. Nausea and other symptoms related to incurable diseases may also be managed with hypnosis. Some practitioners have claimed hypnosis might help boost the immune system of people with cancer. However, according to the American Cancer Society,
"available scientific evidence does not support the idea that hypnosis
can influence the development or progression of cancer."
Hypnosis has been used as a pain relieving technique during dental surgery
and related pain management regimens as well. Researchers like Jerjes
and his team have reported that hypnosis can help even those patients
who have acute to severe orodental pain.
Additionally, Meyerson and Uziel have suggested that hypnotic methods
have been found to be highly fruitful for alleviating anxiety in
patients suffering from severe dental phobia.
For some psychologists who uphold the altered state theory of
hypnosis, pain relief in response to hypnosis is said to be the result
of the brain's dual-processing
functionality. This effect is obtained either through the process of
selective attention or dissociation, in which both theories involve the
presence of activity in pain receptive regions of the brain, and a
difference in the processing of the stimuli by the hypnotised subject.
The American Psychological Association published a study
comparing the effects of hypnosis, ordinary suggestion, and placebo in
reducing pain. The study found that highly suggestible individuals
experienced a greater reduction in pain from hypnosis compared with
placebo, whereas less suggestible subjects experienced no pain reduction
from hypnosis when compared with placebo. Ordinary non-hypnotic
suggestion also caused reduction in pain compared to placebo, but was
able to reduce pain in a wider range of subjects (both high and low
suggestible) than hypnosis. The results showed that it is primarily the
subject's responsiveness to suggestion, whether within the context of
hypnosis or not, that is the main determinant of causing reduction in
pain.
Other medical and psychotherapeutic uses
The
success rate for habit control is varied. A meta-study researching
hypnosis as a quit-smoking tool found it had a 20 to 30 percent success
rate,
while a 2007 study of patients hospitalised for cardiac and pulmonary
ailments found that smokers who used hypnosis to quit smoking doubled
their chances of success.
In 2019, a Cochrane review was unable to find evidence of benefit of
hypnosis in smoking cessation, and suggested if there is, it is small at
best.
Hypnosis may be useful as an adjunct therapy for weight loss. A
1996 meta-analysis studying hypnosis combined with cognitive behavioural
therapy found that people using both treatments lost more weight than
people using cognitive behavioural therapy alone. The virtual gastric band procedure mixes hypnosis with hypnopedia.
The hypnosis instructs the stomach that it is smaller than it really
is, and hypnopedia reinforces alimentary habits. A 2016 pilot study
found that there was no significant difference in effectiveness between
VGB hypnotherapy and relaxation hypnotherapy.
Controversy surrounds the use of hypnotherapy to retrieve
memories, especially those from early childhood or (supposed)
past-lives. The American Medical Association and the American
Psychological Association caution against recovered-memory therapy
in cases of alleged childhood trauma, stating that "it is impossible,
without corroborative evidence, to distinguish a true memory from a
false one." Past life regression, meanwhile, is often viewed with skepticism.
American psychiatric nurses, in most medical facilities, are
allowed to administer hypnosis to patients in order to relieve symptoms
such as anxiety, arousal, negative behaviours, uncontrollable behaviour,
and to improve self-esteem and confidence. This is permitted only when
they have been completely trained about their clinical side effects and
while under supervision when administering it.
Military
A 2006 declassified 1966
document obtained by the US Freedom of Information Act archive shows that hypnosis was investigated for military applications. The full paper explores the potentials of operational uses.
The overall conclusion of the study was that there was no evidence that
hypnosis could be used for military applications, and no clear evidence
whether "hypnosis" is a definable phenomenon outside ordinary
suggestion, motivation, and subject expectancy. According to the
document:
The use of hypnosis in intelligence would present certain technical problems not encountered in the clinic or laboratory. To obtain compliance from a resistant source, for example, it would be necessary to hypnotise the source under essentially hostile circumstances. There is no good evidence, clinical or experimental, that this can be done.
Furthermore, the document states that:
It would be difficult to find an area of scientific interest more beset by divided professional opinion and contradictory experimental evidence... No one can say whether hypnosis is a qualitatively unique state with some physiological and conditioned response components or only a form of suggestion induced by high motivation and a positive relationship between hypnotist and subject... T.X. Barber has produced "hypnotic deafness" and "hypnotic blindness", analgesia and other responses seen in hypnosis—all without hypnotizing anyone... Orne has shown that unhypnotized persons can be motivated to equal and surpass the supposed superhuman physical feats seen in hypnosis.
The study concluded that there are no reliable accounts of its effective use by an intelligence service in history.
Research into hypnosis in military applications is further verified by the Project MKULTRA experiments, also conducted by the CIA. According to Congressional testimony, the CIA experimented with utilizing LSD and hypnosis for mind control.
Many of these programs were done domestically and on participants who
were not informed of the study's purposes or that they would be given
drugs.
Self-hypnosis
Self-hypnosis happens when a person hypnotises oneself, commonly involving the use of autosuggestion. The technique is often used to increase motivation for a diet,
to quit smoking, or to reduce stress. People who practise self-hypnosis
sometimes require assistance; some people use devices known as mind machines to assist in the process, whereas others use hypnotic recordings.
Self-hypnosis is claimed to help with stage fright, relaxation, and physical well-being.
Stage hypnosis
Stage hypnosis is a form of entertainment, traditionally employed in a
club or theatre before an audience. Due to stage hypnotists'
showmanship, many people believe that hypnosis is a form of mind
control. Stage hypnotists typically attempt to hypnotise the entire
audience and then select individuals who are "under" to come up on stage
and perform embarrassing acts, while the audience watches. However, the
effects of stage hypnosis are probably due to a combination of
psychological factors, participant selection, suggestibility, physical
manipulation, stagecraft, and trickery.
The desire to be the centre of attention, having an excuse to violate
their own fear suppressors, and the pressure to please are thought to
convince subjects to "play along". Books by stage hypnotists sometimes explicitly describe the use of deception in their acts; for example, Ormond McGill's New Encyclopedia of Stage Hypnosis describes an entire "fake hypnosis" act that depends upon the use of private whispers throughout.
Music hypnosis
The idea of music as hypnosis developed from the work of Franz Mesmer. Instruments such as pianos, violins, harps and, especially, the glass harmonica often featured in Mesmer's treatments; and were considered to contribute to Mesmer's success.
Hypnotic music became an important part in the development of a
'physiological psychology' that regarded the hypnotic state as an
'automatic' phenomenon that links to physical reflex. In their
experiments with sound hypnosis, Jean-Martin Charcot used gongs and tuning forks, and Ivan Pavlov
used bells. The intention behind their experiments was to prove that
physiological response to sound could be automatic, bypassing the
conscious mind.
Music as Satanic brainwashing
In the 1980s and 1990s, a moral panic took place in the US fearing Satanic ritual abuse. As part of this, certain books such as The Devil's Disciples stated that some bands, particularly in the musical genre of heavy metal, brainwashed
American teenagers with subliminal messages to lure them into the
worship of the devil, sexual immorality, murder, and especially suicide. The use of satanic iconography
and rhetoric in this genre provokes the parents and society, and also
advocate masculine power for an audience, especially on teenagers who
were ambivalent of their identity. The counteraction on heavy metal in
terms of satanic brainwashing is an evidence that linked to the
automatic response theories of musical hypnotism.
Crime
Various people have been suspected of or convicted for hypnosis-related crimes, including robbery and sexual abuse.
In 1951, Palle Hardrup shot and killed two people during a botched robbery in Copenhagen.
Hardrup claimed that his friend and former cellmate Bjørn Schouw
Nielsen had hypnotized him to commit the robbery, inadvertently causing
the deaths. Both were sentenced to jail time.
In 2011, a Russian "evil hypnotist" was suspected of tricking customers in banks around Stavropol
into giving away thousands of pounds worth of money. According to the
local police, he would approach them and make them withdraw all of the
money from their bank accounts, which they would then freely give to the
man.
A similar incident was reported in London in 2014, where a video
seemingly showed a robber hypnotizing a shopkeeper before robbing him.
The victim did nothing to stop the robber from looting his pockets and
taking his cash, only calling out the thief when he was already getting
away.
In 2013, the then-40-year-old amateur hypnotist Timothy Porter
attempted to sexually abuse his female weight-loss client. She reported
awaking from a trance and finding him behind her with his pants down,
telling her to touch herself. He was subsequently called to court and
included on the sex offender list.
In 2015, Gary Naraido, then 52, was sentenced to 10 years in prison for
several hypnosis-related sexual abuse charges. Besides the primary
charge by a 22-year-old woman who he sexually abused in a hotel under
the guise of a free therapy session, he also admitted to having sexually
assaulted a 14-year-old girl.
In December 2018, a Brazilian Medium named João Teixeira de Faria (also
known as "João de Deus"), famous for performing Spiritual Surgeries
through hypnosis techniques, was accused of sexual abuse by 12 women.
State versus nonstate debate
The
central theoretical disagreement regarding hypnosis is known as the
"state versus nonstate" debate. When Braid introduced the concept of
hypnotism, he equivocated over the nature of the "state", sometimes
describing it as a specific sleep-like neurological state comparable to
animal hibernation or yogic meditation, while at other times he
emphasised that hypnotism encompasses a number of different stages or
states that are
an extension of ordinary psychological and physiological processes.
Overall, Braid appears to have moved from a more "special state"
understanding of hypnotism toward a more complex "nonstate" orientation.
State theorists interpret the effects of hypnotism as due
primarily to a specific, abnormal, and uniform psychological or
physiological state of some description, often referred to as "hypnotic
trance" or an "altered state of consciousness". Nonstate theorists
rejected the idea of hypnotic trance and interpret the effects of
hypnotism as due to a combination of multiple task-specific factors
derived from normal cognitive, behavioural, and social psychology, such
as social role-perception and favorable motivation (Sarbin), active imagination and positive cognitive set (Barber), response expectancy (Kirsch), and the active use of task-specific subjective strategies (Spanos).
The personality psychologist Robert White is often cited as providing
one of the first nonstate definitions of hypnosis in a 1941 article:
Hypnotic behaviour is meaningful, goal-directed striving, its most general goal being to behave like a hypnotised person as this is continuously defined by the operator and understood by the client.
Put simply, it is often claimed that, whereas the older "special
state" interpretation emphasises the difference between hypnosis and
ordinary psychological processes, the "nonstate" interpretation
emphasises their similarity.
Comparisons between hypnotised and non-hypnotised subjects
suggest that, if a "hypnotic trance" does exist, it only accounts for a
small proportion of the effects attributed to hypnotic suggestion, most
of which can be replicated without hypnotic induction.
Hyper-suggestibility
Braid
can be taken to imply, in later writings, that hypnosis is largely a
state of heightened suggestibility induced by expectation and focused
attention. In particular, Hippolyte Bernheim
became known as the leading proponent of the "suggestion theory" of
hypnosis, at one point going so far as to declare that there is no
hypnotic state, only heightened suggestibility. There is a general
consensus that heightened suggestibility is an essential characteristic
of hypnosis. In 1933, Clark L. Hull wrote:
If a subject after submitting to the hypnotic procedure shows no genuine increase in susceptibility to any suggestions whatever, there seems no point in calling him hypnotised, regardless of how fully and readily he may respond to suggestions of lid-closure and other superficial sleeping behaviour.
Conditioned inhibition
Ivan Pavlov
stated that hypnotic suggestion provided the best example of a
conditioned reflex response in human beings; i.e., that responses to
suggestions were learned associations triggered by the words used:
Speech, on account of the whole preceding life of the adult, is connected up with all the internal and external stimuli which can reach the cortex, signaling all of them and replacing all of them, and therefore it can call forth all those reactions of the organism which are normally determined by the actual stimuli themselves. We can, therefore, regard "suggestion" as the most simple form of a typical reflex in man.
He also believed that hypnosis was a "partial sleep", meaning that a
generalised inhibition of cortical functioning could be encouraged to
spread throughout regions of the brain. He observed that the various
degrees of hypnosis did not significantly differ physiologically from
the waking state and hypnosis depended on insignificant changes of
environmental stimuli. Pavlov also suggested that lower-brain-stem
mechanisms were involved in hypnotic conditioning.
Pavlov's ideas combined with those of his rival Vladimir Bekhterev
and became the basis of hypnotic psychotherapy in the Soviet Union, as
documented in the writings of his follower K.I. Platonov. Soviet
theories of hypnotism subsequently influenced the writings of Western
behaviourally oriented hypnotherapists such as Andrew Salter.
Neuropsychology
Changes
in brain activity have been found in some studies of highly responsive
hypnotic subjects. These changes vary depending upon the type of
suggestions being given.
The state of light to medium hypnosis, where the body undergoes
physical and mental relaxation, is associated with a pattern mostly of
alpha waves
However, what these results indicate is unclear. They may indicate that
suggestions genuinely produce changes in perception or experience that
are not simply a result of imagination. However, in normal circumstances
without hypnosis, the brain regions associated with motion detection
are activated both when motion is seen and when motion is imagined,
without any changes in the subjects' perception or experience.
This may therefore indicate that highly suggestible hypnotic subjects
are simply activating to a greater extent the areas of the brain used in
imagination, without real perceptual changes. It is, however, premature
to claim that hypnosis and meditation are mediated by similar brain
systems and neural mechanisms.
Another study has demonstrated that a colour hallucination
suggestion given to subjects in hypnosis activated colour-processing
regions of the occipital cortex. A 2004 review of research examining the EEG laboratory work in this area concludes:
Hypnosis is not a unitary state and therefore should show different patterns of EEG activity depending upon the task being experienced. In our evaluation of the literature, enhanced theta is observed during hypnosis when there is task performance or concentrative hypnosis, but not when the highly hypnotizable individuals are passively relaxed, somewhat sleepy and/or more diffuse in their attention.
Studies have shown an association of hypnosis with stronger theta-frequency activity as well as with changes to the gamma-frequency activity. Neuroimaging techniques have been used to investigate neural correlates of hypnosis.
The induction phase of hypnosis may also affect the activity in brain regions that control intention and process conflict. Anna Gosline claims:
Gruzelier and his colleagues studied brain activity using an fMRI while subjects completed a standard cognitive exercise, called the Stroop task. The team screened subjects before the study and chose 12 that were highly susceptible to hypnosis and 12 with low susceptibility. They all completed the task in the fMRI under normal conditions and then again under hypnosis. Throughout the study, both groups were consistent in their task results, achieving similar scores regardless of their mental state. During their first task session, before hypnosis, there were no significant differences in brain activity between the groups. But under hypnosis, Gruzelier found that the highly susceptible subjects showed significantly more brain activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus than the weakly susceptible subjects. This area of the brain has been shown to respond to errors and evaluate emotional outcomes. The highly susceptible group also showed much greater brain activity on the left side of the prefrontal cortex than the weakly susceptible group. This is an area involved with higher level cognitive processing and behaviour.
Dissociation
Pierre Janet originally developed the idea of dissociation of consciousness
from his work with hysterical patients. He believed that hypnosis was
an example of dissociation, whereby areas of an individual's behavioural
control separate from ordinary awareness. Hypnosis would remove some
control from the conscious mind, and the individual would respond with
autonomic, reflexive behaviour. Weitzenhoffer describes hypnosis via
this theory as "dissociation of awareness from the majority of sensory
and even strictly neural events taking place."
Neodissociation
Ernest Hilgard,
who developed the "neodissociation" theory of hypnotism, hypothesized
that hypnosis causes the subjects to divide their consciousness
voluntarily. One part responds to the hypnotist while the other retains
awareness of reality. Hilgard made subjects take an ice water bath. None
mentioned the water being cold or feeling pain. Hilgard then asked the
subjects to lift their index finger if they felt pain and 70% of the
subjects lifted their index finger. This showed that, even though the
subjects were listening to the suggestive hypnotist, they still sensed
the water's temperature.
Social role-taking theory
The main theorist who pioneered the influential role-taking theory of hypnotism was Theodore Sarbin.
Sarbin argued that hypnotic responses were motivated attempts to
fulfill the socially constructed roles of hypnotic subjects. This has
led to the misconception that hypnotic subjects are simply "faking".
However, Sarbin emphasised the difference between faking, in which there
is little subjective identification with the role in question, and
role-taking, in which the subject not only acts externally in accord
with the role but also subjectively identifies with it to some degree,
acting, thinking, and feeling "as if" they are hypnotised. Sarbin drew
analogies between role-taking in hypnosis and role-taking in other areas
such as method acting,
mental illness, and shamanic possession, etc. This interpretation of
hypnosis is particularly relevant to understanding stage hypnosis, in
which there is clearly strong peer pressure to comply with a socially
constructed role by performing accordingly on a theatrical stage.
Hence, the social constructionism and role-taking theory of hypnosis suggests that individuals are enacting (as opposed to merely playing)
a role and that really there is no such thing as a hypnotic trance. A
socially constructed relationship is built depending on how much rapport has been established between the "hypnotist" and the subject.
Psychologists such as Robert Baker
and Graham Wagstaff claim that what we call hypnosis is actually a form
of learned social behaviour, a complex hybrid of social compliance,
relaxation, and suggestibility that can account for many esoteric
behavioural manifestations.
Cognitive-behavioural theory
Barber,
Spanos, and Chaves (1974) proposed a nonstate "cognitive-behavioural"
theory of hypnosis, similar in some respects to Sarbin's social
role-taking theory and building upon the earlier research of Barber. On
this model, hypnosis is explained as an extension of ordinary
psychological processes like imagination, relaxation, expectation,
social compliance, etc. In particular, Barber argued that responses to
hypnotic suggestions were mediated by a "positive cognitive set"
consisting of positive expectations, attitudes, and motivation. Daniel
Araoz subsequently coined the acronym "TEAM" to symbolise the subject's
orientation to hypnosis in terms of "trust", "expectation", "attitude",
and "motivation".
Barber et al. noted that similar factors appeared to mediate the
response both to hypnotism and to cognitive behavioural therapy, in
particular systematic desensitization.
Hence, research and clinical practice inspired by their interpretation
has led to growing interest in the relationship between hypnotherapy and
cognitive behavioural therapy.
Information theory
An approach loosely based on information theory uses a brain-as-computer model. In adaptive systems, feedback increases the signal-to-noise ratio,
which may converge towards a steady state. Increasing the
signal-to-noise ratio enables messages to be more clearly received. The
hypnotist's object is to use techniques to reduce interference and
increase the receptability of specific messages (suggestions).
Systems theory
Systems theory,
in this context, may be regarded as an extension of Braid's original
conceptualization of hypnosis as involving "the brain and nervous system
generally". Systems theory considers the nervous system's
organization into interacting subsystems. Hypnotic phenomena thus
involve not only increased or decreased activity of particular
subsystems, but also their interaction. A central phenomenon in this
regard is that of feedback loops, which suggest a mechanism for creating
hypnotic phenomena.
Societies
There
is a huge range of societies in England who train individuals in
hypnosis; however, one of the longest-standing organisations is the
British Society of Clinical and Academic Hypnosis (BSCAH). It origins
date back to 1952 when a group of dentists set up the 'British Society
of Dental Hypnosis'. Shortly after, a group of sympathetic medical
practitioners merged with this fast-evolving organisation to form 'The
Dental and Medical Society for the Study of Hypnosis'; and, in 1968,
after various statutory amendments had taken place, the 'British Society
of Medical and Dental Hypnosis' (BSMDH) was formed. This society always
had close links with the Royal Society of Medicine
and many of its members were involved in setting up a hypnosis section
at this centre of medical research in London. And, in 1978, under the
presidency of David Waxman, the Section of Medical and Dental Hypnosis
was formed. A second society, the British Society of Experimental and
Clinical Hypnosis (BSECH), was also set up a year before, in 1977, and
this consisted of psychologists, doctors and dentists with an interest
in hypnosis theory and practice. In 2007, the two societies merged to
form the 'British Society of Clinical and Academic Hypnosis' (BSCAH).
This society only trains health professionals and is interested in
furthering research into clinical hypnosis.
The American Society of Clinical Hypnosis
(ASCH) is unique among organizations for professionals using hypnosis
because members must be licensed healthcare workers with graduate
degrees. As an interdisciplinary organization, ASCH not only provides a
classroom to teach professionals how to use hypnosis as a tool in their
practice, it provides professionals with a community of experts from
different disciplines. The ASCH's missions statement is to provide and
encourage education programs to further, in every ethical way, the
knowledge, understanding, and application of hypnosis in health care; to
encourage research and scientific publication in the field of hypnosis;
to promote the further recognition and acceptance of hypnosis as an
important tool in clinical health care and focus for scientific
research; to cooperate with other professional societies that share
mutual goals, ethics and interests; and to provide a professional
community for those clinicians and researchers who use hypnosis in their
work. The ASCH also publishes the American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis