Capital punishment debate in the United States existed as early as the colonial period. As of 2017 it remains a legal penalty in 31 states, the federal government, and military criminal justice systems.
Gallup, Inc.
monitors support for the death penalty in the United States since 1937
by asking "Are you in favor of the death penalty for a person convicted
of murder?" Opposition to the death penalty peaked in 1966, with 47% of
Americans opposing it;
by comparison, 42% supported the death penalty and 11% had "no
opinion." The death penalty increased in popularity throughout the 1970s
and 1980s, when crime went up and politicians campaigned on fighting
crime and drugs; in 1994, the opposition rate was less than 20%, less
than in any other year. Since then, the crime rate has fallen and the
anti-death penalty movement has strengthened again. In the October 2016
poll, 60% of respondents said they were in favor and 37% were opposed.
History
Colonial period
Abolitionists gathered support for their claims from writings by European Enlightenment philosophers such as Montesquieu, Voltaire (who became convinced the death penalty was cruel and unnecessary) and Bentham. In addition to various philosophers, many members of Quakers, Mennonites and other peace churches opposed the death penalty as well. Perhaps the most influential essay for the anti-death penalty movement was Cesare Beccaria's 1767 essay, On Crimes and Punishment.
Beccaria's strongly opposed the state's right to take lives and
criticized the death penalty as having very little deterrent effect.
After the American Revolution, influential and well-known Americans, such as Thomas Jefferson, Benjamin Rush, and Benjamin Franklin made efforts to reform or abolish the death penalty in the United States. All three joined the Philadelphia Society for Alleviating the Miseries of Public Prisons,
which opposed capital punishment. Following colonial times, the
anti-death penalty movement has risen and fallen throughout history. In Against Capital Punishment: Anti-Death Penalty Movement in America, Herbert H. Haines describes the presence of the anti-death penalty movement as existing in four different eras.
First abolitionist era, mid-to-late 19th century
The
anti-death penalty movement began to pick up pace in the 1830s and many
Americans called for abolition of the death penalty. Anti-death penalty
sentiment rose as a result of the Jacksonian era, which condemned
gallows and advocated for better treatment of orphans, criminals, poor
people, and the mentally ill. In addition, this era also produced
various enlightened individuals who were believed to possess the
capacity to reform deviants.
Although some called for complete abolition of the death penalty,
the elimination of public hangings was the main focus. Initially,
abolitionists opposed public hangings because they threatened public
order, caused sympathy for the condemned, and were bad for the community
to watch. However, after multiple states restricted executions to
prisons or prison yards, the anti-death penalty movement could no longer
capitalize on the horrible details of execution.
The anti-death penalty gained some success by the end of the 1850s as Michigan, Rhode Island, and Wisconsin
passed abolition bills. Abolitionists also had some success in
prohibiting laws that placed mandatory death sentences of convicted
murderers. However, some of these restrictions were overturned and the
movement was declining. Conflict between the North and the South in the
run-up to the American Civil War and the Mexican–American War
took attention away from the movement.
In addition, the anti-gallow groups who were responsible for lobbying
for abolition legislation were weak. The groups lacked strong
leadership, because most members were involved in advocating for other
issues as well, such as slavery abolishment and prison reform. Members
of anti-gallow groups did not have enough time, energy, or resources to
make any substantial steps towards abolition. Thus, the movement
declined and remained latent until after the post-Civil War period.
Second abolitionist era, late 19th and early 20th centuries
The anti-death penalty gained momentum again at the end of the 19th century. Populist and progressive reforms contributed to the reawakened anti-capital punishment sentiment. In addition, a "socially conscious" form of Christianity and the growing support of "scientific" corrections contributed to the movement's success.
New York introduced the electric chair in 1890. This method was
supposed to be more humane and appease death penalty opponents. However,
abolitionists condemned this method and claimed it was inhumane and
similar to burning someone on a stake.
In an 1898 op-ed in The New York Times, prominent physician Austin Flint called for the abolition of the death penalty and suggested more criminology-based methods should be used to reduce crime.
Anti-death penalty activism of this period was largely state and
locally based. An organization called the Anti-Death Penalty League was
established Massachusetts in 1897.
However, national leagues, such as the Anti-capital Punishment Society
of America and the Committee on Capital Punishment of the National
Committee on Prisons, developed shortly after.
Many judges, prosecutors, and police opposed the abolition of
capital punishment. They believed capital punishment held a strong
deterrent capacity and that abolishment would result in more violence,
chaos, and lynching. Despite opposition from these authorities, ten
states banned execution through legislation by the beginning of World
War I and numerous others came close. However, many of these victories
were reversed and the movement once again died out due to World War I
and the economic problems which followed.
The American Civil Liberties Union,
however, developed in 1925 and proved influential. The group focused on
educating the public about the moral and pragmatic trouble of the death
penalty. They also organized campaigns for legislative abolition and
developed a research team which looked into empirical evidence
surrounding issues such as death penalty deterrence and racial
discrimination within the capital punishment process. Although the
organization had little success when it came to abolition, they gathered
a multitude of members and financial support for their cause. Many of
their members and presidents were well-known prison wardens, attorneys,
and academic scholars. These influential people wrote articles and
pamphlets that were given out across the nation. They also gave
speeches. Along with other social movements of the time, however, the
group lost momentum and attention due to the Great Depression and World War II.
Third abolitionist era, mid-20th century
The
movement in 1950s and 1960s shifted focus from legislation to the
courts. Although public opinion remained in favor of execution (aside
from during the mid-1960s when pro and anti opinions were roughly
equal), judges and jurors executed fewer people than they did in the
1930s. The decline in executions gave strength to various new
anti-capital punishment organizations. Among these groups were: a California-based Citizens Against Legalized Murder, the Ohio Committee to Abolish Capital Punishment, the New Jersey
Council to Abolish Capital Punishment, California's People Against
Capital Punishment, the New York Committee to Abolish Capital
Punishment, the Oregon
Council to Abolish the Death Penalty, and the national Committee to
Abolish the Federal Death Penalty. In addition to growing organizations,
the movement also profited from growing European abolishment of the
death penalty and from the controversial executions of Barbara Graham and Caryl Chessman. Success mounted in the late 1950s as Alaska, Hawaii, and Delaware abolished capital punishment. Oregon and Iowa
followed their leads in the 1960s. Many other states added laws that
restricted the use of the death penalty except in cases of extreme
serious offenses. Abolitionists began to strongly challenge the
constitutionality of the death penalty in the 1960s. Lawyers from the American Civil Liberties Union and from the NAACP Legal Defense and Educational Fund
launched a major campaign challenging the death penalty's
constitutionality and insisted a moratorium for all executions while it
was in process. The United States executed zero people from 1968 to
1976. The anti-death penalty movement's biggest victory of this time
period was the Supreme Court Case, Furman v. Georgia, of 1972. The Supreme Court found the current state of the death penalty unconstitutional due to its "arbitrary and discriminatory manner" of application.
The court, however, left states with the option to revamp their laws
and make them more constitutional. Twenty eight states did just that and
the court eventually allowed the death penalty again through a series
of cases in 1976, collectively known as Gregg v. Georgia.
Contemporary anti-death penalty movement
The
anti-death penalty movement rose again in response to the reinstatement
of capital punishment in many states. In the courts, the movement's
response has yielded certain limitations on the death penalty's
application. For example, juveniles, the mentally ill, and the
intellectually disabled can no longer be executed. However, the Supreme Court also made it more difficult to allege racial discrimination within the capital punishment process.
During this era, the movement diversified its efforts beyond those of
litigation and lawyers, to include a wide range of organizations that
attacked the death penalty legislatively. Some of the most influential
organizations who continue to work against capital punishment today
include Amnesty International USA, the American Civil Liberties Union, the NAACP Legal Defense and Education Fund, and the National Coalition to Abolish the Death Penalty.
The works of these organizations have brought about various
restrictions on the use of capital punishment at the state level,
including several statewide moratoriums and bans on capital punishment.
As a result, some scholars consider the American death penalty to be
relatively vulnerable in this contemporary period.
Through both litigation and activism, the anti-death penalty movement
has specifically targeted lethal injection as an unacceptable method of
execution. By pressuring pharmaceutical manufacturers and raising
awareness about protracted, painful, or "botched" execution attempts,
activists have achieved some success at limiting the number of
executions carried out. Contemporary activism and advocacy has also
highlighted the possibility of executing innocent people, an issue that
has gained salience as DNA testing has established the innocence of
several death-row convicts. The Innocence Project
has gained widespread recognition for its efforts to clear convictions
using DNA evidence. Finally, many contemporary arguments focus on the
greater cost of the death penalty compared to alternate sentences, which
has attracted strong support in some state legislatures.
Rather than possessing leaders and members who are possible
beneficiaries of the movement's success, the anti-death penalty movement
is composed of "moral entrepreneurs" who speak up for those who are
under threat of being executed.
Membership is not as strong as those of mass movements because it is
often composed of "paper membership," which means members are with a
group that represents other issues as well or members are involved in
multiple other issue-oriented projects.
Public opinion
In a poll completed by Gallup
in October 2009, 65% of Americans supported the death penalty for
persons convicted of murder, while 31% were against and 5% did not have
an opinion.
In the U.S., surveys have long shown a majority in favor of capital punishment. An ABC News survey in July 2006 found 65 percent in favour of capital punishment, consistent with other polling since 2000.
About half the American public says the death penalty is not imposed
frequently enough and 60 percent believe it is applied fairly, according
to a Gallup poll from May 2006. Yet surveys also show the public is more divided when asked to choose between the death penalty and life without parole, or when dealing with juvenile offenders.
Roughly six in 10 tell Gallup they do not believe capital punishment
deters murder and majorities believe at least one innocent person has
been executed in the past five years.
As a comparison, in Canada, Australia, New Zealand, Latin
America, and Western Europe, the death penalty is a controversial issue.
However certain cases of mass murder, terrorism, and child murder
occasionally cause waves of support for restoration, such as the Robert Pickton case, the Greyhound bus beheading, Port Arthur massacre and Bali bombings,
though none of these events or similar events actually caused the death
penalty to be re-instated. Between 2000 and 2010, support for the
return of capital punishment in Canada dropped from 44% to 40%, and
opposition to it returning rose from 43% to 46%. The Canadian government currently "has absolutely no plans to reinstate capital punishment."
Nonetheless, in a 2011 interview given to Canadian media, Canadian
Prime Minister Stephen Harper affirmed his private support for capital
punishment by saying, "I personally think there are times where capital
punishment is appropriate." According to some polls, as of 2012, 63% of
surveyed Canadians believe the death penalty is sometimes appropriate,
while 61% said capital punishment is warranted for murder. In Australia, a 2009 poll found that 23% of the public support the death penalty for murder, while a 2014 poll found that 52.5% support the death penalty for fatal terrorist attacks.
A number of polls and studies have been done in recent years with various results.
In the punishment phase of the federal capital case against Dzhokhar Tsarnaev in 2015 for the 2013 Boston Marathon bombings,
the convict was given the death penalty. Opinion polls in the state of
Massachusetts, where the crime and the trial transpired, "showed that
residents overwhelmingly favored life in prison for Mr. Tsarnaev. Many
respondents said that life in prison for one so young would be a fate
worse than death, and some worried that execution would make him a
martyr. But the jurors in his case had to be 'death qualified' — that
is, they all had to be willing to impose the death penalty to serve on
the jury. So in that sense, the jury was not representative of the
state."
Deterrence
In regard to capital punishment, deterrence
is the notion that the death penalty (for crimes such as murder) may
deter other individuals from engaging in crimes of a similar nature. Up
till 1975, most studies agreed that executing convicted criminals and
publicizing these executions did not significantly deter other
individuals from committing similar crimes.
In 1975, however, Ehrlich famously contradicted existing social
science literature by seemingly proving the validity of the deterrence
argument.
Although Ehrlich's study appeared to show that executing individuals
and publicizing said execution resulted in lower crime rates from the
1930s through the 1960s, his findings drew criticism, due to other researchers' inability to replicate the study and its findings. Since the publication of Ehrlich's controversial findings, studies have been increasingly contradictory.
As studies' findings become increasingly contradictory, the validity of
the deterrence argument has become even more highly contested. In fact,
the most recently published article about the validity of the
deterrence effect problematizes previous studies, arguing that
econometric estimates of execution deterrence are easily manipulated
and, by extension, fallible.
One reason that there is no general consensus on whether or not
the death penalty is a deterrent is that it is used so rarely – only
about one out of every 300 murders actually results in an execution. In
2005 in the Stanford Law Review, John J. Donohue III, a law professor at Yale with a doctorate in economics, and Justin Wolfers,
an economist at the University of Pennsylvania, wrote that the death
penalty "is applied so rarely that the number of homicides it can
plausibly have caused or deterred cannot reliably be disentangled from
the large year-to-year changes in the homicide rate caused by other
factors. ... The existing evidence for deterrence ... is surprisingly
fragile." Wolfers stated, "If I was allowed 1,000 executions and 1,000
exonerations, and I was allowed to do it in a random, focused way, I
could probably give you an answer."
Naci Mocan,
an economist at Louisiana State University, authored a study that
looked at all 3,054 U.S. counties over death penalty on lots of
different grounds. The study found that each execution prevented five
homicides.
Emory University law professor Joanna Shepherd, who has contributed to
multiple studies on capital punishment and deterrence, has said, "I am
definitely against the death penalty on lots of different grounds. But I
do believe that people respond to incentives." Shepherd found that the
death penalty had a deterrent effect only in those states that executed
at least nine people between 1977 and 1996. In the Michigan Law Review in 2005, Shepherd wrote, "Deterrence cannot be achieved with a halfhearted execution program."
The question of whether or not the death penalty deters murder
usually revolves around the statistical analysis. Studies have produced
disputed results with disputed significance. Some studies have shown a positive correlation between the death penalty and murder rates –
in other words, they show that where the death penalty applies, murder
rates are also high. This correlation can be interpreted in either that
the death penalty increases murder rates by brutalizing society, which
is known as the brutalization
hypothesis, or that higher murder rates cause the state to retain or
reintroduce the death penalty. However, supporters and opponents of the
various statistical studies, on both sides of the issue, argue that correlation does not imply causation.
There is evidence that some of the major studies of capital punishment
and deterrence are flawed due to model uncertainty, and that once this
is accounted for, little evidence of deterrence remains.
The case for a large deterrent effect of capital punishment has
been significantly strengthened since the 1990s, as a wave of
sophisticated econometric studies have exploited a newly-available form
of data, so-called panel data. Most of the recent studies demonstrate statistically a deterrent effect of the death penalty.
However, critics claim severe methodological flaws in these studies and
hold that the empirical data offer no basis for sound statistical
conclusions about the deterrent effect. A similar conclusion was reached by the National Research Council
in its 2012 report "Deterrence and the Death Penalty", which stated
that "research to date on the effect of capital punishment on homicide
rates is not useful in determining whether the death penalty increases,
decreases, or has no effect on these rates."
In 2009, a survey of leading criminologists found that 88% of them did
not think capital punishment was an effective deterrent to crime.
Surveys and polls conducted in the last 15 years show that some
police chiefs and others involved in law enforcement may not believe
that the death penalty has any deterrent effect on individuals who
commit violent crimes. In a 1995 poll of randomly selected police chiefs
from across the U.S., the officers rank the death penalty last as a way
of deterring or preventing violent crimes. They ranked it behind many
other forms of crime control including reducing drug abuse and use,
lowering technical barriers when prosecuting, putting more officers on
the streets, and making prison sentences longer. They responded that a
better economy with more jobs would lessen crime rates more than the
death penalty.
In fact, only one percent of the police chiefs surveyed thought that
the death penalty was the primary focus for reducing crime.
However, the police chiefs surveyed were more likely to favor capital punishment than the general population.
In addition to statistical evidence, psychological studies
examine whether murderers think about the consequences of their actions
before they commit a crime. Most homicides are spur-of-the-moment,
spontaneous, emotionally impulsive acts. Murderers do not weigh their
options very carefully in this type of setting (Jackson 27). It is very
doubtful that killers give much thought to punishment before they kill
(Ross 41).
But some say the death penalty must be enforced even if the
deterrent effect is unclear, like John McAdams, who teaches political
science at Marquette University: "If we execute murderers and there is
in fact no deterrent effect, we have killed a bunch of murderers. If we
fail to execute murderers, and doing so would in fact have deterred
other murders, we have allowed the killing of a bunch of innocent
victims. I would much rather risk the former. This, to me, is not a
tough call."
Maimonides argued that executing a defendant on anything less
than absolute certainty would lead to a slippery slope of decreasing
burdens of proof, until we would be convicting merely "according to the
judge's caprice." Caprice of various sorts are more visible now with DNA testing, and digital computer searches and discovery requirements opening DA's
files. Maimonides' concern was maintaining popular respect for law, and
he saw errors of commission as much more threatening than errors of
omission.
Cass R. Sunstein and Adrian Vermeule,
both of Harvard law school, however, have argued that if there is a
deterrent effect it will save innocent lives, which gives a life-life
tradeoff. "The familiar problems with capital punishment—potential
error, irreversibility, arbitrariness, and racial skew—do not argue in
favor of abolition, because the world of homicide suffers from those
same problems in even more acute form." They conclude that "a serious
commitment to the sanctity of human life may well compel, rather than
forbid, that form of punishment." Regarding any attempt to make a utilitarian moral argument for capital punishment, Albert Camus wrote:
Capital punishment is the most premeditated of murders, to which no criminal's deed, however calculated, can be compared. For there to be an equivalency, the death penalty would have to punish a criminal who had warned his victim of the date on which he would inflict a horrible death on him and who, from that moment onward, had confined him at his mercy for months. Such a monster is not to be encountered in private life.
The extent to which the deterrence argument is well-founded, however,
is far from the only interesting and important aspect of this common
justification of capital punishment. In fact, current conceptualizations
of the deterrence argument are also paramount, insofar as they
implicitly operate under the assumption that the media and publicity are
integral to shaping individuals' awareness and understandings of
capital punishment.
In other words, current conceptualizations of the deterrence argument
presuppose that most people are made aware of executions through the
media's coverage of said executions, which means that the media's
selection of executions to cover, as well as the media's coverage of
said executions are necessary for the deterrence effect to transpire. In
this regard, in contemporary society, the deterrence argument relies
upon the implicit understanding that people's understandings and
actions – including actions that may deprive an individual of life – are
influenced by the media. Although it is increasingly unclear as to
whether or not the media's coverage has affected criminal behavior, it
is necessary to examine how the media's coverage of executions and, more
abstractly, its holistic construction of capital punishment has shaped
people's actions and understandings related to this controversial
practice.
Use of the death penalty on plea bargain
Supporters
of the death penalty, especially those who do not believe in the
deterrent effect of the death penalty, say the threat of the death
penalty could be used to urge capital defendants to plead guilty,
testify against accomplices, or disclose the location of the victim's
body. Norman Frink, a senior deputy district attorney in the state of
Oregon, considers capital punishment a valuable tool for prosecutors.
The threat of death leads defendants to enter plea deals for life
without parole or life with a minimum of 30 years – the two other
penalties, besides death, that Oregon allows for aggravated murder. In a plea agreement reached with Washington state prosecutors, Gary Ridgway,
a Seattle-area man who admitted to 48 murders since 1982, accepted a
sentence of life in prison without parole in 2003. Prosecutors spared
Ridgway from execution in exchange for his cooperation in leading police
to the remains of still-missing victims.
The media and the capital punishment debate
The media plays a crucial role in the production and reproduction of various cultural discourses, and is imperative to reflexively shaping and being shaped by pervading cultural beliefs and attitudes.
In this regard, media messages and, by extension, people's beliefs and
attitudes towards practices such as capital punishment may have
considerable ramifications for not only convicted criminals, but also
for jurors, attorneys, politicians, victims' families, and the broader
public debate of capital punishment.
Thus, it is imperative to understand how the media's framing of
executions has massaged people's understandings and their support of
capital punishment, as well as how this framing affects individuals'
engagement in criminal activity.
Media framing of capital punishment
Journalists
and producers play integral roles in shaping the media's framing of the
death penalty. But frames develop through a wide variety of social
actors and stakeholders. In terms of capital punishment, the media's
framing of Timothy McVeigh's execution was interactionally accomplished by a variety of people.
Specifically, the Federal Bureau of Prisons, which historically shied
away from media attention, responded to increased scrutiny through
enlisting a media advisory group to help shape the media's framing of
McVeigh's execution.
Despite the fact that media frames are ubiquitous, the public is
not always cognizant of the particular frames with which they are
bombarded. This is largely because the media frames issues in a way
that, more often than not, keeps people from fully realizing said
frames.
For instance, examining the media's coverage of three Nebraskan
executions reveals that the death penalty was framed in a particularly
positive way, to ensure media coverage would correspond with the
public's growing support for capital punishment at the time.
This meant that journalists did not focus on the problems or tensions
within each case, nor did they ask public officials hard-hitting
questions regarding the cases or the death penalty more broadly.
Media frames can dramatically over-simplify complex social
issues. More specifically, the media simplifies complex cases by
ensuring news stories adhere to generally taken-for-granted, preexisting
cultural understandings of capital crimes.
More specifically, the media frames capital punishment in a
particularly negative and inaccurate way, by almost exclusively covering
cases that involve minority offenders, 'worthy' victims, and especially
heinous crimes; this is especially true for capital crimes that involve the sexual degradation of women. A 209 thematic content analysis of Associated Press
articles finds that the media frames the death penalty in a way that
portrays capital punishment as being overly fair, palatable, and simple.
To accomplish such discursively positive illustrations of the death
penalty and individual executions, journalists frame stories around
inmates' choice. In order of popularity, the other common frames
journalists use to frame execution and the death penalty pertain to
competency, legal procedures, politics, religion, state-assisted
suicide, and inmate suffering.
Although most literature shows that in general, the media frames
executions and capital punishment favorably by minimizing the
complexities of each case, conversely, some studies show that the media
frames executions and capital punishment in an overly negative way. Both
conditions are achieved through reducing and obscuring the complexities
embedded in capital crime cases. Content analyses reveal that The New York Times, Washington Post,
and Associated Press have framed the death penalty negatively by
focusing on exceptions that challenge acceptance: innocence of some
people convicted of capital crimes, the wrongfully accused and
convicted, and convicted individuals' lack of competency.
A formal content analysis of articles in Time, Newsweek, The Progressive, and National Review found that frames used in the left-leaning Progressive and right-wing National Review contributed to each magazine's respective bias. Time and Newsweek, however, were very centrist in their approaches to social issues, including the death penalty. Although these biased frames may seem insignificant, the media's framing of capital punishment has significant implications.
Effect on public opinion
The
media plays a critical role in shaping people's understanding of
capital punishment. This is especially true insofar as the media's
increased focus on the wrongful convictions of innocent people has
resulted in the public becoming less supportive of the death penalty. This finding is supported by more recent studies, including a study involving the analysis of The New York Times articles' contents and the public's opinions on the death penalty.
The media's increased focus on innocent people's wrongful convictions,
referred to as the 'innocence frame,' has highlighted larger
fallibilities within the justice system; it has contributed to a decline
in public support of the death penalty.
Furthermore, examinations of whether individuals' exposure to press
coverage has the ability to alter their understandings of capital
punishment reveal that the way in which the media portrays the public's
support of capital punishment has bearings on the public's support of
capital punishment.
More specifically, if the media suggests there is widespread support of
the death penalty, something of which the media has been guilty,
individuals are more apt to support the death penalty.
It is not only the abstracted 'general public' that is affected
by the media's coverage of the death penalty. The media's framing of
cases involving the sexual degradation of women affects district
attorneys' conceptualizations of said cases, resulting in prosecutors
being more apt to pursue the death penalty in cases that involve the
sexual mistreatment of women.
Cases involving the sexual degradation of women receive much more media
attention than others do. Prosecutors are consequently more likely to
pursue the death penalty for these crimes, despite the fact that they
were, oftentimes, less heinous and gruesome than other capital crimes
that did not involve the sexual degradation of women.
News coverage has been found to shape people's understandings of
the death penalty and specific cases of legally sanctioned execution.
Dramatic television has also been found to have significant bearings on
people's understandings of and actions pertaining to capital punishment.
Viewing police reality shows and television news programs, one's
viewership of crime dramas affects their support of the death penalty.
In fact, people's viewership of crime dramas has been associated with
completely altering people's pre-existing convictions about the death
penalty.
More to the point, crime dramas are able to reframe cases in ways that
correspond with people's broader ideological beliefs, while challenging
and changing their specific beliefs about execution.
For example, people who identify as liberals have historically been
against the death penalty, but crime dramas like Law and Order reframe
criminal cases in a way that associates the death penalty with another
closely held liberal value, such as the safety and protection of women.
In doing so, crime dramas are able to appeal to and sustain people's
ideological beliefs, while simultaneously influencing and altering their
stances on the death penalty.
The media's ability to reframe capital punishment and, by
extension, affect people's support of capital punishment, while still
appealing to their pre-existing ideological beliefs that may
traditionally contradict death penalty support is a testament to the
complexities embedded in the media's shaping of people's beliefs about
capital punishment. How the media shapes people's understandings about
capital punishment can be further complicated by the fact that certain
mediums shape people's beliefs and subjectivities differently.
People exposed to more complex forms of media, such as traditional,
hard-hitting news shows, approach the death penalty in more complex,
sophisticated ways than people who are exposed to less complex forms of
media, including news magazine television shows.
Although the medium is the message to some extent, it is also clear
that every media form has some bearing – large or small – on the
public's support of the death penalty. In this regard, questions must be raised about the ethics of capital punishment in an increasingly media-saturated society.
Furthermore, the public and journalists alike must pay increasing
attention to new investigative techniques that lend themselves to
increased exonerations.
These new techniques are illustrative of the fact that oftentimes, the
media can play a meaningful role in matters of life and death.
Racial and gender factors
People
who oppose capital punishment have argued that the arbitrariness
present in its administration make the practice both immoral and unjust.
In particular, they point to the systemic presence of racial,
socio-economic, geographic, and gender bias in its implementation as
evidence of how the practice is illegitimate and in need of suspension
or abolition.
Anti-death penalty groups specifically argue that the death penalty is unfairly applied to African Americans.
African Americans have constituted 34.5 percent of those persons
executed since the death penalty's reinstatement in 1976 and 41 percent
of death row inmates as of April 2018, despite representing only 13 percent of the general population in 2010.
Furthermore, the race of the victim has also been demonstrated to
affect sentencing in capital cases, with those murders with white
victims more likely to result in a death sentence than those with
non-white victims.
Advocates have been mostly unsuccessful at alleging systemic racial
bias at the Supreme Court, due to the necessity of demonstrating
individualized bias in a defendant's case.
Some attribute the racial disparities in capital punishment to individual factors. According to Craig Rice,
a black member of the Maryland state legislature: "The question is, are
more people of color on death row because the system puts them there or
are they committing more crimes because of unequal access to education
and opportunity? The way I was raised, it was always to be held
accountable for your actions."
Others point to academic studies that suggest African American
defendants are more likely to receive a death sentence than defendants
of other races, even when controlling for the circumstances of the
murder, suggesting that individual factors do not explain the racial
disparities.
As of 2017, women account for 1.88% (53 people) of inmates on
death row, with men accounting for the other 98.12% (2764). Since 1976,
1.1% (16) of those executed were women.
Sexual orientation may also bias sentencing. In 1993, a jury
deliberating over the sentencing of convicted murderer Charles Rhines
submitted a written question to the judge asking if Rhines might enjoy
prison because he was sexually attracted to men. The judge would not
answer that question, and the jury sentenced Rhines to death. In 2018, the Supreme Court said that it would not interfere with the execution of Rhines.
Diminished capacity
In the United States, there has been an evolving debate as to whether
capital punishment should apply to persons with diminished mental
capacity. In Ford v. Wainwright, the Supreme Court held that the Eighth Amendment
prohibits the state from carrying out the death penalty on an
individual who is insane, and that properly raised issues of
execution-time sanity must be determined in a proceeding satisfying the
minimum requirements of due process. In Atkins v. Virginia,
the Supreme Court addressed whether the Eighth Amendment prohibits the
execution of mentally retarded persons. The Court noted that a "national
consensus" had developed against it.
While such executions are still permitted for people with marginal
retardation, evidence of retardation is allowed as a mitigating
circumstance. However, the recent case of Teresa Lewis, the first woman executed in Virginia since 1912, proved to be very controversial because Governor Bob McDonnell refused to commute her sentence to life imprisonment, even though she had an IQ of 70.
Limits to majority
In
theory, opponents of capital punishment might argue that as a matter of
principle, death penalties collide with the substance of Madison's
understanding on democratic rule. According to the Madisonian
principle, the majority's will shall prevail, but at the same time, the
minority shall be respected. Hence, the majority cannot pass legislation
which imposes the death penalty for the simple reason that such
legislation eliminates in total the minority that chooses to disobey the
law. Thus the question pertaining to capital punishment is whether the
majority has the power to enact legislation imposing capital punishment
on the minorities that disobey the laws and exercise the prohibited
conduct. As a result, the punishment for disobeying the law – i.e., the
prohibition to murder, cannot be the death penalty, because it threatens
the existence of the minority.
Cost
Recent
studies show that executing a criminal costs more than life imprisonment
does. Many states have found it cheaper to sentence criminals to life
in prison than to go through the time-consuming and bureaucratic process
of executing a convicted criminal. Donald McCartin, an Orange County, California,
jurist famous for sending nine men to death row during his career, said
that "it's 10 times more expensive to kill [criminals] than to keep
them alive."
McCartin's estimate is actually low, according to a June 2011 study by
former death penalty prosecutor and federal judge Arthur L. Alarcón, and
law professor Paula Mitchell. According to Alarcón and Mitchell,
California has spent $4 billion on the death penalty since 1978, and
death penalty trials are 20 times more expensive than trials seeking a
sentence of life in prison without possibility of parole. Studies in other states show similar patterns.
Wrongful execution
Capital punishment is often opposed on the grounds that innocent people will inevitably be executed. In a study carried out by National Academy of Sciences
in the US it states that 1 in 25 people executed in the US are
innocent. Supporters of capital punishment object that these lives have
to be weighed against the far more numerous innocent people whose lives
can be saved if the murderers are deterred by the prospect of being executed.
Between 1973 and 2005, 123 people in 25 states were released from death row when new evidence of their innocence emerged. Whether all of these exonerations are cases of actual innocence
rather than technical exonerations of the defendants due to legal
issues in their cases that allow their convictions to be legally quashed
is disputed by death penalty supporters.
Statistics likely understate the actual problem of wrongful
convictions because once an execution has occurred there is often
insufficient motivation and finance to keep a case open, and it becomes
unlikely at that point that the miscarriage of justice will ever be
exposed. In the case of Joseph Roger O'Dell III,
executed in Virginia in 1997 for a rape and murder, a prosecuting
attorney bluntly argued in court in 1998 that if posthumous DNA results
exonerated O'Dell, "it would be shouted from the rooftops that ...
Virginia executed an innocent man." The state prevailed, and the
evidence was destroyed.
Despite this, some controversial cases have been re-investigated
following the execution by state authorities, such as post-conviction
DNA testing ordered by Mark Warner of evidence in the Roger Keith Coleman case in Virginia and reviewing the forensic evidence in the Cameron Todd Willingham case in Texas.
Another issue is the quality of the defense in a case where the accused has a public defender.
The competence of the defense attorney "is a better predictor of
whether or not someone will be sentenced to death than the facts of the
crime".
In 2015, the Justice Department and the FBI formally acknowledged
that nearly every examiner in an FBI forensic squad overstated forensic
hair matches for two decades before the year 2000.
26 out of 28 forensic examiners overstated evidence of forensic hair
matches in 268 trials reviewed, and 95% of the overstatements favored
the prosecution. Those cases involve 32 cases in which defendants were
sentenced to death.