From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Genetic drift (allelic drift or the Sewall Wright effect) is the change in the frequency of an existing gene variant (allele) in a population due to random sampling of organisms. The alleles in the offspring are a sample of those in the parents, and chance has a role in determining whether a given individual survives and reproduces. A population's allele frequency is the fraction of the copies of one gene that share a particular form.
Genetic drift may cause gene variants to disappear completely and thereby reduce genetic variation. It can also cause initially rare alleles to become much more frequent and even fixed.
When there are few copies of an allele, the effect of genetic
drift is larger, and when there are many copies the effect is smaller.
In the middle of the 20th century, vigorous debates occurred over the
relative importance of natural selection versus neutral processes, including genetic drift. Ronald Fisher, who explained natural selection using Mendelian genetics, held the view that genetic drift plays at the most a minor role in evolution, and this remained the dominant view for several decades. In 1968, population geneticist Motoo Kimura rekindled the debate with his neutral theory of molecular evolution, which claims that most instances where a genetic change spreads across a population (although not necessarily changes in phenotypes) are caused by genetic drift acting on neutral mutations.
Analogy with marbles in a jar
The process of genetic drift can be illustrated using 20 marbles in a jar to represent 20 organisms in a population.
Consider this jar of marbles as the starting population. Half of the
marbles in the jar are red and half are blue, with each colour
corresponding to a different allele of one gene in the population. In
each new generation the organisms reproduce at random. To represent this
reproduction, randomly select a marble from the original jar and
deposit a new marble with the same colour into a new jar. This is the
"offspring" of the original marble, meaning that the original marble
remains in its jar. Repeat this process until there are 20 new marbles
in the second jar. The second jar will now contain 20 "offspring", or
marbles of various colours. Unless the second jar contains exactly 10
red marbles and 10 blue marbles, a random shift has occurred in the
allele frequencies.
If this process is repeated a number of times, the numbers of red
and blue marbles picked each generation will fluctuate. Sometimes a jar
will have more red marbles than its "parent" jar and sometimes more
blue. This fluctuation is analogous to genetic drift – a change in the
population's allele frequency resulting from a random variation in the
distribution of alleles from one generation to the next.
It is even possible that in any one generation no marbles of a
particular colour are chosen, meaning they have no offspring. In this
example, if no red marbles are selected, the jar representing the new
generation contains only blue offspring. If this happens, the red allele
has been lost permanently in the population, while the remaining blue
allele has become fixed: all future generations are entirely blue. In
small populations, fixation can occur in just a few generations.
In this simulation each black dot on a marble signifies that it has been chosen for copying (reproduction) one time. There is
fixation in the blue "allele" within five generations.
Probability and allele frequency
The mechanisms of genetic drift can be illustrated with a simplified example. Consider a very large colony of bacteria isolated in a drop of solution. The bacteria are genetically identical except for a single gene with two alleles labeled A and B. A and B
are neutral alleles meaning that they do not affect the bacteria's
ability to survive and reproduce; all bacteria in this colony are
equally likely to survive and reproduce. Suppose that half the bacteria
have allele A and the other half have allele B. Thus A and B each have allele frequency 1/2.
The drop of solution then shrinks until it has only enough food
to sustain four bacteria. All other bacteria die without reproducing.
Among the four who survive, there are sixteen possible combinations for the A and B alleles:
(A-A-A-A), (B-A-A-A), (A-B-A-A), (B-B-A-A),
(A-A-B-A), (B-A-B-A), (A-B-B-A), (B-B-B-A),
(A-A-A-B), (B-A-A-B), (A-B-A-B), (B-B-A-B),
(A-A-B-B), (B-A-B-B), (A-B-B-B), (B-B-B-B).
Since all bacteria in the original solution are equally likely to
survive when the solution shrinks, the four survivors are a random
sample from the original colony. The probability
that each of the four survivors has a given allele is 1/2, and so the
probability that any particular allele combination occurs when the
solution shrinks is
(The original population size is so large that the sampling
effectively happens with replacement). In other words, each of the
sixteen possible allele combinations is equally likely to occur, with
probability 1/16.
Counting the combinations with the same number of A and B, we get the following table.
A
|
B
|
Combinations
|
Probability
|
4
|
0
|
1
|
1/16
|
3
|
1
|
4
|
4/16
|
2
|
2
|
6
|
6/16
|
1
|
3
|
4
|
4/16
|
0
|
4
|
1
|
1/16
|
As shown in the table, the total number of combinations that have the same number of A alleles as of B
alleles is six, and the probability of this combination is 6/16. The
total number of other combinations is ten, so the probability of unequal
number of A and B alleles is 10/16. Thus, although the original colony began with an equal number of A and B
alleles, it is very possible that the number of alleles in the
remaining population of four members will not be equal. Equal numbers is
actually less likely than unequal numbers. In the latter case, genetic
drift has occurred because the population's allele frequencies have
changed due to random sampling. In this example the population
contracted to just four random survivors, a phenomenon known as population bottleneck.
The probabilities for the number of copies of allele A (or B) that survive (given in the last column of the above table) can be calculated directly from the binomial distribution where the "success" probability (probability of a given allele being present) is 1/2 (i.e., the probability that there are k copies of A (or B) alleles in the combination) is given by
where n=4 is the number of surviving bacteria.
Mathematical models
Mathematical models of genetic drift can be designed using either branching processes or a diffusion equation describing changes in allele frequency in an idealised population.
Wright–Fisher model
Consider a gene with two alleles, A or B. In diploid populations consisting of N individuals there are 2N
copies of each gene. An individual can have two copies of the same
allele or two different alleles. We can call the frequency of one allele
p and the frequency of the other q. The Wright–Fisher model (named after Sewall Wright and Ronald Fisher) assumes that generations do not overlap (for example, annual plants
have exactly one generation per year) and that each copy of the gene
found in the new generation is drawn independently at random from all
copies of the gene in the old generation. The formula to calculate the
probability of obtaining k copies of an allele that had frequency p in the last generation is then
where the symbol "!" signifies the factorial function. This expression can also be formulated using the binomial coefficient,
Moran model
The Moran model
assumes overlapping generations. At each time step, one individual is
chosen to reproduce and one individual is chosen to die. So in each
timestep, the number of copies of a given allele can go up by one, go
down by one, or can stay the same. This means that the transition matrix is tridiagonal, which means that mathematical solutions are easier for the Moran model than for the Wright–Fisher model. On the other hand, computer simulations
are usually easier to perform using the Wright–Fisher model, because
fewer time steps need to be calculated. In the Moran model, it takes N timesteps to get through one generation, where N is the effective population size. In the Wright–Fisher model, it takes just one.
In practice, the Moran and Wright–Fisher models give
qualitatively similar results, but genetic drift runs twice as fast in
the Moran model.
Other models of drift
If
the variance in the number of offspring is much greater than that given
by the binomial distribution assumed by the Wright–Fisher model, then
given the same overall speed of genetic drift (the variance effective
population size), genetic drift is a less powerful force compared to
selection. Even for the same variance, if higher moments
of the offspring number distribution exceed those of the binomial
distribution then again the force of genetic drift is substantially
weakened.
Random effects other than sampling error
Random changes in allele frequencies can also be caused by effects other than sampling error, for example random changes in selection pressure.
One important alternative source of stochasticity, perhaps more important than genetic drift, is genetic draft. Genetic draft is the effect on a locus by selection on linked loci. The mathematical properties of genetic draft are different from those of genetic drift. The direction of the random change in allele frequency is autocorrelated across generations.
Drift and fixation
The Hardy–Weinberg principle
states that within sufficiently large populations, the allele
frequencies remain constant from one generation to the next unless the
equilibrium is disturbed by migration, genetic mutations, or selection.
However, in finite populations, no new alleles are gained from
the random sampling of alleles passed to the next generation, but the
sampling can cause an existing allele to disappear. Because random sampling
can remove, but not replace, an allele, and because random declines or
increases in allele frequency influence expected allele distributions
for the next generation, genetic drift drives a population towards
genetic uniformity over time. When an allele reaches a frequency of 1
(100%) it is said to be "fixed" in the population and when an allele
reaches a frequency of 0 (0%) it is lost. Smaller populations achieve
fixation faster, whereas in the limit of an infinite population,
fixation is not achieved. Once an allele becomes fixed, genetic drift
comes to a halt, and the allele frequency cannot change unless a new
allele is introduced in the population via mutation or gene flow. Thus even while genetic drift is a random, directionless process, it acts to eliminate genetic variation over time.
Rate of allele frequency change due to drift
Ten
simulations of random genetic drift of a single given allele with an
initial frequency distribution 0.5 measured over the course of 50
generations, repeated in three reproductively synchronous populations of
different sizes. In these simulations, alleles drift to loss or
fixation (frequency of 0.0 or 1.0) only in the smallest population.
Assuming genetic drift is the only evolutionary force acting on an allele, after t generations in many replicated populations, starting with allele frequencies of p and q, the variance in allele frequency across those populations is
Time to fixation or loss
Assuming
genetic drift is the only evolutionary force acting on an allele, at
any given time the probability that an allele will eventually become
fixed in the population is simply its frequency in the population at
that time. For example, if the frequency p for allele A is 75% and the frequency q for allele B is 25%, then given unlimited time the probability A will ultimately become fixed in the population is 75% and the probability that B will become fixed is 25%.
The expected number of generations for fixation to occur is proportional to the population size, such that fixation is predicted to occur much more rapidly in smaller populations.
Normally the effective population size, which is smaller than the total
population, is used to determine these probabilities. The effective
population (Ne) takes into account factors such as the level of inbreeding,
the stage of the lifecycle in which the population is the smallest, and
the fact that some neutral genes are genetically linked to others that
are under selection. The effective population size may not be the same for every gene in the same population.
One forward-looking formula used for approximating the expected
time before a neutral allele becomes fixed through genetic drift,
according to the Wright–Fisher model, is
where T is the number of generations, Ne is the effective population size, and p is the initial frequency for the given allele. The result is the number of generations expected to pass before fixation occurs for a given allele in a population with given size (Ne) and allele frequency (p).
The expected time for the neutral allele to be lost through genetic drift can be calculated as
When a mutation appears only once in a population large enough for
the initial frequency to be negligible, the formulas can be simplified
to
for average number of generations expected before fixation of a neutral mutation, and
for the average number of generations expected before the loss of a neutral mutation.
Time to loss with both drift and mutation
The
formulae above apply to an allele that is already present in a
population, and which is subject to neither mutation nor natural
selection. If an allele is lost by mutation much more often than it is
gained by mutation, then mutation, as well as drift, may influence the
time to loss. If the allele prone to mutational loss begins as fixed in
the population, and is lost by mutation at rate m per replication, then
the expected time in generations until its loss in a haploid population
is given by
where is Euler's constant.
The first approximation represents the waiting time until the first
mutant destined for loss, with loss then occurring relatively rapidly by
genetic drift, taking time Ne ≪ 1/m.
The second approximation represents the time needed for deterministic
loss by mutation accumulation. In both cases, the time to fixation is
dominated by mutation via the term 1/m, and is less affected by the effective population size.
Versus natural selection
In
natural populations, genetic drift and natural selection do not act in
isolation; both phenomena are always at play, together with mutation and
migration. Neutral evolution is the product of both mutation and drift,
not of drift alone. Similarly, even when selection overwhelms genetic
drift, it can only act on variation that mutation provides.
While natural selection has a direction, guiding evolution towards heritable adaptations to the current environment, genetic drift has no direction and is guided only by the mathematics of chance. As a result, drift acts upon the genotypic frequencies
within a population without regard to their phenotypic effects. In
contrast, selection favors the spread of alleles whose phenotypic
effects increase survival and/or reproduction of their carriers, lowers
the frequencies of alleles that cause unfavorable traits, and ignores
those that are neutral.
The law of large numbers predicts that when the absolute number of copies of the allele is small (e.g., in small populations),
the magnitude of drift on allele frequencies per generation is larger.
The magnitude of drift is large enough to overwhelm selection at any
allele frequency when the selection coefficient
is less than 1 divided by the effective population size. Non-adaptive
evolution resulting from the product of mutation and genetic drift is
therefore considered to be a consequential mechanism of evolutionary
change primarily within small, isolated populations.
The mathematics of genetic drift depend on the effective population
size, but it is not clear how this is related to the actual number of
individuals in a population. Genetic linkage
to other genes that are under selection can reduce the effective
population size experienced by a neutral allele. With a higher recombination rate, linkage decreases and with it this local effect on effective population size. This effect is visible in molecular data as a correlation between local recombination rate and genetic diversity, and negative correlation between gene density and diversity at noncoding DNA regions.
Stochasticity associated with linkage to other genes that are under
selection is not the same as sampling error, and is sometimes known as genetic draft in order to distinguish it from genetic drift.
When the allele frequency is very small, drift can also overpower
selection even in large populations. For example, while disadvantageous
mutations are usually eliminated quickly in large populations, new
advantageous mutations are almost as vulnerable to loss through genetic
drift as are neutral mutations. Not until the allele frequency for the
advantageous mutation reaches a certain threshold will genetic drift
have no effect.
Population bottleneck
A population bottleneck is when a population contracts to a
significantly smaller size over a short period of time due to some
random environmental event. In a true population bottleneck, the odds
for survival of any member of the population are purely random, and are
not improved by any particular inherent genetic advantage. The
bottleneck can result in radical changes in allele frequencies,
completely independent of selection.
The impact of a population bottleneck can be sustained, even when
the bottleneck is caused by a one-time event such as a natural
catastrophe.
An interesting example of a bottleneck causing unusual genetic
distribution is the relatively high proportion of individuals with total
rod cell color blindness (achromatopsia) on Pingelap atoll in Micronesia.
After a bottleneck, inbreeding increases. This increases the damage
done by recessive deleterious mutations, in a process known as inbreeding depression. The worst of these mutations are selected against, leading to the loss of other alleles that are genetically linked to them, in a process of background selection. For recessive harmful mutations, this selection can be enhanced as a consequence of the bottleneck, due to genetic purging.
This leads to a further loss of genetic diversity. In addition, a
sustained reduction in population size increases the likelihood of
further allele fluctuations from drift in generations to come.
A population's genetic variation can be greatly reduced by a
bottleneck, and even beneficial adaptations may be permanently
eliminated. The loss of variation leaves the surviving population vulnerable to any new selection pressures such as disease, climatic change
or shift in the available food source, because adapting in response to
environmental changes requires sufficient genetic variation in the
population for natural selection to take place.
There have been many known cases of population bottleneck in the recent past. Prior to the arrival of Europeans, North American prairies were habitat for millions of greater prairie chickens. In Illinois
alone, their numbers plummeted from about 100 million birds in 1900 to
about 50 birds in the 1990s. The declines in population resulted from
hunting and habitat destruction, but a consequence has been a loss of
most of the species' genetic diversity. DNA
analysis comparing birds from the mid century to birds in the 1990s
documents a steep decline in the genetic variation in just the latter
few decades. Currently the greater prairie chicken is experiencing low reproductive success.
However, the genetic loss caused by bottleneck and genetic drift can increase fitness, as in Ehrlichia.
Over-hunting also caused a severe population bottleneck in the northern elephant seal in the 19th century. Their resulting decline in genetic variation can be deduced by comparing it to that of the southern elephant seal, which were not so aggressively hunted.
Founder effect
When
very few members of a population migrate to form a separate new
population, the founder effect occurs. For a period after the
foundation, the small population experiences intensive drift. In the
figure this results in fixation of the red allele.
The founder effect is a special case of a population bottleneck,
occurring when a small group in a population splinters off from the
original population and forms a new one. The random sample of alleles in
the just formed new colony is expected to grossly misrepresent the
original population in at least some respects.
It is even possible that the number of alleles for some genes in the
original population is larger than the number of gene copies in the
founders, making complete representation impossible. When a newly formed
colony is small, its founders can strongly affect the population's
genetic make-up far into the future.
A well-documented example is found in the Amish migration to Pennsylvania in 1744. Two members of the new colony shared the recessive allele for Ellis–Van Creveld syndrome.
Members of the colony and their descendants tend to be religious
isolates and remain relatively insular. As a result of many generations
of inbreeding, Ellis–Van Creveld syndrome is now much more prevalent
among the Amish than in the general population.
The difference in gene frequencies between the original population and colony may also trigger the two groups to diverge significantly over the course of many generations. As the difference, or genetic distance, increases, the two separated populations may become distinct, both genetically and phenetically,
although not only genetic drift but also natural selection, gene flow,
and mutation contribute to this divergence. This potential for
relatively rapid changes in the colony's gene frequency led most
scientists to consider the founder effect (and by extension, genetic
drift) a significant driving force in the evolution of new species. Sewall Wright was the first to attach this significance to random drift and small, newly isolated populations with his shifting balance theory of speciation. Following after Wright, Ernst Mayr
created many persuasive models to show that the decline in genetic
variation and small population size following the founder effect were
critically important for new species to develop.
However, there is much less support for this view today since the
hypothesis has been tested repeatedly through experimental research and
the results have been equivocal at best.
History
The role of random chance in evolution was first outlined by Arend L.
Hagedoorn and A. C. Hagedoorn-Vorstheuvel La Brand in 1921.
They highlighted that random survival plays a key role in the loss of
variation from populations. Fisher (1922) responded to this with the
first, albeit marginally incorrect, mathematical treatment of the
'Hagedoorn effect'.
Notably, he expected that many natural populations were too large (an N
~10,000) for the effects of drift to be substantial and thought drift
would have an insignificant effect on the evolutionary process. The
corrected mathematical treatment and term "genetic drift" was later
coined by a founder of population genetics, Sewall Wright. His first use of the term "drift" was in 1929,
though at the time he was using it in the sense of a directed process
of change, or natural selection. Random drift by means of sampling error
came to be known as the "Sewall–Wright effect," though he was never
entirely comfortable to see his name given to it. Wright referred to all
changes in allele frequency as either "steady drift" (e.g., selection)
or "random drift" (e.g., sampling error). "Drift" came to be adopted as a technical term in the stochastic sense exclusively. Today it is usually defined still more narrowly, in terms of sampling error, although this narrow definition is not universal.
Wright wrote that the "restriction of "random drift" or even "drift" to
only one component, the effects of accidents of sampling, tends to lead
to confusion."
Sewall Wright considered the process of random genetic drift by means
of sampling error equivalent to that by means of inbreeding, but later
work has shown them to be distinct.
In the early days of the modern evolutionary synthesis, scientists were beginning to blend the new science of population genetics with Charles Darwin's
theory of natural selection. Within this framework, Wright focused on
the effects of inbreeding on small relatively isolated populations. He
introduced the concept of an adaptive landscape
in which phenomena such as cross breeding and genetic drift in small
populations could push them away from adaptive peaks, which in turn
allow natural selection to push them towards new adaptive peaks.
Wright thought smaller populations were more suited for natural
selection because "inbreeding was sufficiently intense to create new
interaction systems through random drift but not intense enough to cause
random nonadaptive fixation of genes."
Wright's views on the role of genetic drift in the evolutionary
scheme were controversial almost from the very beginning. One of the
most vociferous and influential critics was colleague Ronald Fisher.
Fisher conceded genetic drift played some role in evolution, but an
insignificant one. Fisher has been accused of misunderstanding Wright's
views because in his criticisms Fisher seemed to argue Wright had
rejected selection almost entirely. To Fisher, viewing the process of
evolution as a long, steady, adaptive progression was the only way to
explain the ever-increasing complexity from simpler forms. But the
debates have continued between the "gradualists" and those who lean more
toward the Wright model of evolution where selection and drift together
play an important role.
In 1968, Motoo Kimura
rekindled the debate with his neutral theory of molecular evolution,
which claims that most of the genetic changes are caused by genetic
drift acting on neutral mutations.
The role of genetic drift by means of sampling error in evolution has been criticized by John H. Gillespie and William B. Provine, who argue that selection on linked sites is a more important stochastic force.