Optimism is a mental attitude
reflecting a belief or hope that the outcome of some specific endeavor,
or outcomes in general, will be positive, favorable, and desirable. A
common idiom used to illustrate optimism versus pessimism is a glass filled with water to the halfway point: an optimist is said to see the glass as half full, while a pessimist sees the glass as half empty.
The term derives from the Latin optimum, meaning "best".
Being optimistic, in the typical sense of the word, is defined as
expecting the best possible outcome from any given situation. This is usually referred to in psychology as dispositional optimism. It thus reflects a belief that future conditions will work out for the best. For this reason, it is seen as a trait that fosters resilience in the face of stress.
Theories of optimism include dispositional models, and models of explanatory style.
Methods to measure optimism have been developed within both theoretical
systems, such as various forms of the Life Orientation Test, for the
original definition of optimism, or the Attributional Style
Questionnaire designed to test optimism in terms of explanatory style.
Variation in optimism and pessimism is somewhat heritable and reflects biological trait systems to some degree. It is also influenced by environmental factors, including family environment, with some suggesting it can be learned. Optimism may also be linked to health.
Psychological optimism
Dispositional optimism
Researchers operationalize
the term differently depending on their research. As with any trait
characteristic, there are several ways to evaluate optimism, such as the
Life Orientation Test (LOT). This 8-item scale was developed in 1985 by
Michael Scheier and Charles Carver.
Dispositional optimism and pessimism
are typically assessed by asking people whether they expect future
outcomes to be beneficial or negative (see below). The LOT returns
separate optimism and pessimism scores for each individual.
Behaviourally, these two scores correlate around r = 0.5. Optimistic
scores on this scale predict better outcomes in relationships, higher social status, and reduced loss of well-being following adversity. Health preserving behaviors are associated with optimism while health-damaging behaviors are associated with pessimism.
Some have argued that optimism is the opposite end of a single dimension with pessimism, with any distinction between them reflecting factors such as social desirability. Confirmatory modelling, however, supports a two-dimensional model and the two dimensions predict different outcomes. Genetic
modelling confirms this independence, showing that pessimism and
optimism are inherited as independent traits, with the typical
correlation between them emerging as a result of a general well-being
factor and family environment influences.
It is suggested that patients with high dispositional optimism appear
to have stronger immune system since it buffers it against psychological
stressors. Optimists appear to live longer.
Explanatory style
Explanatory style
is distinct from dispositional theories of optimism. While related to
life-orientation measures of optimism, attributional style theory
suggests that dispositional optimism and pessimism are reflections of the ways people explain events, i.e., that attributions cause these dispositions. Here, an optimist would view defeat as temporary, does not apply to other cases, and is not considered his fault.
Measures of attributional style distinguish three dimensions among
explanations for events: Whether these explanations draw on internal
versus external causes; whether the causes are viewed as stable versus
unstable; and whether explanations apply globally versus being
situationally specific. In addition, the measures distinguish
attributions for positive and for negative events.
An optimistic person attributes internal, stable, and global
explanations to good things. Pessimistic explanations attribute these
traits of stability, globality, and internality to negative events, such
as difficulty in relationships.
Models of Optimistic and Pessimistic attributions show that
attributions themselves are a cognitive style – individuals who tend to
focus on the global explanations do so for all types of events, and the
styles correlate among each other. In addition to this, individuals vary
in how optimistic their attributions are for good events, and on how
pessimistic their attributions are for bad events, but these two traits
of optimism and pessimism are un-correlated.
There is much debate about the relationship between explanatory
style and optimism. Some researchers argue that optimism is simply the
lay-term for what researchers know as explanatory style. More commonly, it is found that explanatory style is quite distinct from dispositional optimism,
and the two should not be used interchangeably as they are marginally
correlated at best. More research is required to "bridge" or further
differentiate these concepts.
Origins
As with all psychological traits, differences in both dispositional optimism and pessimism and in attributional style are heritable. Both optimism and pessimism are strongly influenced by environmental factors, including family environment. It has been suggested that optimism may be indirectly inherited as a reflection of underlying heritable traits such as intelligence, temperament, and alcoholism.
There is evidence from twin studies that show, for instance, that the
inherited component of the dispositional optimism is about 25 percent,
making this trait a stable personality dimension and a predictor of life outcomes.
Its genetic origin, which interacts with environmental influences and
other risks, also determines the vulnerability to depression across the
lifespan. Many theories assume optimism can be learned, and research supports a modest role of family-environment acting to raise (or lower) optimism and lower (or raise) neuroticism and pessimism.
Work utilising brain imaging and biochemistry suggests that at a biological trait
level, optimism and pessimism reflect brain systems specialised for the
tasks of processing and incorporating beliefs regarding good and bad information respectively.
Assessment
Life Orientation Test
The
Life Orientation Test (LOT) was designed by Scheier and Carver (1985)
to assess dispositional optimism – expecting positive or negative
outcomes,
and is one of the more popular tests of optimism and pessimism. This
was also often used in early studies that examine the effects of these
dispositions in health-related domains.
Scheier and Carver's initial research, which surveyed college students,
found that optimistic participants were less likely to show an increase
in symptoms like dizziness, muscle soreness, fatigue, blurred vision,
and other physical complaints than pessimistic respondents.
There are eight items and four filler items in the test. Four are
positive items (e.g. "In uncertain times, I usually expect the best")
and four are negative items e.g. "If something can go wrong for me, it
will."
The LOT has been revised twice—once by the original creators (LOT-R)
and also by Chang, Maydeu-Olivares, and D'Zurilla as the Extended Life
Orientation Test (ELOT). The Revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R:
Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) consists of six items, each scored
on a 5-point scale from "Strongly disagree" to "Strongly agree" and
four filler items.
Half of the coded items are phrased in an optimistic way while the
other half in a pessimistic way. In comparison with its previous
iteration, LOT-R offers good internal consistency overtime although
there are item overlaps, making the correlation between the LOT and
LOT-R extremely high.
Attributional Style Questionnaire
This Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ: Peterson et al. 1982) is based on the explanatory style model of optimism. Subjects read a list of six positive and negative events (e.g. "you have been looking for a job unsuccessfully for some time"),
and are asked to record a possible cause for the event. They then rate
whether this is internal or external, stable or changeable, and global
or local to the event.
There are several modified versions of the ASQ including the Expanded
Attributional Style Questionnaire (EASQ), the Content Analysis of
Verbatim Explanations (CAVE), and the ASQ designed for testing the
optimism of children.
Associations with health
Optimism and health are correlated moderately. Optimism has been shown to explain between 5–10% of the variation in the likelihood of developing some health conditions (correlation coefficients between .20 and .30), notably including cardiovascular disease, stroke,[40] and depression.
The relationship between optimism and health has also been
studied with regards to physical symptoms, coping strategies and
negative affect for those suffering from rheumatoid arthritis, asthma, and fibromyalgia.
It has been found that among individuals with these diseases,
optimists are not more likely than pessimists to report pain alleviation
due to coping strategies, despite differences in psychological
well-being between the two groups. A meta-analysis
has confirmed the assumption that optimism is related to psychological
well-being: "Put simply, optimists emerge from difficult circumstances
with less distress than do pessimists."
Furthermore, the correlation appears to be attributable to coping
style: "That is, optimists seem intent on facing problems head-on,
taking active and constructive steps to solve their problems; pessimists
are more likely to abandon their effort to attain their goals."
Optimists may respond better to stress: pessimists have shown
higher levels of cortisol (the "stress hormone") and trouble regulating
cortisol in response to stressors. Another study by Scheier examined the recovery process for a number of patients that had undergone surgery.
The study showed that optimism was a strong predictor of the rate of
recovery. Optimists achieved faster results in "behavioral milestones"
such as sitting in bed, walking around, etc. They also were rated by
staff as having a more favorable physical recovery. In a 6-month later
follow-up, it was found that optimists were quicker to resume normal
activities.
Optimism and well-being
A
number of studies have been done on optimism and psychological
well-being. One 30 year study undertaken by Lee et al. (2019) assessed
the overall optimism and longevity of cohorts of men from the Veterans
Affairs Normative Aging Study and women from the Nurses’ Health Study.
The study found a positive correlation between higher levels of
optimism and exceptional longevity, which the study defined as a
lifespan exceeding 85 years.
Another study conducted by Aspinwall and Taylor (1990) assessed
incoming freshmen on a range of personality factors such as optimism,
self-esteem, locus of self-control, etc.
It was found that freshmen who scored high on optimism before entering
college were reported to have lower levels of psychological distress
than their more pessimistic peers, while controlling for the other
personality factors. Over time, the more optimistic students were less
stressed, less lonely, and less depressed than their pessimistic
counterparts. Thus, this study suggests a strong link between optimism
and psychological well-being.
In addition low optimism may help explain the association between caregivers' anger and reduced sense of vitality.
A recent meta-analysis of optimism supported past findings that optimism is positively correlated with life satisfaction, happiness, psychological and physical well-being and negatively correlated with depression and anxiety.
Seeking to explain the correlation, researchers find that
optimists choose healthier lifestyles. For example, optimists smoke
less, are more physically active, consume more fruit, vegetables and
whole-grain bread, and are more moderate in alcohol consumption.
Translating association into modifiability
Research
to date has demonstrated that optimists are less likely to have certain
diseases or develop certain diseases over time. By comparison, research
has not yet been able to demonstrate the ability to change an
individual's level of optimism through psychological interventions, and thereby alter the course of disease or likelihood for development of disease. Though in that same vein, an article by Mayo Clinic argues steps to change self-talk from negative to positive may shift individuals from a negative to a more positive/optimistic outlook.
Strategies claimed to be of value include surrounding oneself with
positive people, identifying areas of change, practicing positive
self-talk, being open to humor, and following a healthy lifestyle. There is also the notion of "learned optimism" in positive psychology,
which holds that joy is a talent that can be cultivated and can be
achieved through specific actions such as the challenging negative self
talk or overcoming "learned helplessness".
There are researchers in a study involving twins who found that optimism is largely inherited at birth.
Along with the recognition that childhood experiences determine an
individual's outlook, such studies demonstrate the genetic basis for
optimism reinforces the recognized difficulty in changing or
manipulating the direction of an adult's disposition from pessimist to
optimist.
Philosophical optimism
One
of the earliest forms of philosophical optimism was Socrates' theory of
moral intellectualism, which formed part of the thinker's enlightenment
model through the process of self-improvement.
According to the philosopher, it is possible to attain virtuous life by
leading and completing moral perfection that is attained through
philosophical self-examination. He maintained that knowledge of moral
truth is necessary and sufficient for leading a good life.
In his philosophical investigations, Socrates followed a model that did
not merely focus on the intellect or reason but a balanced practice
that also consider the emotion as an important contributor to the
richness of human experience.
Distinct from a disposition to believe that things will work out,
there is a philosophical idea that, perhaps in ways that may not be
fully comprehended, the present moment is in an optimum state. This
view that all of nature - past, present, and future - operates by laws
of optimization along the lines of Hamilton's principle in the realm of physics is countered by views such as idealism, realism, and philosophical pessimism. Philosophers often link the concept of optimism with the name of Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz, who held that we live in the best of all possible worlds (le meilleur des mondes possibles), or that God created a physical universe that applies the laws of physics. The concept was also reflected in an aspect of Francois-Marie Arouet de Voltaire's early philosophy, one that was based on Isaac Newton's view that described a divinely ordered human condition. This philosophy would also later emerge in Alexander Pope's Essay on Man.
Leibniz proposed that it was not in God's power to create a perfect world, but among possible worlds, he created the best. In one of his writings, he responded to the Blaise Pascal's
philosophy of awe and desperation in the face of the infinite by
claiming that infinity should be celebrated. While Pascal advocated for
making man's rational aspirations more humble, Leibniz was optimistic
about the capacity of human reason to further extend itself.
Some think incorrectly that this idea was mocked by Voltaire in his satirical novel Candide as baseless optimism of the sort exemplified by the beliefs of one of its characters Dr. Pangloss, which are the opposite of his fellow traveller Martin's pessimism and emphasis on free will. The optimistic position is also called Panglossianism and became an adjective for excessive, even stupendous, optimism. The phrase "panglossian pessimism" has been used
to describe the pessimistic position that, since this is the best of
all possible worlds, it is impossible for anything to get any better.
Conversely, philosophical pessimism might be
associated with an optimistic long-term view because it implies that no
change for the worse is possible. Later, Voltaire found it difficult to
reconcile Leibniz' optimism with human suffering as demonstrated by the
earthquake that devastated Lisbon in 1755 as well as the atrocities committed by the pre-revolutionary France against its people.
Optimalism
Philosophical optimalism, as defined by Nicholas Rescher, holds that this universe exists because it is better than the alternatives. While this philosophy does not exclude the possibility of a deity, it also does not require one, and is compatible with atheism.
Rescher explained that the concept can stand on its own feet, arguing
that there is no necessity to seeing optimalism realization as divinely
instituted because it is a naturalistic theory in principle.
Psychological optimalism, as defined by the positive psychologist Tal Ben-Shahar,
means willingness to accept failure while remaining confident that
success will follow, a positive attitude he contrasts with negative perfectionism.
Perfectionism can be defined as a persistent compulsive drive toward
unattainable goals and valuation based solely in terms of
accomplishment.
Perfectionists reject the realities and constraints of human ability.
They cannot accept failures, delaying any ambitious and productive
behavior in fear of failure again. This neuroticism can even lead to clinical depression and low productivity.
As an alternative to negative perfectionism, Ben-Shahar suggests the
adoption of optimalism. Optimalism allows for failure in pursuit of a
goal, and expects that while the trend of activity is towards the
positive, it is not necessary to always succeed while striving towards
goals. This basis in reality prevents the optimalist from being
overwhelmed in the face of failure.
Optimalists accept failures and also learn from them, which encourages further pursuit of achievement.
Dr. Tal Ben-Shahar believes that Optimalists and Perfectionists show
distinct different motives. Optimalists tend to have more intrinsic,
inward desires, with a motivation to learn, while perfectionists are
highly motivated by a need to consistently prove themselves worthy.
Optimalism has also been classified into two: product optimalism
and process optimalism. The former is described as an outlook that looks
to provide the realization of the best possible result while the latter
looks for a maximization of the chances of achieving the best possible
result.
Others classify it either as full-scale, one that implies determinism,
or weak determinism, which claims that we have the best laws and initial
conditions.
Some sources also distinguish the concept from optimism since it does
not focus on how things are going well but on how things are going as
well as possible.